Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T10:57:45.913Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Neurocognitive predictors of transition to psychosis: medium- to long-term findings from a sample at ultra-high risk for psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 February 2013

A. Lin*
Affiliation:
Orygen Youth Health Research Centre and Centre for Youth Mental Health, University of Melbourne, Australia School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Australia
A. R. Yung
Affiliation:
Orygen Youth Health Research Centre and Centre for Youth Mental Health, University of Melbourne, Australia Institute of Brain, Behaviour and Mental Health, University of Manchester, UK
B. Nelson
Affiliation:
Orygen Youth Health Research Centre and Centre for Youth Mental Health, University of Melbourne, Australia
W. J. Brewer
Affiliation:
Orygen Youth Health Research Centre and Centre for Youth Mental Health, University of Melbourne, Australia
R. Riley
Affiliation:
Public Health, Epidemiology and Biostatistics, School of Health and Population Sciences, University of Birmingham, UK
M. Simmons
Affiliation:
Orygen Youth Health Research Centre and Centre for Youth Mental Health, University of Melbourne, Australia
C. Pantelis
Affiliation:
Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Australia
S. J. Wood
Affiliation:
School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, UK Melbourne Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry, University of Melbourne and Melbourne Health, Australia
*
*Address for correspondence: A. Lin, Ph.D., School of Psychology, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston, Birmingham B15 2TT, UK. (Email: a.lin@bham.ac.uk)

Abstract

Background

Individuals at ultra-high risk (UHR) for psychosis show reduced neurocognitive performance across domains but it is unclear which reductions are associated with transition to frank psychosis. The aim of this study was to investigate differences in baseline neurocognitive performance between UHR participants with (UHR-P) and without transition to psychosis (UHR-NP) and a healthy control (HC) group and examine neurocognitive predictors of transition over the medium to long term.

Method

A sample of 325 UHR participants recruited consecutively from the Personal Assessment and Crisis Evaluation (PACE) Clinic in Melbourne and 66 HCs completed a neurocognitive assessment at baseline. The UHR group was followed up between 2.39 and 14.86 (median = 6.45) years later. Cox regression was used to investigate candidate neurocognitive predictors of psychosis onset.

Results

The UHR group performed more poorly than the HC group across a range of neurocognitive domains but only performance on digit symbol coding and picture completion differed between the groups. The risk of transition was only significantly associated with poorer performance on visual reproduction [hazard ratio (HR) 0.919, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.876–0.965, p = 0.001] and matrix reasoning (HR 0.938, 95% CI 0.883–0.996, p = 0.037). These remained significant even after controlling for psychopathology at baseline.

Conclusions

This study is the longest follow-up of an UHR sample to date. UHR status was associated with poorer neurocognitive performance compared to HCs on some tasks. Cognition at identification as UHR was not a strong predictor of risk for transition to psychosis. The results suggests the need to include more experimental paradigms that isolate discrete cognitive processes to better understand neurocognition at this early stage of illness.

Type
Original Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2013 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Andreasen, N (1982). Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). University of Iowa Press: Iowa City, IA.Google Scholar
APA (1994). DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. American Psychiatric Association: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Army Individual Test Battery (1944). Manual of Directions and Scoring. War Department, Adjutant General's Office: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Becker, HE, Nieman, DH, Dingemans, PM, van de Fliert, JR, De Haan, L, Linszen, DH (2010). Verbal fluency as a possible predictor for psychosis. European Psychiatry 25, 105110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benton, A, Hamsher, K (1983). Multilingual Aphasia Examination. AJA Associates: Iowa City, IA.Google Scholar
Brewer, W, Francey, S, Wood, S, Jackson, H, Pantelis, C, Phillips, L, Yung, A, Anderson, V, McGorry, P (2005). Memory impairments identified in people at ultra-high risk for psychosis who later develop first-episode psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry 162, 7178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, M, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Harrington, H, Taylor, A, Murray, RM, Poulton, R (2002). Evidence for early-childhood, pan-developmental impairment specific to schizophreniform disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort. Archives of General Psychiatry 59, 449456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, TD, Cadenhead, K, Cornblatt, B, Woods, SW, Addington, J, Walker, E, Seidman, LJ, Perkins, D, Tsuang, M, McGlashan, T, Heinssen, R (2008). Prediction of psychosis in youth at high clinical risk. Archives of General Psychiatry 65, 2837.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, D, Ramsey, ME, Gold, JM (2007). Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry 64, 532542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eastvold, AD, Heaton, RK, Cadenhead, KS (2007). Neurocognitive deficits in the (putative) prodrome and first episode of psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 93, 266277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
First, MB, Gibbon, M, Spitzer, RL, Williams, JBW, Benjamin, LS (1997). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders (SCID-II). American Psychiatric Press, Inc.: Washington, DC.Google Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Bonoldi, I, Yung, AR, Borgwardt, SJ, Kempton, M, Valmaggia, L, Barale, F, Caverzasi, E, McGuire, P (2012 a). Predicting psychosis: meta-analysis of evidence of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 220229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fusar-Poli, P, Deste, G, Smieskova, R, Barlati, S, Yung, AR, Howes, O, Stieglitz, RD, Vita, A, McGuire, P, Borgwardt, S (2012 b). Cognitive functioning in prodromal psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry 69, 562571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giuliano, AJ, Li, H, Mesholam-Gately, RI, Sorenson, SM, Woodberry, KA, Seidman, LJ (2012). Neurocognition in the psychosis risk syndrome: a quantitative and qualitative review. Current Pharmaceutical Design 18, 399415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldman, HH, Skodol, AE, Lave, T (1992). Revising Axis V for DSM-IV: a review of measures of social functioning. American Journal of Psychiatry 149, 11481156.Google ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, M (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 23, 5662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, LP, Harris, MG, Amminger, GP, Yuen, HP, Harrigan, SM, Lambert, M, Conus, P, Schwartz, O, Prosser, A, Farrelly, S (2007). Early Psychosis Prevention and Intervention Centre long-term follow-up study of first-episode psychosis: methodology and baseline characteristics. Early Intervention in Psychiatry 1, 4960.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ilonen, T, Heinimaa, M, Korkeila, J, Svirskis, T, Salokangas, RKR (2010). Differentiating adolescents at clinical high risk for psychosis from psychotic and non-psychotic patients with the Rorschach. Psychiatry Research 179, 151156.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, RSE, Perkins, DO, Gu, H, Zipursky, RB, Christensen, BK, Lieberman, JA (2006). A longitudinal study of neurocognitive function in individuals at-risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 88, 2635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, HS, Shin, NY, Jang, JH, Kim, E, Shim, G, Park, HY, Hong, KS, Kwon, JS (2011). Social cognition and neurocognition as predictors of conversion to psychosis in individuals at ultra high risk. Schizophrenia Research 130, 170175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lencz, T, Smith, CW, McLaughlin, D, Auther, A, Nakayama, E, Hovey, L, Cornblatt, BA (2006). Generalized and specific neurocognitive deficits in prodromal schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry 59, 863871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindgren, M, Manninen, M, Laajasalo, T, Mustonen, U, Kalska, H, Suvisaari, J, Moilanen, K, Cannon, TD, Huttunen, M, Therman, S (2010). The relationship between psychotic-like symptoms and neurocognitive performance in a general adolescent psychiatric sample. Schizophrenia Research 123, 7785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, AW, Carter, CS (2002). Approaches to investigating impaired cognition in schizophrenia: a paradigm shift. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 24, 873882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mesholam-Gately, RI, Giuliano, AJ, Goff, D, Faraone, SV, Seidman, LJ (2009). Neurocognition in first-episode schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 23, 315336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, B, Yuen, HP, Wood, SJ, Lin, A, Spiliotacopoulos, D, Bruxner, A, Broussard, C, Simmons, M, Foley, DL, Brewer, WJ, Francey, SM, Amminger, GP, Thompson, A, McGorry, PD, Yung, AR (in press). Long term follow up of a group at ultra high risk (‘prodromal’) for psychosis: the PACE 400 Study. JAMA Psychiatry.Google Scholar
Overall, JE, Gorham, DR (1962). The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychological Reports 10, 799812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pukrop, R, Ruhrmann, S, Schultze-Lutter, F, Bechdolf, A, Brockhaus-Dumke, A, Klosterkötter, J (2007). Neurocognitive indicators for a conversion to psychosis: comparison of patients in a potentially initial prodromal state who did or did not convert to a psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 92, 116125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reichenberg, A, Weiser, M, Rabinowitz, J, Caspi, A, Schmeidler, J, Mark, M, Kaplan, Z, Davidson, M (2002). A population-based cohort study of premorbid intellectual, language, and behavioral functioning in patients with schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and nonpsychotic bipolar disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry 159, 20272035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rey, A (1941). Psychological examination of traumatic encephalopathy. Archives de Psychologie 28, 286340.Google Scholar
Riecher-Rössler, A, Pflueger, MO, Aston, J, Borgwardt, SJ, Brewer, WJ, Gschwandtner, U, Stieglitz, RD (2009). Efficacy of using cognitive status in predicting psychosis: a 7-year follow-up. Biological Psychiatry 66, 10231030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, JE, Fennig, S, Tanenberg-Karant, M, Carlson, G, Craig, T, Galambos, N, Lavelle, J, Bromet, EJ (2000). Congruence of diagnoses 2 years after a first-admission diagnosis of psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry 57, 593600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidman, LJ, Giuliano, AJ, Meyer, EC, Addington, J, Cadenhead, KS, Cannon, TD, McGlashan, TH, Perkins, DO, Tsuang, MT, Walker, EF (2010). Neuropsychology of the prodrome to psychosis in the NAPLS Consortium: relationship to family history and conversion to psychosis. Archives of General Psychiatry 67, 578588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, AE, Grädel, M, Cattapan-Ludewig, K, Gruber, K, Ballinari, P, Roth, B, Umbricht, D (2012). Cognitive functioning in at-risk mental states for psychosis and 2-year clinical outcome. Schizophrenia Research 142, 108115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, AE, Velthorst, E, Nieman, DH, Linszen, D, Umbricht, D, de Haan, L (2011). Ultra high-risk state for psychosis and non-transition: a systematic review. Schizophrenia Research 132, 817.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velthorst, E, Nieman, DH, Becker, HE, van de Fliert, R, Dingemans, PM, Klaassen, R, de Haan, L, van Amelsvoort, T, Linszen, DH (2009). Baseline differences in clinical symptomatology between ultra high risk subjects with and without a transition to psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 109, 6065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1987). Wechsler Memory Scale – Revised. Psychological Corporation: New York.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence. Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Woodberry, KA, Seidman, LJ, Giuliano, AJ, Verdi, MB, Cook, WL, McFarlane, WR (2010). Neuropsychological profiles in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis: relationship to psychosis and intelligence. Schizophrenia Research 123, 188198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yung, A, Phillips, L, Yuen, H, McGorry, P (2004). Risk factors for psychosis in an ultra high-risk group: psychopathology and clinical features. Schizophrenia Research 67, 131142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yung, A, Yuen, H, McGorry, P, Phillips, L, Kelly, D, Dell'Olio, M, Francey, S, Cosgrave, E, Killackey, E, Stanford, C, Godfrey, K, Buckby, J (2005). Mapping the onset of psychosis: the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 39, 964971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yung, AR, Yuen, HP, Berger, G, Francey, S, Hung, TC, Nelson, B, Phillips, L, McGorry, P (2007). Declining transition rate in ultra high risk (prodromal) services: dilution or reduction of risk? Schizophrenia Bulletin 33, 673681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed