Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T22:34:03.109Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of attention bias modification on depressive symptoms in a comorbid sample: a randomized controlled trial

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 January 2023

Ragnhild Bø*
Affiliation:
Clinical Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Brage Kraft
Affiliation:
Clinical Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Division of Psychiatry, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Mads Lund Pedersen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway NORMENT, Division of Mental Health and Addiction, Oslo University Hospital & Institute of Clinical Medicine, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway
Jutta Joormann
Affiliation:
Affect Regulation and Cognition Lab, Yale University, USA
Rune Jonassen
Affiliation:
Faculty of Health Sciences, Oslo Metropolitan University, Norway
Kåre Osnes
Affiliation:
Division of Psychiatry, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway
Catherine J. Harmer
Affiliation:
Clinical Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Department of Psychiatry, Oxford University, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Warneford Hospital, Oxford, OX3 7JX
Nils Inge Landrø
Affiliation:
Clinical Neuroscience Research Group, Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway Division of Psychiatry, Diakonhjemmet Hospital, Oslo, Norway
*
Author for correspondence: Ragnhild Bø, E-mail: ragnhild.bo@psykologi.uio.no
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Studies investigating the long-term effect of attention bias modification (ABM) in clinical samples are lacking. This study investigates the 6-months follow-up effect of ABM on depressive symptoms in participant with major depressive disorder with and without comorbid disorders.

Methods

We conducted a double-blind randomized sham-controlled trial in 101 participants between 19 November 2019, and 17 August 2021. Follow-up ended 3 April 2022. Participants were allocated to ABM or sham condition twice daily for 14 consecutive days. Primary outcomes were the total score on the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) at 6 months, mean Brief State Rumination Inventory (BSRI) score post-treatment and reduction in BSRI post-treatment. Secondary outcome was change in attentional bias (AB). The trial was preregistered in ClinicalTrials.gov (#NCT 04137367).

Results

A total of 118 patients aged 18–65 years were assessed for eligibility, and 101 were randomized and subjected to intention-to-treat analyses. At 6 months, ABM had no effect on depression and anxiety compared to a sham condition. While rumination decreased during the intervention, there was no effect of condition on rumination and AB. Predictor analysis did not reveal differences between participants with ongoing major depressive episode or comorbid anxiety.

Conclusion

Compared to sham training, there was no effect of ABM on depressive symptoms at 6-months follow-up. Since the intervention failed at modifying AB, it is unclear whether changes in AB are related to long-term outcomes.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Attention bias modification (ABM) has shown some promising results as a low-cost intervention for reducing depression symptoms in clinical groups (Hallion & Ruscio, Reference Hallion and Ruscio2011), but only a small number of RCTs have been conducted and overall effect size estimates are small (Fodor et al., Reference Fodor, Georgescu, Cuijpers, Szamoskozi, David, Furukawa and Cristea2020). ABM is a computerized intervention aiming to reduce negative attentional bias (AB). Furthermore, a negative AB is found to be causally related to depressive (Wells & Beevers, Reference Wells and Beevers2010), and anxiety symptoms (Van Bockstaele et al., Reference Van Bockstaele, Verschuere, Tibboel, De Houwer, Crombez and Koster2014). Depression is often comorbid with other mental disorders (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Crum, Warner, Nelson, Schulenberg and Anthony1997), and comorbidity is associated with poorer prognosis (Coplan, Aaronson, Panthangi, & Kim, Reference Coplan, Aaronson, Panthangi and Kim2015) and more severe psychopathology (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005). Treatments targeting transdiagnostic processes, such as negative AB, are therefore needed. ABM paradigms which demonstrate a modification of AB has been found to influence emotional vulnerability (Grafton et al., Reference Grafton, MacLeod, Rudaizky, Holmes, Salemink, Fox and Notebaert2017). ABM may therefore be a viable option for modifying transdiagnostic processes in comorbid samples and lead to symptom reduction

Depression is a disorder characterized by high relapse rates, especially among patients with comorbid disorders (Burcusa & Iacono, Reference Burcusa and Iacono2007), hence treatments should ideally have lasting effects beyond the effect found when terminating treatment. ABM may produce long-lasting effects. For example, a study by Yang, Ding, Dai, Peng, and Zhang (Reference Yang, Ding, Dai, Peng and Zhang2015) found better outcome in the ABM condition as compared to the waitlist and placebo conditions at 3 months in a sample of students with subclinical depressive symptoms. However, the effect was not evident at 7 months follow-up. The same group (Yang, Zhang, Ding, & Xiao, Reference Yang, Zhang, Ding and Xiao2016) also found greater reductions in depressive symptoms for ABM compared to placebo in a sample of adolescents with major depressive disorder (MDD) at 12 months follow-up. de Voogd et al. (Reference de Voogd, Wiers, Prins, de Jong, Boendermaker, Zwitser and Salemink2016) found reductions in depressive symptoms among unselected adolescents up to 12 months following ABM or attentional training, but no difference when compared to placebo. Browning, Holmes, Charles, Cowen, and Harmer (Reference Browning, Holmes, Charles, Cowen and Harmer2012) reported beneficial effect on depressive symptoms 1 month after ABM in adults with remitted MDD, as did Dai, Hu, and Feng (Reference Dai, Hu and Feng2019) in their study including patients with clinical depression. However, findings are not equivocally positive. In a large trial combining ABM and interpretation bias, incidence of depression and symptom severity, was similar across the active and the control condition over 12 months (Basanovic et al., Reference Basanovic, Grafton, Ford, Hirani, Glance, MacLeod and Almeida2020). None of the studies reported on comorbidity. Studies examining the effects of ABM tend to have a short follow-up period, often only assessing the effect immediately after the completion of the intervention (see online Supplemental eTable 2 in review by Fodor et al., Reference Fodor, Georgescu, Cuijpers, Szamoskozi, David, Furukawa and Cristea2020). This limits our knowledge regarding the long-term effect of ABM for depression.

Many studies are also limited by a low number of ABM sessions, spanning 4–10 sessions (Fodor et al., Reference Fodor, Georgescu, Cuijpers, Szamoskozi, David, Furukawa and Cristea2020). According to Blackwell (Reference Blackwell2020), the low number of sessions contradicts knowledge we have about associative learning, which normally requires more and frequent repetitions to produce lasting effects. A notable exception is a recent study by Basanovic et al. (Reference Basanovic, Grafton, Ford, Hirani, Glance, MacLeod and Almeida2020), involving 44 sessions over 52 weeks investigating various depression-related outcomes. In our research group we have examined an ABM paradigm consisting of 28 sessions over 14 consecutive days. This resulted in a small reduction in clinician rated depression (but not in self-reports), immediately after the intervention (Jonassen et al., Reference Jonassen, Harmer, Hilland, Maglanoc, Kraft, Browning and Landrø2019). The same paradigm was previously found by Browning et al. (Reference Browning, Holmes, Charles, Cowen and Harmer2012) to successfully modify AB. Thus, the intensity and frequency of this paradigm seems to be sufficient to induce meaningful change in AB and symptoms.

Building on our former study, the present study includes participants with a broader spectrum of depressive symptoms and comorbid disorders. By targeting a presumed underlying transdiagnostic mechanism, that is negative AB (Garland & Howard, Reference Garland and Howard2013), we investigate whether the effect of this intervention also upholds in a heterogeneous clinical group that reports having problems with depressive symptoms, independent of currently experiencing an ongoing episode of MDD, having comorbid disorders, or not. Negative AB is a trait marker apparent in all phases of MDD (Joormann & Gotlib, Reference Joormann and Gotlib2007), and modification of this cognitive vulnerability marker could both reduce symptom severity and prevent relapse (Browning et al., Reference Browning, Holmes, Charles, Cowen and Harmer2012). Rumination is associated with AB (Hur, Gaul, & Berenbaum, Reference Hur, Gaul and Berenbaum2019) and is a transdiagnostic mechanism mediating the relation between depression and anxiety (McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, Reference McLaughlin and Nolen-Hoeksema2011), and we investigate whether the effect of ABM is mediated by state rumination and change in state rumination. With the long-term aim of introducing ABM as a viable low-threshold treatment option, there is an apparent need for a translational study including long-term follow-up and a representative sample of participants who self-report depressive symptoms. The aim of the current study is to investigate the long-term effect of ABM on depressive symptoms, and whether it is mediated by state rumination, in a sample with and without comorbid disorders in a preregistered trial. Given that symptom reduction is reliant on modification of AB, we predict that ABM will lead to lower levels of depressive symptoms compared to a sham condition at 6-months follow-up.

Methods

Study design

The present study is a double-blind randomized controlled trial comparing the effect of ABM v. a sham condition between November 2019 and April 2022. The study was preregistered at the US National Institutes of Health (ClinicalTrials.gov) # NCT04137367 and approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REK SørØst 2019/33).

Study population

Participants aged 18–65 years with ‘bothersome depression symptoms’ were recruited using local advertisements and social media. Entrance to the trial was via self-referral by phone or online registration. Potential participants were pre-screened by telephone before in-person formal clinical evaluation and enrollment to the trial. Participants gave written informed consent before further evaluation. Demographic information along with information about current and past treatments for mental health, including the current use of psychotropic medication, was obtained. Clinical psychologists or trained psychology students conducted a diagnostic interview using the Norwegian version of the MINI International Neuropsychiatric Interview PLUS 5.0.0 (M.I.N.I.). Inclusion criteria were current or previous MDD, with or without comorbid mental disorders. Exclusion criteria were current or former manic episodes, psychotic episodes, or neurological disorders. It is commonly found that patients experiencing a major depressive episode fulfill the criteria for bipolar disorder (Angst et al., Reference Angst, Azorin, Bowden, Perugi, Vieta and Gamma2011). Therefore, we allowed patients with bipolar II disorder to enter the trial. Since bipolar disorder involves episodes with mood congruent psychotic symptoms, we excluded bipolar I disorder as these symptoms require treatments which targets other mechanisms compared to what ABM may offer.

Eligible participants were invited to return for face-to-face baseline assessment 2 weeks later wherein self-reported anxiety symptomatology [Beck's Anxiety Inventory (BAI); Beck, Epstein, Brown, & Steer, Reference Beck, Epstein, Brown and Steer1988a) and self-reported alcohol consumption habits (AUDIT; Saunders, Aasland, Babor, De la Fuente, & Grant, Reference Saunders, Aasland, Babor, De la Fuente and Grant1993) were assessed. See Fig. 1 for details regarding enrollment.

Fig. 1. CONSORT flow diagram.

Randomization and masking

Allocation to condition occurred once the participant was included in the trial. An independent lab-technician not related to the current investigation conducted a simple randomization of participants by means of lottery draw from slips in a container (1:1 ratio). Both study investigators and participants were blinded to treatment allocation. The code for revealing condition allocation was unlocked in April 2022, after the 6-months follow-up data collection came to an end and after statistical analyses were conducted.

Study intervention

Participants were randomized to receive either ABM or a sham condition. ABM was a computerized visual dot-probe procedure adopted from Browning et al. (Reference Browning, Holmes, Charles, Cowen and Harmer2012) and identical to the one reported in Jonassen et al. (Reference Jonassen, Harmer, Hilland, Maglanoc, Kraft, Browning and Landrø2019). The total number of prescribed sessions was 28 for 14 consecutive days (i.e. twice daily). Each session lasted for approximately 5–7 min and was conducted at home on laptops provided by the research group for this purpose only.

Stimuli consisted of pairs of images of faces displaying emotional expressions: positive (happy), negative (fear or anger) and neutral. Each stimuli pair was derived from two out of three valences (positive-negative, negative-neutral, positive-neutral) displayed horizontally in random order, for either 500 or 1000 ms. Immediately after, a probe (one or two dots) appeared on the computer screen in one of the two locations of the previously displayed stimuli. Participants were instructed to as fast and accurately as possible to indicate the correct number of dots in the probe. Each session consisted of 96 trials with equal number of the three stimuli pairs.

In the ABM condition, the probe appeared in the location of the screen where the relatively more positive stimuli had been displayed for 87% of the trials, and in the location of the relatively more negative stimuli for 13% of the trials. By being exposed to this condition, participants should implicitly learn to deploy their attention to the location of the relatively more positive stimuli, thereby developing a less negative AB. In the sham condition, there was no contingency between probe location and stimuli (i.e. the probe appeared in the location of the screen where the relatively more positive stimuli had been displayed for 50% of the trials). In all other respects, the sham condition was equal to the active condition.

Participants were informed that the trial would investigate the relation between attentional focus and depression. They were also informed about the potential therapeutic properties of ABM and about the randomization procedure but were kept blind of their allocation. However, they were not given any details on the difference between conditions.

According to the preregistration of the trial, the plan was to include an assessment only condition in addition to ABM and the sham condition. However, due to restraints on conducting research during the COVID-19 pandemic, allocation to this condition was discontinued after including five participants. This change to the protocol was registered in clinicaltrials.gov.

Study outcomes

The primary outcome was the Beck's Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II; Beck, Steer, & Carbin, Reference Beck, Steer and Carbin1988b) at six-months follow-up. BDI-II is a self-report measure consisting of 21 items assessing depressive symptoms and attitudes on a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 0 to 3. The questionnaire is among the most commonly used scales for assessing depression in clinical research. Cronbach's α was: α = 0.870 at baseline, α = 0.970 post intervention, and α = 0.907 at six-months follow-up.

Other primary outcomes were state rumination as measured by the Brief State Rumination Inventory (BSRI; Marchetti, Mor, Chiorri, & Koster, Reference Marchetti, Mor, Chiorri and Koster2018) post-treatment and reduction in BSRI from baseline to post-treatment. As stated in the protocol, the original plan was to investigate whether (change in) state rumination mediated the relation between ABM and BDI-II at 6-months follow-up. The BSRI consists of eight items (e.g. ‘Right now, I am thinking: Why do I have problems other people don't have?’) and is a valid and reliable measure of state rumination. Each item is rated on a 100 mm visual analogue scale ranging from ‘completely disagree’ to ‘completely agree’. Before assessing state rumination, the participants went through a stress induction procedure consisting of listening to a recording of an imagery script that was personalized by autobiographical content (see Bø et al., Reference Bø, Kraft, Joormann, Jonassen, Osnes, Harmer and Landrø2022 for details), based on the procedure described by Sinha and Tuit (Reference Sinha and Tuit2012).

The secondary outcome was decrease in negative AB from baseline to post-treatment as measured by a dot probe task. This task is the same as one session of sham condition. AB was calculated as the difference in reaction time in milliseconds between trials where the probe replaced the relatively more negative face v. the more positive face (see Jonassen et al. (Reference Jonassen, Harmer, Hilland, Maglanoc, Kraft, Browning and Landrø2019) for details). Hence, a positive score reflects a relative AB away from negative stimuli, while a negative score reflects a relative AB toward negative stimuli.

Sample size and statistical analysis

We estimated a priori that a total of 100 participants would be needed to detect a difference between ABM and sham condition, with a two-tailed α of 0.05 and (1-β) of 0.80. Power calculation was based on the assumption that 50% of the participants allocated to ABM would report a minimum of 3 points reduction on BDI-II at 6 months, compared to 20% of the participants allocated to the sham condition. In comparison, in the Jonassen et al. (Reference Jonassen, Harmer, Hilland, Maglanoc, Kraft, Browning and Landrø2019) study, 45.1% of the participants in the active condition reported reductions in depression symptom of this magnitude. That sample consisted of participants with MDD in remission and comparatively lower symptom levels, thus justifying a slight increase in the proportion of responders in this trial due to larger opportunity for improvement. According to the National Institute of Health and Clinical Excellence (2009), a difference in ⩾3 BDI-II points is considered a clinically meaningful effect of depression treatment. Assuming that 15% of participants would be lost to follow-up, power calculation indicated the need for 50 participants in each condition.

Baseline characteristics of participants in the two treatment groups were reported using frequency distributions and descriptive statistics including measures of central tendency and dispersion. The primary analysis was conducted using an intention-to-treat approach and included all randomized participants who had either baseline and/or follow-up data (n = 87). Primary outcomes (BDI-II at six-months follow-up, BSRI post training, and change in BSRI from baseline to post-intervention), secondary outcome (change in AB), were analyzed using four mixed-model analyses with random intercepts. For exploratory purposes we investigated the effect of ABM on anxiety (BAI). We also conducted a predictor analysis, including the interaction effect between Ongoing MDD/Comorbid anxiety × Assessment point × Condition in the analysis of BDI-II. This is presented in the online Supplementary materials.

For BDI-II and BAI, the model included condition (ABM, sham training) and assessment point (baseline, post ABM, six-months follow-up) and condition by time interaction as fixed factors. Due to the different intervals between assessment points, we treat time as a categorical variable. The same specifications were used for the analysis of BSRI and AB but included two time points only (baseline and post ABM). The model was fit using restricted maximum likelihood using the lme4 package in R (Bates, Mächler, Bolker, & Walker, Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2015). The ABM treatment effect was operationalized as the least squares mean difference at six-months from this mixed-model repeated measure model. Statistical significance was assessed using the LS mean p value < 0.05.

If a Condition × Assessment point interaction effect [or in the case of predictor analyses (ongoing MDD or comorbid anxiety disorder): Predictor × Condition × Assessment point] was significant, pairwise analysis was used for decomposing the interaction effect. In case of an effect of ABM, we investigated whether state rumination and change in state rumination mediated the effect.

At baseline, 15 participants had missing data on BDI-II, AB, and BAI and 14 participants had missing BSRI data. Participants with missing data at baseline, except for one who had follow-up data, were not included in the analysis. Missing data on follow-ups had the following distribution at various timepoints; BDI-II post-intervention: ABM n = 13, sham condition n = 10; BDI 6-months follow-up: ABM: n = 13, sham condition n = 12; BSRI post-intervention: ABM n = 12, sham condition n = 9; AB post-intervention: ABM n = 15, sham condition n = 9; BAI post-intervention: ABM n = 13, sham condition n = 10; BAI 6-months follow-up: ABM n = 13, sham condition n = 12. Missing data were not imputed as analysis with the mixed-model approach without any ad hoc imputation is shown to be more powerful compared to other alternatives (Chakraborty & Gu, Reference Chakraborty and Gu2009).

All analyses were conducted using SPSS (version 27) and R (version 4.2.0).

Role of the funding source

The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report.

Results

Study population

A total of 118 adults were screened for eligibility, and 101 met eligibility criteria and were randomized between November 2019 and September 2021 (Fig. 1). See Table 1 for baseline demographic and clinical characteristics between participants who were allocated to ABM or a sham condition. Forty-seven participants fulfilled criteria for MDD, 96 for previous MDD, 24 for dysthymia, two for hypomania, 12 for previous hypomania, 14 for panic disorder, 18 for agoraphobia, 31 for social phobia, seven for obsessive-compulsive disorder, eight for post-traumatic stress disorder, 20 for alcohol use disorder, seven for substance use disorder, and 40 for generalized anxiety disorder. The median number of current mental disorder was three. Seventy-six participants completed the trial and provided data at six-months follow-up.

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of all randomized participants

ABM, attentional bias modification; AUDIT, Alcohol Use Disorder Identification Test; BAI, Beck's Anxiety Inventory; BDI-II, Beck's Depression Inventory-II; BSRI, Brief State Rumination Inventory; ISCED, International Standard Classification of Education; MDD, major depressive disorder; SSRI, participants currently using an antidepressant belonging to the selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors.

Note. Data are n (%) or mean (s.d.).

a Data not available for all randomized participants. See text for details.

While potential baseline differences between study arms were not subjected to significance testing, as the differences per definition should be at random due to randomization procedure, we note that the difference in self-reported depression scores correspond to what has been deemed as clinically meaningful (3.2 BDI-II points; NICE, 2009). Prior depression scores are highly predictive of future depression scores, and the random intercepts in the mixed-model analysis takes into account this difference when assessing outcomes.

Primary outcome

Depressive symptoms

Compared to baseline, there was a significant reduction in depression from baseline to post-treatment, t (196.466) = −12.85, p < 0.001, d = −1.84, from baseline to six-months follow-up, t (202.100) = −10.63, p < 0.001, d = −1.50, and from post-treatment to six-months follow-up t (163.4632) = −10.09, p < 0.001, d = −1.58. There was no main effect of ABM condition, t (172.375) = 1.34, p = 0.18, d = 0.20. There was no significant interaction effect between ABM and assessment point from baseline to post-treatment, t (168.810) = 0.018, p = 0.86, d = 0.03, but there was a significant interaction effect between ABM and assessment point from baseline to six-months follow-up, t (169.170) = 2.54, p = 0.012, d = 0.39, and from post-treatment to six-months follow-up, t (150.8753) = 2.25, p = .026, d = 0.37. The interaction effect was in the opposite direction to what was predicted, that is reduction in the sham condition, but not in the ABM condition. The results showed a similar reduction in depression from baseline to post-treatment for both groups, and that the symptom development differed between ABM and sham condition at longer follow-ups, and after treatment termination. At six months, the difference between groups was not significant, F (1,74) = 0.876, p = 0.38, η2 = 0.20 (see Fig. 2). Due to the lack of effect, the planned mediation analysis was not performed.

Fig. 2. Estimated self-reported symptom levels for ABM and placebo at baseline, post-training, and six-months follow-up.

Note. N = 87. BDI-II, Becks Depression Inventory-II.

Conducting pairwise comparisons of assessment points within groups, we found that in the sham condition, there was a significant reduction from baseline to post-training, t (40) = 4.32, p < 0.001, d = 0.68, and from baseline to six months, t (38) = 5.21, p < 0.001, d = 0.83, but no significant change from post-training to six-months follow-up, t (37) = 1.57, p = 0.126, d = 0.25. In the ABM group there was a significant reduction from baseline to post-training, t (35) = 5.78, p < 0.001, d = 0.91, and from baseline to six months, t (35) = 2.280, p = .029, d = 0.38, but no significant change from post-training to six-months follow-up, t (35) = −1.250, p = 0.220, d = −0.21.

See online Supplementary material for predictor analysis for the effect of ongoing MDD and comorbid anxiety, respectively.

State rumination

In BSRI scores, there was a main effect of assessment point, suggesting decrease in state rumination from pre- to post-training, t (80.69) = −2.41, p = 0.02, d = −0.54. There was no main effect of condition, t (124.34) = 0.50, p = 0.62, d = 0.09, nor a significant Assessment point × Condition interaction effect, t (81.11) = −1.20, p = 0.23, d = −0.27. Estimated mean in the active condition was 32.3 and 38.3 in the sham condition. At post-intervention, mean BSRI score in the active group was 34.6 and 32.1 in the sham group. The difference was not significantly different, t (70.692) = −0.47, p = 0.64, d = −0.11.

Secondary outcome

In AB, there was no main effect of assessment point, t (78.41) = 1.52, p = 0.13, d = 0.34, hence AB was not significantly changed from pre- to post-treatment. There was no main effect of condition, t (158.70) = −1.07, p = 0.28, d = −0.17. There was no significant Assessment point × Condition interaction effect, t (80.38) = 1.08, p = 0.28, d = 0.24. Estimated mean AB score in the active condition was −10.73 at baseline and 6.06 post-intervention, and for the sham condition −4.75 at baseline and 3.46 post-intervention. The difference was not significantly different, t (75) = −0.68, p = 0.5, d = −0.16.

Exploratory outcome

For exploratory purposes, we investigated if there was an effect of ABM on anxiety, as measured by BAI. Compared to baseline, there was a significant reduction in anxiety from baseline to follow-up t (145.24) = −3.51, p < 0.001, d = −1.80, and from baseline to 6-months follow-up t (145.92) = −5.35, p < 0.001, d = −0.89, but not from post-treatment to six-months follow-up t (143.612) = −1.88, p = 0.06, d = −0.38. There was no main effect of ABM, t (125.45) = 1.72, p = 0.09, d = 0.31. There was no significant interaction effect between ABM and assessment point from baseline to post-treatment, t (146.257) = −0.74, p = 0.46, d = −0.12, but there was a significant interaction effect between ABM and assessment point from baseline to six-months follow-up, t (143.240) = 2.25, p = 0.03, d = 0.38, and from post-treatment to 6-months follow-up, t (146.57) = 3.01, p < 0.01, d = 0.50. The results indicate a similar reduction in anxiety from baseline to post-treatment for both groups, and that the symptom development differs between groups at longer follow-ups and after treatment termination, where the sham condition continues reductions, while the ABM condition does not.

Pairwise comparisons of assessment points within groups, showed that in the sham condition, there was a significant reduction from baseline to post-training, t (40) = −4.87, p < 0.001, d = −0.76, and from baseline to 6 months, t (38) = −4.36, p < 0.001, d = −0.70, but no significant change from post-training to six-months follow-up, t (37) = 1.34, p = 0.17, d = 0.23. In the ABM condition, there was a significant reduction from baseline to post-training, t (35) = −3.79, p < 0.001, d = 0.63, but not from baseline to six months, t (35) = 0.83, p = 0.41, d = 0.14, and from post-training to six-months follow-up, t (35) = −1.19, p = 0.24, d = −0.20. The difference between conditions at 6 months was not statistically significant, F (1,74) = 1.39, p = 0.41, η2 = 0.19 (see online Supplementary Table S3).

For exploratory purposes, we investigated if change in AB (post-intervention AB–baseline AB) was associated with change in depressive symptoms from baseline to post-intervention (post-intervention BDI-II–baseline BDI-II) and six-months follow-up (six-months follow-up BDI-II–baseline BDI-II), respectively. Correlation analyses showed no association between change in AB and change in BDI-II symptoms between either timepoints in either group, or in groups combined, all r's < 0. 17, p's > 0.3.

Discussion

Main findings

The current study examined the long-term effect of ABM v. a sham condition for depressive symptoms in a sample with and without comorbid disorder. At six-months follow-up, there were no significant differences for either depression or anxiety symptoms. Both groups reduced depression levels during the 14 days training period, but no further reduction was found during six-months follow-up. State rumination was decreased during intervention, but there were no significant differences between conditions. Attention bias was not significantly reduced. Predictor analysis of the effect of ongoing MDD and comorbid anxiety, respectively, on depression symptoms, did not reveal any difference.

While we did find a general reduction in self-reported depression, anxiety symptoms, and state rumination over the course of this trial, the add-on benefit of this intervention compared to the natural course of symptoms is unknown due to lack of data from the discontinued assessment only group.

The intervention failed to modify AB which is the proposed mechanism of ABM. It has been argued that modification of AB is a requirement for ABM to have the intended effect on symptoms (Grafton et al., Reference Grafton, MacLeod, Rudaizky, Holmes, Salemink, Fox and Notebaert2017), hence an effect on symptoms in the ABM group might not be expected. Therefore, the current trial was not able to elucidate the relation between reduced AB and long-term depression severity. In general, long-term follow-ups within depression research are challenging, considering that the depressive episodes for the majority (63%) of the general MDD population ultimately tend to reside within six months (Spijker et al., Reference Spijker, De Graaf, Bijl, Beekman, Ormel and Nolen2002), and the reliance on mean values might have masked different trajectories in depression symptoms.

There is currently no available evidence to support the effectiveness of ABM in long-term reduction of depressive symptoms in adult populations (see also Basanovic et al., Reference Basanovic, Grafton, Ford, Hirani, Glance, MacLeod and Almeida2020; Ferrari, Becker, Smit, Rinck, & Spijker, Reference Ferrari, Becker, Smit, Rinck and Spijker2016 for an ongoing trial). However, the current trial failed to have an effect on AB. Therefore, it might be premature to abandon ABM for long-term reduction in depressive symptom based on this study, in particular since the long-term effect of ABM for depressive symptoms in adolescence shows equivocal effects (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Ding, Dai, Peng and Zhang2015, Reference Yang, Zhang, Ding and Xiao2016). In summary, the current trial brings the overall effect sizes of ABM for depressive symptoms closer to zero.

After ABM, the development of depressive symptoms up to six months was significantly different among groups. While the change within groups was insignificant, the depressive symptoms among the active condition increased, while it continued to decrease among the sham condition. We speculate that the different trajectories might be related to a withdrawal effect from ABM prior to complete recovery. Pertaining to research on antidepressants, there are some indications for a rebound effect, i.e. higher relapse rates or especially severe relapses of depression after the discontinuation of an antidepressant (Henssler, Heinz, Brandt, & Bschor, Reference Henssler, Heinz, Brandt and Bschor2019), and we cannot exclude the possibility of a rebound effect among the participants in the ABM group after treatment termination. If this be the case, a tapering period might be required.

This study took place during the covid-19 pandemic and lockdowns, and we do not know how this affected the outcome of this trial. Implicit tuning of AB through ABM may require face-to-face contact to reach its full potential (Godlewska & Harmer, Reference Godlewska and Harmer2021). Since direct social contact was restricted during the pandemic, it might, although speculative, have limited the effect of ABM.

So why did the intervention fail at modifying AB? The intervention has previously been successful in modifying AB (Browning et al., Reference Browning, Holmes, Charles, Cowen and Harmer2012). The ABM intervention included fearful and angry facial expression only, and sad facial expressions might perhaps have had greater relevance to depression. On the other hand, there has been extensive criticism pointed at the reliability of the dot probe task in measuring AB (e.g. Meissel et al., Reference Meissel, Liu, Stevens, Evans, Britton, Letkiewicz and Shankman2022), and we cannot exclude the possibility that this affects the sensitivity of this measure in this study also.

Limitations

Measures of depressive symptoms should ideally also include a clinician-rated scale (Uher et al., Reference Uher, Perlis, Placentino, Dernovšek, Henigsberg, Mors and Farmer2012). Given that a previous study only found a reduction in clinician-rated (and not self-reported) depressive symptoms (Jonassen et al., Reference Jonassen, Harmer, Hilland, Maglanoc, Kraft, Browning and Landrø2019), including a clinician-rated scale could unveil an ABM effect. Several participants withdrew shortly after randomization; hence, baseline BDI-II was missing for several of the participants. This is a limitation and suggests the need for setting up data collection as to ascertain the recording of important data points at the earliest occasion.

Despite the randomization procedure there was a difference in depressive symptoms at baseline which was clinically meaningful (3.2 BDI-II points). Moreover, current MDD was more prevalent in the ABM group. The therapeutic effect of ABM might be dependent on depression severity and the presence of a diagnosable disorder (Baert, De Raedt, Schacht, & Koster, Reference Baert, De Raedt, Schacht and Koster2010), though symptom severity was not found to moderate the effect of ABM among patients with remitted MDD (Bø et al., Reference Bø, Kraft, Jonassen, Harmer, Hilland, Stiles and Landrø2021). The predictor analyses, investigating the interaction effect between intervention and ongoing MDD/comorbid anxiety disorders, did not reveal any differences based on depression status nor comorbidity. However, analyses were not a priori specified and not subjected to power calculations, nor was randomization procedures stratified for this purpose. The current trial most likely lacks power to detect any but very large predictor effects.

The lack of an assessment-only condition limits interferences of the added benefit of taking part in a structured daily cognitive activity beyond the natural course of depressive symptoms. Moreover, the closely matched control condition of the present trial might be better apt at delineating mechanism in basic research than investigating the effectiveness of ABM in clinical setting (Blackwell, Woud, & MacLeod, Reference Blackwell, Woud and Macleod2017), and future translational studies should reconsider the use of sham training as control condition.

Conclusion

This trial showed that there was no effect of ABM on depressive symptoms at six-months follow-up. These results bring the overall effect sizes of ABM as a depression intervention further towards zero and question the presence of long-term effects. However, since the intervention failed to impact the presumed mechanism of the intervention (AB), we still do not know whether successful modification of AB is related to depressive symptoms in the long-term.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722003956

Financial support

RB is funded as a postdoctoral research fellow by a grant from the Foundation Dam (2019/FO249225). The research is also funded by a grant from South-Eastern Norway Regional Health Authority (2020021) to NIL. The Department of Psychology, University of Oslo, Norway did also support the project. CJH was supported by the NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre. The funding agencies had no role in design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; and preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

NIL has received consultancy fees and travel expenses from Lundbeck outside this work. CJH has received consultancy fees from P1vital, Lundbeck, Sage Therapeutics, Compass Pathways, Zogenix outside of this work. The remaining author(s) declared that there were no conflicts of interest with respect to the authorship or the publication of this article.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Angst, J., Azorin, J.-M., Bowden, C.L., Perugi, G.., Vieta, E., Gamma, A., … BRIDGE Study Group (2011). Prevalence and characteristics of undiagnosed bipolar disorders in patients with a major depressive episode: The BRIDGE study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68 (8), 791798. doi: 10.1001/arcgenpsychiatry.2011.87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baert, S., De Raedt, R., Schacht, R., & Koster, E. H. W. (2010). Attentional bias training in depression: Therapeutic effects depend on depression severity. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41(3), 265274. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basanovic, J., Grafton, B., Ford, A., Hirani, V., Glance, D., MacLeod, C., & Almeida, O. P. (2020). Cognitive bias modification to prevent depression (COPE): Results of a randomised controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 50(15), 25142525. doi: 10.1017/S0033291719002599CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 148. doi: 10.18637/jss.v067.i01CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, A. T., Epstein, N., Brown, G., & Steer, R. A. (1988a). An inventory for measuring clinical anxiety: Psychometric properties. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 56(6), 893.10.1037/0022-006X.56.6.893CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beck, A. T., Steer, R. A., & Carbin, M. G. (1988b). Psychometric properties of the Beck Depression Inventory: Twenty-five years of evaluation. Clinical Psychology Review, 8(1), 77100.10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blackwell, S., Woud, M., & Macleod, C. (2017). A question of control? Examining the role of control conditions in experimental psychopathology using the example of cognitive bias modification research. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 20, E54. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2017.41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blackwell, S. E. (2020). Clinical efficacy of cognitive bias modification interventions. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(6), 465467. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30170-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, R., Kraft, B., Jonassen, R., Harmer, C. J., Hilland, E., Stiles, T. C., … Landrø, N. I. (2021). Symptom severity moderates the outcome of attention bias modification for depression: An exploratory study. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 138, 528534. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.04.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
, R., Kraft, B., Joormann, J., Jonassen, R., Osnes, K., Harmer, C. J., … Landrø, N. I. (2022). Cognitive predictors of mood reactivity and recovery in depression. OSF Preprints, 119. doi: 10.31219/osf.io/45xfjGoogle Scholar
Browning, M., Holmes, E. A., Charles, M., Cowen, P. J., & Harmer, C. J. (2012). Using attentional bias modification as a cognitive vaccine against depression. Biological Psychiatry, 72(7), 572579. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.04.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burcusa, S. L., & Iacono, W. G. (2007). Risk for recurrence in depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 27(8), 959985. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.02.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chakraborty, H., & Gu, H. (2009). A mixed model approach for intent-to-treat analysis in longitudinal clinical trials with missing values [Internet]. Research Triangle Park, NC: RTI Press. doi: 10.3768/rtipress.2009.mr.0009.0903.Google ScholarPubMed
Coplan, J. D., Aaronson, C. J., Panthangi, V., & Kim, Y. (2015). Treating comorbid anxiety and depression: Psychosocial and pharmacological approaches. World Journal of Psychiatry, 5(4), 366378. doi: 10.5498/wjp.v5.i4.366CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dai, Q., Hu, L., & Feng, Z. (2019). Attentional bias modification reduces clinical depression and enhances attention toward happiness. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 109, 145155. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.11.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Voogd, E. L., Wiers, R. W., Prins, P. J. M., de Jong, P. J., Boendermaker, W. J., Zwitser, R. J., & Salemink, E. (2016). Online attentional bias modification training targeting anxiety and depression in unselected adolescents: Short- and long-term effects of a randomized controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 87, 1122. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.08.018CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, G. R., Becker, E. S., Smit, F., Rinck, M., & Spijker, J. (2016). Investigating the (cost-) effectiveness of attention bias modification (ABM) for outpatients with major depressive disorder (MDD): A randomized controlled trial protocol. BMC Psychiatry, 16(1), 370. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-1085-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, L. A., Georgescu, R., Cuijpers, P., Szamoskozi, Ş, David, D., Furukawa, T. A., & Cristea, I. A. (2020). Efficacy of cognitive bias modification interventions in anxiety and depressive disorders: A systematic review and network meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(6), 506514. doi: 10.1016/s2215-0366(20)30130-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garland, E., & Howard, M. O. (2013). A transdiagnostic perspective on cognitive, affective, and neurobiological processes underlying human suffering. Research on Social Work Practices, 24(1), 142151. doi: 10.1177/1049731513503909CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godlewska, B. R., & Harmer, C. J. (2021). Cognitive neuropsychological theory of antidepressant action: A modern-day approach to depression and its treatment. Psychopharmacology, 238(5), 12651278. doi: 10.1007/s00213-019-05448-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grafton, B., MacLeod, C., Rudaizky, D., Holmes, E. A., Salemink, E., Fox, E., & Notebaert, L. (2017). Confusing procedures with process when appraising the impact of cognitive bias modification on emotional vulnerability. British Journal of Psychiatry, 211(5), 266271. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.176123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallion, L. S., & Ruscio, A. M. (2011). A meta-analysis of the effect of cognitive bias modification on anxiety and depression. Psychological Bulletin, 137(6), 940958. doi: 10.1037/a0024355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henssler, J., Heinz, A., Brandt, L., & Bschor, T. (2019). Antidepressant withdrawal and rebound phenomena. Deutsche Ärzteblatt International, 116(20), 355361. doi: 10.3238/arztebl.2019.0355Google ScholarPubMed
Hur, J., Gaul, K., & Berenbaum, H. (2019). Different patterns of attention bias in worry and rumination. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 43, 713725. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-09993-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonassen, R., Harmer, C. J., Hilland, E., Maglanoc, L. A., Kraft, B., Browning, M., … Landrø, N. I. (2019). Effects of attentional bias modification on residual symptoms in depression: A randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry, 19(1), 141. doi: 10.1186/s12888-019-2105-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joormann, J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2007). Selective attention to emotional faces following recovery from depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 8085. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.80CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Crum, R. M., Warner, L. A., Nelson, C. B., Schulenberg, J., & Anthony, J. C. (1997). Lifetime co-occurrence of DSM-III-R alcohol abuse and dependence with other psychiatric disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey. Archives of General Psychiatry, 54(4), 313321.10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830160031005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marchetti, I., Mor, N., Chiorri, C., & Koster, E. H. W. (2018). The Brief State Rumination Inventory (BSRI): Validation and psychometric evaluation. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 42(4), 447460. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9901-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, K. A., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49(3), 186193. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meissel, E. E. E., Liu, H., Stevens, E. S., Evans, T. C., Britton, J. C., Letkiewicz, A. M., & Shankman, S. A. (2022). The reliability and validity of response-based measures of attention bias. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 46(1), 146160. doi: 10.1007/s10608-021-10212-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
National Institute of Health and Clinical Excellence. (2009). Treatment and management of depression in adults, including adults with a chronic physical health problem. Leicester, UK: National Institute of Health and Clinical Excellence.Google Scholar
Saunders, J. B., Aasland, O. G., Babor, T. F., De la Fuente, J. R., & Grant, M. (1993). Development of the alcohol use disorders identification test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption-II. Addiction, 88(6), 791804.10.1111/j.1360-0443.1993.tb02093.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinha, R., & Tuit, K. L. (2012). Imagery script development procedures. New Haven, USA: Yale University School of Medicine.Google Scholar
Spijker, J., De Graaf, R., Bijl, R., Beekman, A., Ormel, J., & Nolen, W. (2002). Duration of major depressive episodes in the general population: Results from the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS). British Journal of Psychiatry, 181(3), 208213. doi: 10.1192/bjp.181.3.208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uher, R., Perlis, R. H., Placentino, A., Dernovšek, M. Z., Henigsberg, N., Mors, O., … Farmer, A. (2012). Self-report and clinician-rated measures of depression severity: Can one replace the other? Depression and Anxiety, 29(12), 10431049.10.1002/da.21993CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Bockstaele, B., Verschuere, B., Tibboel, H., De Houwer, J., Crombez, G., & Koster, E. H. (2014). A review of current evidence for the causal impact of attentional bias on fear and anxiety. Psychological Bulletin, 140(3), 682721. doi: 10.1037/a0034834CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, T. T., & Beevers, C. G. (2010). Biased attention and dysphoria: Manipulating selective attention reduces subsequent depressive symptoms. Cognition and Emotion, 24(4), 719728. doi: 10.1080/02699930802652388CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, W., Ding, Z., Dai, T., Peng, F., & Zhang, J. X. (2015). Attention bias modification training in individuals with depressive symptoms: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 49, 101111. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2014.08.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, W., Zhang, J. X., Ding, Z., & Xiao, L. (2016). Attention bias modification treatment for adolescents with major depression: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 55(3), 208218. e202. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.12.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. CONSORT flow diagram.

Figure 1

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of all randomized participants

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Estimated self-reported symptom levels for ABM and placebo at baseline, post-training, and six-months follow-up.Note. N = 87. BDI-II, Becks Depression Inventory-II.

Supplementary material: File

Bø et al. supplementary material

Bø et al. supplementary material

Download Bø et al. supplementary material(File)
File 24.5 KB