Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T10:16:27.913Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perception of emotional facial expressions in aggression and psychopathy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 September 2024

Timo Stein*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Nina Gehrer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Germany Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
Aiste Jusyte
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Germany
Jonathan Scheeff
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Germany Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
Michael Schönenberg
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Tübingen Center for Mental Health (TüCMH), University of Tübingen, Germany Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, University of Tübingen, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Timo Stein; Email: t.stein@uva.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Altered affective state recognition is assumed to be a root cause of aggressive behavior, a hallmark of psychopathologies such as psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. However, the two most influential models make markedly different predictions regarding the underlying mechanism. According to the integrated emotion system theory (IES), aggression reflects impaired processing of social distress cues such as fearful faces. In contrast, the hostile attribution bias (HAB) model explains aggression with a bias to interpret ambiguous expressions as angry.

Methods

In a set of four experiments, we measured processing of fearful and angry facial expressions (compared to neutral and other expressions) in a sample of 65 male imprisoned violent offenders rated using the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R, Hare, R. D. (1991). The psychopathy checklist–revised. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems) and in 60 age-matched control participants.

Results

There was no evidence for a fear deficit in violent offenders or for an association of psychopathy or aggression with impaired processing of fearful faces. Similarly, there was no evidence for a perceptual bias for angry faces linked to psychopathy or aggression. However, using highly ambiguous stimuli and requiring explicit labeling of emotions, violent offenders showed a categorization bias for anger and this anger bias correlated with self-reported trait aggression (but not with psychopathy).

Conclusions

These results add to a growing literature casting doubt on the notion that fear processing is impaired in aggressive individuals and in psychopathy and provide support for the idea that aggression is related to a hostile attribution bias that emerges from later cognitive, post-perceptual processing stages.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Aggressive behavior is a hallmark of several psychopathologies such as psychopathy and antisocial personality disorder. Two influential models link abnormal aggressive behavior to altered processing of social information. However, the two models differ fundamentally with regard to the proposed alterations in social information processing underlying aggression. In the integrated emotion system theory (IES), successful inhibition of aggressive impulses during development and socialization relies on accurate perception of social distress cues (Blair, Reference Blair2005). In support of the IES theory, deficient perception of distress cues, e.g. of fearful or angry facial expressions, has been found in psychopathy and in antisocial populations (Brook, Brieman, & Kosson, Reference Brook, Brieman and Kosson2013; Jusyte & Schönenberg, Reference Jusyte and Schönenberg2017; Marsh & Blair, Reference Marsh and Blair2008; Montagne et al., Reference Montagne, van Honk, Kessels, Frigerio, Burt, van Zandvoort and de Haan2005; Schönenberg, Louis, Mayer, & Jusyte, Reference Schönenberg, Louis, Mayer and Jusyte2013; Wilson, Juodis, & Porter, Reference Wilson, Juodis and Porter2011; but see Dawel, O'Kearney, McKone, & Palermo, Reference Dawel, O'Kearney, McKone and Palermo2012). This is in marked contrast with other results showing that aggressive individuals, rather than being impaired in processing distress cues, have a heightened sensitivity for angry expressions (Mellentin, Dervisevic, Stenager, Pilegaard, & Kirk, Reference Mellentin, Dervisevic, Stenager, Pilegaard and Kirk2015; Schönenberg & Jusyte, Reference Schönenberg and Jusyte2014; Wilkowski & Robinson, Reference Wilkowski and Robinson2012). Such findings have been interpreted as reflecting a hostile attribution bias (HAB), where aggression results from a bias to interpret ambiguous expressions as angry, resulting in the tendency to ascribe hostile intent to others (Crick & Dodge, Reference Crick and Dodge1994; Dodge, Reference Dodge2006).

Although both models have garnered substantial empirical support, they make opposing predictions regarding the processing of social distress cues in aggressive individuals, with potentially far-reaching implications for the development of interventions and treatment (Penton-Voak et al., Reference Penton-Voak, Thomas, Gage, McMurran, McDonald and Munafò2013; Schönenberg et al., Reference Schönenberg, Christian, Gaußer, Mayer, Hautzinger and Jusyte2014; but see Rosell & Siever, Reference Rosell and Siever2015 for a more contemporary view of mechanisms underlying aggression and violence). While the IES predicts impaired processing of fearful and perhaps angry expressions in aggressive individuals, accounts based on the HAB predict increased sensitivity for angry expressions. One possible reason for these conflicting predictions is that the two models may account for distinct stages in processing social distress cues. Indeed, the IES has been proposed to account for impaired processing at perceptual stages of stimulus encoding (Dadds, el Masry, Wimalaweera, & Guastella, Reference Dadds, el Masry, Wimalaweera and Guastella2008; Sylvers, Brennan, & Lilienfeld, Reference Sylvers, Brennan and Lilienfeld2011), whereas the HAB may reflect an interpretational bias at subsequent cognitive, post-perceptual processing stages. However, not all evidence is consistent with this levels-of-processing account. For example, deficient processing of fearful faces in aggressive individuals has sometimes been linked to impairments at later processing stages related to explicit identification, with initial fear detection and bottom-up attentional orienting to fearful faces being unimpaired (Jusyte, Stein, & Schönenberg, Reference Jusyte, Stein and Schönenberg2019; Stein, Jusyte, Gehrer, Scheeff, & Schönenberg, Reference Stein, Jusyte, Gehrer, Scheeff and Schönenberg2022). Another possibility is that the two models apply to distinct subgroups of aggressive individuals, with the IES providing a better account for instrumental (as opposed to reactive) aggression that is particularly characteristic for individuals with psychopathy (Blair, Reference Blair2001; Glenn & Raine, Reference Glenn and Raine2009). Finally, although results from many individual studies with small sample sizes provide support for the two models, more recent work with larger sample sizes as well as meta-analyses have been calling the robustness and replicability of these findings into question (Faith, Miller, & Kosson, Reference Faith, Miller and Kosson2022; Hoppenbrouwers, Bulten, & Brazil, Reference Hoppenbrouwers, Bulten and Brazil2016).

In a set of four experiments, we measured early, perceptual, and later, post-perceptual levels of processing of fearful and angry facial expressions (compared to neutral and other expressions) in a sample of 65 male imprisoned violent offenders rated using the Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R, Hare, Reference Hare1991; Hollerbach, Mokros, Nitschke, & Habermeyer, Reference Hollerbach, Mokros, Nitschke and Habermeyer2018) and in 60 age-matched control participants. This allowed comparing offenders and controls, as well as testing for an association between psychopathy and self-reported aggression (assessed using the German version of the Buss-Perry Aggression Questionnaire [BPAQ], Buss and Perry, Reference Buss and Perry1992; Herzberg, Reference Herzberg2003) with altered emotion processing in offenders. To test for the fear deficit predicted by the IES theory, we ran two visual search tasks in which participants searched for a target face with a different identity in an array of seven emotionally neutral, identical distractor faces (Jusyte et al., Reference Jusyte, Stein and Schönenberg2019). The target face could show different emotional expressions, and we expected faster visual search for targets with fearful compared to neutral expressions. In the first search task, participants indicated the target's gender, a feature for which emotion was irrelevant, so that a search advantage for fearful faces would reflect bottom-up attentional orienting (Lucas & Vuilleumier, Reference Lucas and Vuilleumier2008). In the second search task, to test for a fear deficit at later processing stages, participants explicitly categorized the target's emotional expression. To test for an HAB, in a separate ‘ambivalence’ experiment we presented blends of angry, fearful, and happy faces and asked participants to identify their emotions (Jusyte & Schönenberg, Reference Jusyte and Schönenberg2017; Schönenberg & Jusyte, Reference Schönenberg and Jusyte2014), expecting that aggression would be related to an increased tendency to categorize ambiguous faces as angry. Finally, to test for biases in perceptual sensitivity for both angry and fearful expressions we used a standard morphing task in which neutral faces were slowly morphed into emotional expressions and participants indicated when they first perceived any emotion in those morphs (Schönenberg et al., Reference Schönenberg, Louis, Mayer and Jusyte2013).

Method

Participants, clinical, and control measures

Offenders with a primary conviction for violent offenses were recruited from cooperating German correctional facilities. We also recruited age-matched control participants. For detailed demographic and clinical sample descriptions see the Supplement and online Supplementary Table S1. Offenders and controls did not differ significantly with regard to age or cognitive abilities as assessed by the 18-item short version of the Wiener Matrizen Test (Formann, Waldherr, & Piswanger, Reference Formann, Waldherr and Piswanger2011). Offenders scored significantly higher than controls on self-reported aggression (all subscales and total score of the BPAQ). On the PCL-R, offenders had a mean score of 19.4 (s.d. 8.4), with 21 offenders scoring 25 or higher, thus qualifying as psychopathic people (this subgroup had a mean PCL-R score of 28.7, s.d. 3.3).

Visual search tasks

For details on the experimental procedure and stimuli for all experiments, see Supplemental Methods. In both visual search task 1 (gender task) and visual search task 2 (emotion task) participants were presented with search arrays consisting of eight faces (Jusyte et al., Reference Jusyte, Stein and Schönenberg2019): seven identical distractors with neutral expression and a target singleton with a different identity. Participants were instructed to press the spacebar as quickly as possible as soon as they identified the gender (task 1) or the emotional expression (task 2) of the identity singleton. After pressing the spacebar, they were asked to indicate the gender (task 1) or the expression (task 2) of the target face by pressing, without speed pressure, a labeled arrow key on the keyboard. In task 1 (gender), the target face differed from the distractors by being a different identity but also having a neutral expression (neutral condition), by being a different identity and having a happy or fearful expression (happy and fear condition), or by being a different identity and being tinted in red (color condition; see Fig. 1a). In task 2 (emotion), the search displays were identical, except that we did not include the color condition.

Figure 1. Visual search tasks. (a) Example search displays. In task 1 (gender task), participants categorized the gender of an identity singleton presented in one of eight locations of an array with seven identical, emotionally neutral distractor faces. In the two emotion conditions, the target additionally had a happy or fearful expression. In the color condition, the emotionally neutral target was additionally tinted in red. In task 2 (emotion task), participants categorized the emotion of an identity singleton presented in one of eight locations of an array with seven identical, emotionally neutral distractor faces. The target face had either a neutral, happy, or fearful expression. (b) Results from task 1 (gender task): Decile plots show RTs for the color, neutral, and fear condition, separately for control participants, all offenders, and for the subgroup of offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (PCL-R score >24). Here and in (c) the happy condition was dropped from the decile plots because of substantial overlap with the fear condition. Shaded error bars represent 95% CIs. Gray insets show median RTs with their associated 95% CIs for all four conditions. (c) Results from task 2 (emotion task). Note that there was no color condition in task 2. (d) Correlation between psychopathy as measured with the PCL-R and the task 1 fear effect (fear minus neutral, left panel) and overall median RTs in the fear and neutral condition from task 1 (right panel). (e) Correlation between PCL-R scores and the task 2 fear effect (fear minus neutral, left panel) and overall median RTs in the fear and neutral condition from task 2 (right panel). In (d) and (e) every circle represents a participant, the solid line the best-fitting linear regression line and the dashed lines the associated 95% confidence bands.

Ambivalence task

Stimuli were morphs of angry, happy, and fearful expressions neutral and emotional expressions (happy, angry, fearful, sad) to create three continuous dimensions (angry/happy, angry/fearful, and fearful/happy), from which we used five distinct intensity levels, yielding

low ambiguity pairs (containing 90% and 10% of each emotion), mid ambiguity pairs (70%/30%), and high ambiguity pairs (50%/50%). Face stimuli were presented for 500 ms and participants identified the subjectively predominant expression via a corresponding button (Jusyte & Schönenberg, Reference Jusyte and Schönenberg2017; Schönenberg & Jusyte, Reference Schönenberg and Jusyte2014).

Morphing task

Stimuli were created by morphing neutral and emotional expressions (happy, angry, fearful, sad), resulting in sequences of 51 frames, presented for 250 ms each, from completely neutral to the full-blown respective emotion. Participants pressed the space bar as soon as they recognized any emotional expression, and we calculated the mean required morphing grade for every emotion (Schönenberg et al., Reference Schönenberg, Louis, Mayer and Jusyte2013).

Data preprocessing, dependent variables, and statistics

For the visual search tasks, we analyzed median response times (RTs) for correct responses. For illustration purposes and to ensure that we did not miss out on differential effects in other parts of the RT distribution, we also calculated and plotted response latencies for all deciles (Fig. 1b and 1c; for analyses of response accuracies and auxiliary analyses, see Supplemental Results). For the ambivalence task, we calculated the proportion of angry and fear judgments for the three different emotion pairs. For the morphing task, we calculated the average required morphing grade for each emotion. For all tasks, participants were excluded from analyses when their grand means (across all experimental conditions) were more than 3 SDs above or below the group mean (across offenders and controls). For all experiments, we report both standard frequentist statistics and Bayes factors (BFs) calculated in JASP (JASP-Team, 2020) with default prior scales (Cauchy distribution, scale 0.707). When frequentist statistics indicate a significant effect, the corresponding BF quantifies the evidence for the alternative hypothesis (BF10); when the effect is not significant, the reported BF quantifies the evidence for the null hypothesis (BF01). When the assumption of sphericity was violated, we used Greenhouse–Geisser correction and report corrected degrees of freedom.

Planned analyses

To test for differences between offenders and controls, we first analyzed the dependent variables from each task with mixed ANOVAs with a repeated-measures factor experimental condition and the between-subject factor group (offenders, controls). Next, another mixed ANOVA restricted to the sample of offenders tested for differences between individuals diagnosed with psychopathy (PCL-R > 24) and without psychopathy (PCL-R < 25). In these ANOVAs we entered all dependent variables, for example all stimulus conditions from the visual search tasks and all ambiguity and emotion conditions from the ambivalence and morphing tasks. Subsequent analyses treated psychopathy and aggression as continuous variables and correlated PCL-R and BPAQ scores with the key dependent variables from each task. The key dependent variables were those for which we expected associations with psychopathy and/or aggression. Specifically, for the visual search tasks, we ran correlations with the fear effect (difference between RTs to fearful and neutral faces) and with overall RTs to fearful faces (as well as with RTs to neutral faces as a non-emotional control). For the ambivalence task, we analyzed responses to highly ambiguous (50%/50%) faces in the fear-happy and angry-happy condition, and for the morphing task we correlated morph grades for fearful and angry faces. For additional correlation analyses including PCL-R and BPAQ sub-scores, see the Supplement (online Supplementary Tables S2 and S3).

Results

Visual search task 1 (gender task)

As can be seen in Fig. 1b, across the whole RT distribution the pattern across conditions was similar for offenders and controls. A mixed ANOVA on the median RTs revealed only a significant main effect of condition (F(2.47, 295.92) = 180.89, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.60, BF10 = 1.46 × 1068), but no significant effect of group (F(1, 120) = 1.66, p = 0.20, ηp 2 = 0.01, BF01 = 1.47), and moderate evidence against an interaction between condition and group (F(2.47, 295.92) = 1.84, p = 0.15, ηp 2 = 0.02, BF01 = 4.97). Responses were significantly faster in the color condition (M = 1.28 s) than in all other conditions (Ms = 1.67–1.82 s, Holm–Sidak's multiple comparison test, all ps < 0.001, all Cohen's ds > 1.44), and significantly slower in the neutral condition than in the happy and fear condition (ps < 0.001, Cohen's ds > 0.53), while there was no significant difference between the happy and fear condition (p = 0.74, Cohen's d = 0.03).

Psychopathy

A mixed ANOVA comparing the effect of condition between offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (N = 21) and without psychopathy (N = 43) revealed no significant effect of group on RTs (F(1, 62) = 0.13, p = 0.72, ηp 2 < 0.01, BF01 = 1.83), and, importantly, moderate evidence against a significant interaction effect (F(2.49, 154.07) = 1.33, p = 0.27, ηp 2 = 0.02, BF01 = 5.21; Figure 1b). Next, we correlated the fear effect with the PCL-R, such that a positive correlation would reflect a smaller effect of fear, relative to neutral, in offenders scoring higher on psychopathic traits. This correlation was not significant (r(62) = −0.04, p = 0.74, BF01 = 6.06; Figure 1d, left panel). The correlation between overall RTs to fearful faces with the PCL-R scores was positive but not significant (r(62) = 0.23, p = 0.069, BF01 = 1.28); a similar positive (non-significant) association was found when correlating RTs to neutral faces with PCL-R scores (r(62) = 0.23, p = 0.064, BF01 = 1.20; Figure 1d, right panel), thus providing no evidence for a fear-specific processing impairment in psychopathy.

Aggression

There were no significant correlations between BPAQ scores and the fear effect (r(120) < 0.01, p = 0.99, BF01 = 8.83) or overall RTs to fearful faces (r(120) = 0.09, p = 0.33, BF01 = 5.49).

Visual search task 2 (emotion task)

Similar to the gender task, also in the emotion task the pattern of RT distributions across conditions was similar for offenders and controls (Fig. 1c). A mixed ANOVA on the median RTs revealed only a significant main effect of condition (F(1.23, 149.21) = 112.22, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.48, BF10 = 1.54 × 1032), but no significant effect of group (F(1, 121) = 0.75, p = 0.39, ηp 2 < 0.01, BF01 = 2.56). Overall, responses were significantly slower in the neutral condition (M = 2.20 s) than in the happy and fear condition (Holm-Sidak's multiple comparison test, both ps < 0.001, both Cohen's ds > 1.11), while the happy and fear condition did not differ significantly (M = 1.60 s and M = 1.65 s, respectively, p = 0.24, Cohen's d = −0.11). There was strong evidence against an interaction between condition and group (F(1.23, 149.21) = 0.19, p = 0.71, ηp 2 < 0.01, BF01 = 16.02).

Psychopathy

A mixed ANOVA comparing the effect of condition between offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (N = 21) and without psychopathy (N = 43) revealed no significant effect of group on RTs (F(1, 62) = 1.27, p = 0.26, ηp 2 = 0.02, BF01 = 1.49), and, importantly, moderate evidence against a group-by-condition effect on RTs (F(1.45, 89.98) = 0.74, p = 0.44, ηp 2 = 0.01, BF01 = 5.66; Figure 1c). As for task 1, we correlated the fear effect (difference between RTs to fearful and neutral faces) with the PCL-R, and again found no significant correlation, with a trend in the opposite direction (r(62) = −0.23, p = 0.070, BF01 = 1.29; Figure 1e, left panel). Although the correlation between overall RTs to fearful faces with the PCL-R scores was positive, it was not significant (r(62) = 0.24, p = 0.061, BF01 = 1.15), and a similar positive association was found when correlating RTs to neutral faces with PCL-R scores (r(62) = 0.29, p = 0.019, BF10 = 2.26; Figure 1e, right panel). Thus, as for gender-search task 1, these results provide no evidence for a fear-specific deficit associated with psychopathy.

Aggression

There were no significant correlations between BPAQ scores and the fear effect (r(121) = −0.12, p = 0.19, BF01 = 3.75) or overall RTs to fearful faces (r(120) = 0.01, p = 0.90, BF01 = 8.79).

Ambivalence task

Figure 2b shows participants’ emotion judgments for the three emotion pairs (fear-happy, angry-happy, angry-fear) as a function of the percentage of each emotion contained in the face stimuli. Judgments of control participants, all offenders, and offenders diagnosed with psychopathy were almost identical for fear-happy and angry-fear pairs, but appeared to differ for angry-happy pairs, in particular for the high ambiguity condition consisting of 50% angry and 50% happy (Fig. 3).

Figure 2. Ambivalence task. (a) Example stimuli, showing 30%/70%, 50%/50%, and 70%/30% blends for fear and happy (left), angry and happy (middle), and angry and fear (right). Participants indicated the emotion in these blends. (b). Proportion of fear/angry responses for fear-happy, angry-happy, and angry-fear blends, shown separately for controls, offenders, and for the subgroup of offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (PCL-R score > 24). (c) Proportion of fear/angry responses averaged across low ambiguity pairs (containing 90% and 10% of each emotion), mid ambiguity pairs (70%/30%), and high ambiguity pairs (50%/50%). Error bars represent 95% CIs, and the arrow highlights the key difference between controls and offenders for high-ambiguity angry-happy pairs. (d) Correlation between PCL-R scores (from offenders) and the proportion of fear/angry responses for high-ambiguity (50%/50%) fear-happy (left panel) and angry-happy (right panel) pairs. (e) Correlation between aggression as measured with the BPAQ (from all participants) and the proportion of fear/angry responses for high-ambiguity (50%/50%) fear-happy (left panel) and angry-happy (right panel) pairs. In (d) and (e), every circle represents a participant, the solid line the best-fitting linear regression line and the dashed lines the associated 95% confidence bands.

Figure 3. Morphing task. (a) Example morph stimuli, from all neutral (0% fear) to 100% fear. (b) Mean required morphing grade for correctly indicating an emotion, shown separately for morphs between neutral and fear, happy, angry, and sad, and for controls, offenders, and for the subgroup of offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (PCL-R score >24). Error bars represent 95% CIs. (c) Correlation between PCL-R scores (from offenders) and the required morphing grade for fear-morphs (left panel) and angry-morphs (right panel). (d) Correlation between BPAQ scores (from all participants) and the required morphing grade for fear-morphs (left panel) and angry-morphs (right panel). In (c) and (d), every circle represents a participant, the solid line the best-fitting linear regression line and the dashed lines the associated 95% confidence bands.

To quantify this impression, for Fig. 2c we averaged responses to low ambiguity pairs (containing 90% and 10% of each emotion), mid ambiguity pairs (70%/30%), and high ambiguity pairs (50%/50%). This increased statistical power for analyzing the proportion of fear/angry responses in a mixed ANOVA with the factors emotion pair (fear-happy, angry-happy, angry-fear), ambiguity (low, mid, high), and group (control, offenders). While the effect of group was not significant, there were significant main effects of emotion pair and ambiguity, as well as an interaction between emotion pair and ambiguity, and an interaction between ambiguity and group (F(1.28, 157.48) = 3.67, p = 0.047, ηp 2 = 0.03, but BF10 = 0.25), reflecting a stronger effect of ambiguity on responses by offenders than controls, and a trend towards a significant three-way interaction (F(2.69, 330.41) = 2.37, p = 0.078, ηp 2 = 0.19, but BF10 = 0.11). Follow-up ANOVAs conducted separately for the three emotion-pair conditions revealed significant differences in the effect of ambiguity between offenders and controls only in the angry-happy condition (F(1.29, 158.61) = 6.82, p = 0.006, ηp 2 = 0.05, BF10 = 21.03), but not in the fear-happy (F(1.36, 167.61) = 1.44, p = 0.24, ηp 2 = 0.01, BF01 = 5.48) or angry-fear (F(1.46, 178.95) = 0.29, p = 0.68, ηp 2 < 0.01, BF01 = 14.14) condition. In the angry-happy condition, offenders judged high-ambiguity (50%/50%) faces significantly more often as ‘angry’ than controls (t(123) = 2.43, p = 0.016, Cohen's d = 0.44, BF10 = 2.70), while there were no significant differences between offenders and controls in the low- and mid-ambiguity conditions (both t(123) < 0.57, p > 0.56, Cohen's d < 0.11, BF01 > 4.50).

Psychopathy

A mixed ANOVA testing the effects of emotion pair and ambiguity between offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (N = 21) and without psychopathy (N = 44) revealed no significant interactions with participant group, with strong evidence against a three-way interaction (F(2.74, 85.31) = 1.11, p = 0.34, ηp 2 = 0.02, BF10 = 12.81). To test for an association of psychopathy with fear- and angry judgments of highly ambiguous stimuli, we correlated PLC-R scores with judgments of highly ambiguous (50%/50%) faces in the fear-happy and angry-happy condition. For fear judgments of fear-happy pairs with high ambiguity, there was no significant correlation (r(63) = −0.08, p = 0.54, BF01 = 5.37; Fig. 2d, left panel). For angry judgments of angry-happy pairs with high ambiguity, there was a trend for an increased (non-significant) tendency to judge ambiguous faces as ‘angry’ with higher PCL-R scores (r(63) = 0.23, p = 0.066, but BF10 = 0.81; Fig. 2d, right panel).

Aggression

Analogous correlation analyses revealed no significant correlation between BPAQ scores and fear judgments of fear-happy pairs with high ambiguity (r(123) = 0.12, p = 0.20, BF01 = 3.75; Fig. 2e, left panel) but a moderate correlation between BPAQ scores and angry judgments of angry-happy pairs with high ambiguity (r(123) = 0.24, p = 0.007, BF10 = 4.28; Fig. 2e, right panel). However, when restricting this analysis to the sample of offenders, the correlation between BPAQ scores and angry judgments of angry-happy pairs with high ambiguity was no longer significant r(63) = 0.19, p = 0.13, BF01 = 2.03; see online Supplementary Table S3).

Morphing task

A mixed ANOVA with the within-subjects factor condition (fear, anger, happy, sad) and the between-subjects factor group (control, offenders) only yielded a significant main effect of condition (F(2.63, 318.60) = 456.37, p < 0.001, ηp 2 = 0.79, BF10 = 6.26 × 10119), reflecting a lower required morphing grade for happy (M = 31.7%) than for the other emotions (Ms = 59.7–63.5%). There was no significant effect of group (F(1, 121) = 0.25, p = 0.62, ηp 2 < 0.01, BF01 = 3.40) and no significant interaction (F(2.63, 318.60) = 0.84, p = 0.46, ηp 2 < 0.01, BF01 = 17.96).

Psychopathy

A mixed ANOVA comparing the four emotions between offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (PCL-R > 24, N = 20) and without psychopathy (PLC-R < 25, N = 43) revealed no significant effect of group (F(1, 61) = 1.70, p = 0.20, ηp 2 = 0.03, BF01 = 1.43) or interaction (F(3, 183) = 1.55, p = 0.20, ηp 2 = 0.03, BF01 = 3.25). Also for accuracies there were no significant group or interaction effects (Fs < 1, ps > 0.39, ηp 2 < 0.02, BF01 > 7.16). Additional correlation analyses revealed no significant correlations between PCL-R scores and morph grade for fear (r(61) = 0.01, p = 0.94, BF01 = 6.34) or anger (r(61) = −0.108, p = 0.43, BF01 = 4.69).

Aggression

Similarly, there were no significant correlations between BPAQ scores and morph grade for fear (r(121) = 0.07, p = 0.48, BF01 = 6.89) or anger (r(121) = −0.05, p = 0.62, BF01 = 7.83).

Discussion

Across four experiments, our results provide no evidence for a fear deficit in violent offenders or for an association of psychopathy or aggression with impaired processing of fearful faces. These results add to a growing literature casting doubt on the idea of impaired fear processing related to aggression and psychopathy (Deming et al., Reference Deming, Eisenbarth, Rodrik, Weaver, Kiehl and Koenigs2022; Faith et al., Reference Faith, Miller and Kosson2022; Hoppenbrouwers et al., Reference Hoppenbrouwers, Bulten and Brazil2016; Stein et al., Reference Stein, Jusyte, Gehrer, Scheeff and Schönenberg2022). Neither did we find evidence for a perceptual bias for angry faces linked to psychopathy or aggression. However, when explicit labeling of the expression of highly ambiguous stimuli was required, we found a categorization bias for anger in violent offenders and a correlation with self-reported trait aggression (but not with psychopathy) for the whole tested sample (controls and offenders). Together, these findings support the notion that aggression is related to a hostile attribution bias that emerges from later post-perceptual ‘cognitive’ processing stages involved in explicit stimulus categorization and labeling.

There are several possible explanations for why the present study did not reveal some of the previously reported alterations in emotion processing associated with psychopathy and aggression. For example, the present explicit visual search task required mental categorization and labeling of the emotion, but in contrast to our previous study (Jusyte et al., Reference Jusyte, Stein and Schönenberg2019) we did not map different emotion categories on separate buttons requiring speeded responses, thus ruling out possible effects related to motor preparation and execution. We also improved the morphing task by including more trials than previous studies (Schönenberg et al., Reference Schönenberg, Louis, Mayer and Jusyte2013, Reference Schönenberg, Christian, Gaußer, Mayer, Hautzinger and Jusyte2014), thus increasing power and reducing possible effects of differential training and familiarity with computer-based tasks between clinical and control samples.

Moreover, our study is unique in matching samples on fluid intelligence (measured with the Wiener Matrizen Test, Formann et al., Reference Formann, Waldherr and Piswanger2011), while previous studies, if at all, only measured crystalline intelligence and did not take general mental ability into account. Indeed, a recent study found that when general mental ability was controlled for, psychopathy was inherently independent of deficits in the ability to perceive emotional expressions, both generally and for specific emotions. Based on their results, the authors conclude that psychopathy is genuinely associated with deficits in general mental ability and that this deficit, in turn, accounts for the observed impairments in emotion perception (Olderbak, Mokros, Nitschke, Habermeyer, & Wilhelm, Reference Olderbak, Mokros, Nitschke, Habermeyer and Wilhelm2018). Taken together, methodological issues and weaknesses including the use of heterogeneous emotion perception tasks with low power, reliance on small samples as well as insufficient control for third variable explanations might have led to the inconsistent findings reported in the field.

With regard to theoretical models that propose a link between abnormal aggressive behavior, psychopathy, and altered processing of social information, the present findings provide no empirical support that emotion perception deficits were strongest for fear, sadness, and happiness, thereby challenging central assumptions of the IES, according to which deficits should primarily concern these care-based emotions (Blair, Reference Blair2001, Reference Blair2005). The IES perspective posits that psychopathic individuals have difficulty inhibiting violence and aggression due to impaired recognition of these emotions, particularly of fearful expressions. The current findings, in concert with a recent study that used a signal detection theory approach (Faith et al., Reference Faith, Miller and Kosson2022), however, suggest that the deficits in violence inhibition seen in psychopathy reflect a mechanism other than impairments in fear recognition.

In contrast, the cognitive bias for anger associated with aggression (i.e. hostile attribution bias) appears to be independent of general mental ability and may thus reflect a genuine emotion-specific alteration in cognitive processing. We found a significantly stronger bias to categorize ambiguous angry–happy morphs as angry in offenders than in controls. Previous work suggests that this bias is particularly related to reactive rather than instrumental aggression (Philipp-Wiegmann, Rösler, Retz-Junginger, & Retz, Reference Philipp-Wiegmann, Rösler, Retz-Junginger and Retz2017), which might explain the correlation with aggressive behavior but not psychopathy in the current study. It should be noted, however, that this correlation was significant only when considering the whole tested sample (controls and offenders), but not when restricting the analysis to offenders only, as for all correlations with psychopathy scores. This observation is consistent with a recent review that reported small to medium associations between the hostile attribution bias and aggression in adults across different samples (Klein Tuente, Bogaerts, & Veling, Reference Klein Tuente, Bogaerts and Veling2019). The authors found no evidence for a stronger association between the hostile attribution bias and aggression in groups displaying higher levels of aggression (e.g. forensic patients, offenders) than in students or general populations. In general, more aggressive individuals attribute more hostile intentions to people in ambiguous and/or clearly hostile social situations. The association between HAB and aggression therefore seems to capture a general mechanism underlying aggression rather than a pathological relation.

Further research is however required to examine whether the hostile attribution bias in face perception represents a general precursor (e.g. in aggression-prone youths), how it relates to subtypes of aggressive behavior, whether it may be modified by conventional therapeutic strategies or even represent a possible target for new computer-based training approaches. Indeed, there is first evidence that hostile attributions may be reduced with relatively simple means, such as implicit bias modification (Penton-Voak et al., Reference Penton-Voak, Thomas, Gage, McMurran, McDonald and Munafò2013; Stoddard et al., Reference Stoddard, Sharif-Askary, Harkins, Frank, Brotman, Penton-Voak and Leibenluft2016). Such experimental manipulations may help understand the dynamics of biased decision-making processes and simultaneously inform effective intervention strategies.

As with other empirical studies, the present study has some limitations. First, we included only a moderately sized sample of male inmates, a relatively rare sample of violent offenders spanning a single developmental period, thus compromising generalizability to female offenders and/or nonincarcerated individuals high in psychopathy or aggression. Second, we only studied psychopathy in a clinical sense, restricting diagnosis of psychopathic traits to offenders, so that we could not examine associations with psychopathy in the absence of violent offending. However, development of and research using the PCL-R has been based on samples of offenders and forensic psychiatric patients, so that this conflation is common in research on psychopathy. Future work could include other groups such as community samples with psychopathic traits. Furthermore, we mostly focused on attentional aspects of emotion processes and exclusively on facial processing, and the stimulus material exclusively consisted of male faces. Future studies should therefore employ a more complete and ecologically valid stimulus set and tasks that go beyond attentional processes. For example, we did not include expressions of all basic emotions in all experiments and only used static images, so that we may have missed potential processing alterations for specific dynamic facial expressions (Decety, Skelly, Yoder, & Kiehl, Reference Decety, Skelly, Yoder and Kiehl2014).

Relatedly, and more fundamentally, some recent evidence challenges the widespread assumption of a universal, uniform, and stable production of six facial expressions of basic emotion and argues for a new, more dynamic view of the function, morphology, and signal value of facial behavior (Durán & Fernández-Dols, Reference Durán and Fernández-Dols2021). Facial movements commonly thought to signal particular emotions regardless of context, person, and culture are not universally diagnostic of emotional states, and thus future research should employ multimodal approaches by utilizing data from multiple sources, such as facial expressions, posture and gait, tone of voice, or gaze to allow a more comprehensive and accurate assessment of human emotional states (Barrett, Adolphs, Marsella, Martinez, & Pollak, Reference Barrett, Adolphs, Marsella, Martinez and Pollak2019). Because of these limitations, our findings do not refute the extensive literature linking emotional processing mechanisms with aggressive behaviors.

In conclusion, across four experiments we found no evidence for perceptual deficits for emotion (including fear) in psychopathy, but a cognitive bias for anger linked to aggression. These results challenge the view that psychopathy arises from altered emotion processing and support the idea that a hostile attribution bias may underlie aggressive behavior.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724001417.

Funding statement

This study was funded by DFG grant SCHO 1448/3-1 awarded to Michael Schönenberg.

Competing interests

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., & Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 20(1), 168. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100619832930CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2001). Neurocognitive models of aggression, the antisocial personality disorders, and psychopathy. Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 71(6), 727731. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.71.6.727CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, R. J. R. (2005). Applying a cognitive neuroscience perspective to the disorder of psychopathy. Development and Psychopathology, 17(3), 865891. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brook, M., Brieman, C. L., & Kosson, D. S. (2013). Emotion processing in psychopathy checklist - assessed psychopathy: A review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review 33(8), 979995. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2013.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buss, A. H., & Perry, M. (1992). The aggression questionnaire. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(3), 452459. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.3.452CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, N. R., & Dodge, K. A. (1994). A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 115(1), 74101. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dadds, M. R., el Masry, Y., Wimalaweera, S., & Guastella, A. J. (2008). Reduced eye gaze explains “fear blindness” in childhood psychopathic traits. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(4), 455463. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816407f1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawel, A., O'Kearney, R., McKone, E., & Palermo, R. (2012). Not just fear and sadness: Meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 36(10), 22882304. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2012.08.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Skelly, L., Yoder, K. J., & Kiehl, K. A. (2014). Neural processing of dynamic emotional facial expressions in psychopaths. Social Neuroscience, 9(1), 3649. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.866905CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deming, P., Eisenbarth, H., Rodrik, O., Weaver, S. S., Kiehl, K. A., & Koenigs, M. (2022). An examination of autonomic and facial responses to prototypical facial emotion expressions in psychopathy. PLoS ONE, 17(7), e0270713. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0270713CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dodge, K. A. (2006). Translational science in action: Hostile attributional style and the development of aggressive behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology, 18(3), 791814. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579406060391CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durán, J. I., & Fernández-Dols, J. M. (2021). Do emotions result in their predicted facial expressions? A meta-analysis of studies on the Co-occurrence of expression and emotion. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 21(7), 15501569. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Faith, R. N., Miller, S. A., & Kosson, D. S. (2022). Facial affect recognition and psychopathy: A signal detection theory perspective. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 44(3), 738749. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-022-09969-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Formann, A. K., Waldherr, K., & Piswanger, K. (2011). Wiener Matrizen–Test 2. Manual. Beltz Test GmbH.Google Scholar
Glenn, A. L., & Raine, A. (2009). Psychopathy and instrumental aggression: Evolutionary, neurobiological, and legal perspectives. International Journal of Law and Psychiatry, 32(4), 253258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijlp.2009.04.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, R. D. (1991). The psychopathy checklist–revised. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems.Google Scholar
Herzberg, P. Y. (2003). Faktorstruktur, Gütekriterien und Konstruktvalidität der deutschen Übersetzung des Aggressionsfragebogens von Buss und Perry. Zeitschrift Für Differentielle Und Diagnostische Psychologie, 24(4), 311323. https://doi.org/10.1024/0170-1789.24.4.311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollerbach, P., Mokros, A., Nitschke, J., & Habermeyer, E. (2018). Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised: German language adaptation and recommendations for correct use. Forensische Psychiatrie, Psychologie, Kriminologie, 12(3), 186191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11757-018-0478-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoppenbrouwers, S. S., Bulten, B. H., & Brazil, I. A. (2016). Parsing fear: A reassessment of the evidence for fear deficits in psychopathy. Psychological Bulletin, 142(6), 573600. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000040CrossRefGoogle Scholar
JASP Team. (2020). JASP (Version 0.12.2) [Computer software].Google Scholar
Jusyte, A., & Schönenberg, M. (2017). Impaired social cognition in violent offenders: Perceptual deficit or cognitive bias? European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 267(3), 257266. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-016-0727-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jusyte, A., Stein, T., & Schönenberg, M. (2019). Fear processing deficit in violent offenders: Intact attentional guidance but impaired explicit categorization. Psychology of Violence, 9(3), 308318. https://doi.org/10.1037/vio0000109CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein Tuente, S., Bogaerts, S., & Veling, W. (2019). Hostile attribution bias and aggression in adults – A systematic review. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 46, 6681. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.avb.2019.01.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucas, N., & Vuilleumier, P. (2008). Effects of emotional and non-emotional cues on visual search in neglect patients: Evidence for distinct sources of attentional guidance. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 14011414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A., & Blair, R. J. R. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(3), 454465. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellentin, A. I., Dervisevic, A., Stenager, E., Pilegaard, M., & Kirk, U. (2015). Seeing enemies? A systematic review of anger bias in the perception of facial expressions among anger-prone and aggressive populations. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 25, 373383. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.AVB.2015.09.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montagne, B., van Honk, J., Kessels, R. P. C., Frigerio, E., Burt, M., van Zandvoort, M. J. E., … de Haan, E. H. F. (2005). Reduced efficiency in recognising fear in subjects scoring high on psychopathic personality characteristics. Personality and Individual Differences, 38(1), 511. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2004.02.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olderbak, S. G., Mokros, A., Nitschke, J., Habermeyer, E., & Wilhelm, O. (2018). Psychopathic men: Deficits in general mental ability, not emotion perception. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 127(3), 294304. https://doi.org/10.1037/ABN0000340CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penton-Voak, I. S., Thomas, J., Gage, S. H., McMurran, M., McDonald, S., & Munafò, M. R. (2013). Increasing recognition of happiness in ambiguous facial expressions reduces anger and aggressive behavior. Psychological Science, 24(5), 688697. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612459657CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Philipp-Wiegmann, F., Rösler, M., Retz-Junginger, P., & Retz, W. (2017). Emotional facial recognition in proactive and reactive violent offenders. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 267(7), 687695. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-017-0776-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosell, D. R., & Siever, L. J. (2015). The neurobiology of aggression and violence. CNS Spectrums 20(3), 254279. https://doi.org/10.1017/S109285291500019XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schönenberg, M., Christian, S., Gaußer, A. K., Mayer, S. v., Hautzinger, M., & Jusyte, A. (2014). Addressing perceptual insensitivity to facial affect in violent offenders: First evidence for the efficacy of a novel implicit training approach. Psychological Medicine, 44(5), 10431052. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291713001517CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schönenberg, M., & Jusyte, A. (2014). Investigation of the hostile attribution bias toward ambiguous facial cues in antisocial violent offenders. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 264(1), 6169. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-013-0440-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schönenberg, M., Louis, K., Mayer, S., & Jusyte, A. (2013). Impaired identification of threat-related social information in male delinquents with antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27(4), 496505. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2013_27_100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stein, T., Jusyte, A., Gehrer, N. A., Scheeff, J., & Schönenberg, M. (2022). Intact prioritization of fearful faces during continuous flash suppression in psychopathy. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 131(5), 517523. https://doi.org/10.1037/ABN0000753CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoddard, J., Sharif-Askary, B., Harkins, E. A., Frank, H. R., Brotman, M. A., Penton-Voak, I. S., … Leibenluft, E. (2016). An open pilot study of training hostile interpretation bias to treat disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 26(1), 4957. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2015.0100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sylvers, P. D., Brennan, P. A., & Lilienfeld, S. O. (2011). Psychopathic traits and preattentive threat processing in children: A novel test of the fearlessness hypothesis. Psychological Science, 22(10), 12801287. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611420730CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkowski, B. M., & Robinson, M. D. (2012). When aggressive individuals see the world more accurately: The case of perceptual sensitivity to subtle facial expressions of anger. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(4), 540553. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211430233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, K., Juodis, M., & Porter, S. (2011). Fear and loathing in psychopaths: A meta-analytic investigation of the facial affect recognition deficit. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 38(7), 659668. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093854811404120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Visual search tasks. (a) Example search displays. In task 1 (gender task), participants categorized the gender of an identity singleton presented in one of eight locations of an array with seven identical, emotionally neutral distractor faces. In the two emotion conditions, the target additionally had a happy or fearful expression. In the color condition, the emotionally neutral target was additionally tinted in red. In task 2 (emotion task), participants categorized the emotion of an identity singleton presented in one of eight locations of an array with seven identical, emotionally neutral distractor faces. The target face had either a neutral, happy, or fearful expression. (b) Results from task 1 (gender task): Decile plots show RTs for the color, neutral, and fear condition, separately for control participants, all offenders, and for the subgroup of offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (PCL-R score >24). Here and in (c) the happy condition was dropped from the decile plots because of substantial overlap with the fear condition. Shaded error bars represent 95% CIs. Gray insets show median RTs with their associated 95% CIs for all four conditions. (c) Results from task 2 (emotion task). Note that there was no color condition in task 2. (d) Correlation between psychopathy as measured with the PCL-R and the task 1 fear effect (fear minus neutral, left panel) and overall median RTs in the fear and neutral condition from task 1 (right panel). (e) Correlation between PCL-R scores and the task 2 fear effect (fear minus neutral, left panel) and overall median RTs in the fear and neutral condition from task 2 (right panel). In (d) and (e) every circle represents a participant, the solid line the best-fitting linear regression line and the dashed lines the associated 95% confidence bands.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Ambivalence task. (a) Example stimuli, showing 30%/70%, 50%/50%, and 70%/30% blends for fear and happy (left), angry and happy (middle), and angry and fear (right). Participants indicated the emotion in these blends. (b). Proportion of fear/angry responses for fear-happy, angry-happy, and angry-fear blends, shown separately for controls, offenders, and for the subgroup of offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (PCL-R score > 24). (c) Proportion of fear/angry responses averaged across low ambiguity pairs (containing 90% and 10% of each emotion), mid ambiguity pairs (70%/30%), and high ambiguity pairs (50%/50%). Error bars represent 95% CIs, and the arrow highlights the key difference between controls and offenders for high-ambiguity angry-happy pairs. (d) Correlation between PCL-R scores (from offenders) and the proportion of fear/angry responses for high-ambiguity (50%/50%) fear-happy (left panel) and angry-happy (right panel) pairs. (e) Correlation between aggression as measured with the BPAQ (from all participants) and the proportion of fear/angry responses for high-ambiguity (50%/50%) fear-happy (left panel) and angry-happy (right panel) pairs. In (d) and (e), every circle represents a participant, the solid line the best-fitting linear regression line and the dashed lines the associated 95% confidence bands.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Morphing task. (a) Example morph stimuli, from all neutral (0% fear) to 100% fear. (b) Mean required morphing grade for correctly indicating an emotion, shown separately for morphs between neutral and fear, happy, angry, and sad, and for controls, offenders, and for the subgroup of offenders diagnosed with psychopathy (PCL-R score >24). Error bars represent 95% CIs. (c) Correlation between PCL-R scores (from offenders) and the required morphing grade for fear-morphs (left panel) and angry-morphs (right panel). (d) Correlation between BPAQ scores (from all participants) and the required morphing grade for fear-morphs (left panel) and angry-morphs (right panel). In (c) and (d), every circle represents a participant, the solid line the best-fitting linear regression line and the dashed lines the associated 95% confidence bands.

Supplementary material: File

Stein et al. supplementary material

Stein et al. supplementary material
Download Stein et al. supplementary material(File)
File 103 KB