Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-21T06:30:01.110Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Social threat, fronto-cingulate-limbic morphometry, and symptom course in depressed adolescents: a longitudinal investigation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2022

Amar Ojha
Affiliation:
Center for Neuroscience, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA Center for Neural Basis of Cognition, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
Giana I. Teresi
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA
George M. Slavich
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences, University of California, Los Angeles, CA, USA
Ian H. Gotlib
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA
Tiffany C. Ho*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Weill Institute for Neurosciences, University of California, San Francisco, CA, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Tiffany C. Ho, Ph.D., E-mail: tiffany.ho@psych.ucla.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Psychosocial stressors characterized by social threat, such as interpersonal loss and social rejection, are associated with depression in adolescents. Few studies, however, have examined whether social threat affects fronto-cingulate-limbic systems implicated in adolescent depression.

Methods

We assessed lifetime stressor severity across several domains using the Stress and Adversity Inventory (STRAIN) in 57 depressed adolescents (16.15 ± 1.32 years, 34 females), and examined whether the severity of social threat and non-social threat stressors was associated with gray matter volumes (GMVs) in the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), amygdala, hippocampus, and nucleus accumbens (NAcc). We also examined how lifetime social threat severity and GMVs in these regions related to depressive symptoms at baseline and over 9 months.

Results

General stressor severity was related to greater depression severity at baseline and over 9 months. Moreover, greater severity of social threat (but not non-social threat) stressors was associated with smaller bilateral amygdala and NAcc GMVs, and smaller bilateral surface areas of caudal and rostral ACC (all pFDR ⩽ 0.048). However, neither social threat nor non-social threat stressor severity was related to hippocampal GMVs (all pFDR ⩾ 0.318). All fronto-cingulate-limbic structures that were associated with the severity of social threat were negatively associated with greater depression severity over 9 months (all pFDR ⩽ 0.014). Post-hoc analyses suggested that gray matter morphometry of bilateral amygdala, NAcc, and rostral and caudal ACC mediated the association between social threat and depression severity in adolescents over 9 months (all pFDR < 0.048).

Conclusions

Social threat specifically affects fronto-cingulate-limbic pathways that contribute to the maintenance of depression in adolescents.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Depression is a leading cause of disability worldwide (World Health Organization, 2017), with nearly one in five people experiencing depression during their lifetime (Friedrich, Reference Friedrich2017). The lifetime prevalence of depression is especially alarming in adolescents, with a cumulative incidence of 13.6% in males and 36.1% in females by age 18 (Breslau et al., Reference Breslau, Gilman, Stein, Ruder, Gmelin and Miller2017). Moreover, individuals who develop depression during adolescence, as compared to those who develop depression later in life, tend to experience more severe and recurrent episodes of the disorder (Naicker, Galambos, Zeng, Senthilselvan, & Colman, Reference Naicker, Galambos, Zeng, Senthilselvan and Colman2013; Zisook et al., Reference Zisook, Rush, Lesser, Wisniewski, Trivedi, Husain and Fava2007). Viewed in this context, adolescence represents an important developmental period marked by heightened vulnerability for the onset and adverse consequences of depression. Therefore, it is critical to identify psychosocial and neurobiological mechanisms that contribute to depression in adolescents.

Social stressors and depression: the role of social threat

Although stressful life events often precede and increase risk for depressive episodes across the lifespan (Hammen, Reference Hammen2005, Reference Hammen2006), this effect is especially pronounced in adolescents (Lewinsohn, Allen, Seeley, & Gotlib, Reference Lewinsohn, Allen, Seeley and Gotlib1999; Monroe & Harkness, Reference Monroe and Harkness2005). This finding is not surprising given that adolescence is a period during which environmental demands have outsized effects on neurodevelopment (Ho, Reference Ho2019; Larsen & Luna, Reference Larsen and Luna2018), an effect that coincides with experiences of heightened stress. Importantly, exposure to psychosocial stressors characterized by social threat, including interpersonal loss and social rejection, has been found to be a strong proximal risk factor not only for the onset of depression in adolescence (Slavich, O'Donovan, Epel, & Kemeny, Reference Slavich, O'Donovan, Epel and Kemeny2010a), but also for the persistence and recurrence of depressive symptoms over time (Hammen, Reference Hammen2006, Reference Hammen2009). Specifically, experiences of social threat have been found to uniquely predict adolescent depression (Flynn & Rudolph, Reference Flynn and Rudolph2011; Vrshek-Schallhorn et al., Reference Vrshek-Schallhorn, Stroud, Mineka, Hammen, Zinbarg, Wolitzky-Taylor and Craske2015) and to be associated with heightened risk for the recurrence of depression in this age group (Sheets & Craighead, Reference Sheets and Craighead2014). Specific experiences of social threat that have been related to adolescent depression include peer rejection, bullying, victimization (Platt, Kadosh, & Lau, Reference Platt, Kadosh and Lau2013), poor-quality relationships, and relationship dissolutions (Mirsu-Paun & Oliver, Reference Mirsu-Paun and Oliver2017).

In addition to being a proximal risk factor for depression, experiences of early adversity – and specifically of social threat during early life – are also associated with adolescent-onset depression (LeMoult et al., Reference Lemoult, Humphreys, Tracy, Hoffmeister, Ip and Gotlib2020). Exposure to early adversity shapes responses to stressors during adolescence and throughout the lifespan (Claessens et al., Reference Claessens, Daskalakis, van Der Veen, Oitzl, de Kloet and Champagne2011). Therefore, it is important to consider cumulative experiences of stressors, including those that may be chronic and span different developmental periods, when investigating the impact of social threat on neural and behavioral outcomes. Few studies, however, have systematically assessed whether experiences of social threat are uniquely associated with neurophenotypes of depression in adolescents.

Cortical neural correlates of social threat and adolescent depression

We also have an incomplete understanding of how lifetime social threat severity may alter specific neurobiological pathways that underlie adolescent depression. Because interpersonal relationships are particularly important in adolescence, adolescents may be more sensitive to social cues than are children and adults, possibly through relevant neurobiological pathways (Blakemore & Mills, Reference Blakemore and Mills2014; Somerville, Jones, & Casey, Reference Somerville, Jones and Casey2010; Steinberg, Reference Steinberg2005). Indeed, several cortical and subcortical structures have been implicated in social stressors and depression (Slavich & Irwin, Reference Slavich and Irwin2014). The anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) – a broad region with multiple functions, including integrating sensory and emotional stimuli, computing value-based signals for action selection, and supporting effective conflict monitoring – is a critical neural substrate underlying the pathophysiology of adolescent depression (Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, Reference Botvinick, Cohen and Carter2004; Lichenstein, Verstynen, & Forbes, Reference Lichenstein, Verstynen and Forbes2016; Shenhav, Botvinick, & Cohen, Reference Shenhav, Botvinick and Cohen2013). Evidence from studies using structural and functional magnetic resonance imaging have consistently identified the rostral ACC (rACC) – a division of the ACC that receives a diversity of corticolimbic inputs and is involved in affective processing – as especially relevant to depression. Specifically, alterations in the morphology and function of the rACC have been associated with depressive phenotypes in youth (Boes, McCormick, Coryell, & Nopoulos, Reference Boes, McCormick, Coryell and Nopoulos2008; Ho et al., Reference Ho, Sacchet, Connolly, Margulies, Tymofiyeva, Paulus and Yang2017a). Researchers have found that rACC thickness predicted treatment response to transcranial magnetic stimulation in patients with medication-refractory depression predicts treatment response to repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (Boes et al., Reference Boes, Uitermarkt, Albazron, Lan, Liston, Pascual-Leone and Fox2018) as well as response to internet-delivered cognitive behavioral therapy (Webb et al., Reference Webb, Olson, Killgore, Pizzagalli, Rauch and Rosso2018). In addition, activation in the rACC to affective stimuli, as well as intrinsic functional connectivity between the rACC and other frontolimbic regions, have been shown to predict improvements in depression across psychosocial and pharmacological therapies in adolescents and young adults with depression (Davey, Cearns, Jamieson, & Harrison, Reference Davey, Cearns, Jamieson and Harrison2021; Jamieson, Harrison, Razi, & Davey, Reference Jamieson, Harrison, Razi and Davey2022). Together, these studies suggest that characteristics of the rACC as central to the experience of depression and may be a useful biomarker of treatment response.

Another line of research suggests that the ACC influences how the body and brain respond to socially stressful events. Investigators have found that the ACC is more active in response to social exclusion than inclusion, and that increased activation is related to greater self-reported distress (Eisenberger, Lieberman, & Williams, Reference Eisenberger, Lieberman and Williams2003). Separate work has found greater activation in the dorsal ACC during social exclusion (v. inclusion) to be associated with increased inflammatory reactivity, implicating the ACC and related circuitry as a pathway through which social stress may contribute to depressive phenotypes (Slavich, Way, Eisenberger, & Taylor, Reference Slavich, Way, Eisenberger and Taylor2010b). Increases in ACC activation also have been documented in response to social-evaluative threat (Eisenberger, Taylor, Gable, Hilmert, & Lieberman, Reference Eisenberger, Taylor, Gable, Hilmert and Lieberman2007; Wager et al., Reference Wager, van Ast, Hughes, Davidson, Lindquist and Ochsner2009). Further, increases in rACC activation have been reported to mediate cardiovascular responses to social-evaluative threat; specifically, threat caused increased activation in the rACC, which, in turn, was associated with increased heart rate (Wager et al., Reference Wager, van Ast, Hughes, Davidson, Lindquist and Ochsner2009). Consistent with this work, in a recent review, activation in the ACC was the most common fMRI response to social exclusion in studies examining this phenomenon (Wang, Braun, & Enck, Reference Wang, Braun and Enck2017). Finally, animal research corroborates human neuroimaging work: following a bilateral ACC lesion in rhesus monkeys, investigators observed a reduction in prosocial preferences (Basile, Schafroth, Karaskiewicz, Chang, & Murray, Reference Basile, Schafroth, Karaskiewicz, Chang and Murray2020), suggesting that ACC functioning is not only implicated in social processes, but may also be necessary for engagement in certain social behaviors.

Subcortical neural correlates of social threat and adolescent depression

Several subcortical regions, including the hippocampus, amygdala, and nucleus accumbens (NAcc), also have been implicated in stress processing, affective and motivated behaviors, and depression (Ho, Reference Ho, Teresi, Ojha, Walker, Kirshenbaum, Singh and Gotlib2021; Ho et al., Reference Ho, Gutman, Pozzi, Grabe, Hosten, Wittfeld and Schmaal2022; Tottenham & Galván, Reference Tottenham and Galván2016). The hippocampus is involved in learning and memory, and, as a target of stress hormones, is a core structure underlying the neural regulation of affect (Sheline, Liston, & McEwen, Reference Sheline, Liston and McEwen2019). Not surprisingly, depression is characterized in part by hippocampal dysfunction, with smaller hippocampal volumes being documented as among the most robust neurophenotypes of depression (Ho et al., Reference Ho, Gutman, Pozzi, Grabe, Hosten, Wittfeld and Schmaal2022; Schmaal et al., Reference Schmaal, Yücel, Ellis, Vijayakumar, Simmons, Allen and Whittle2017). In contrast, the amygdala detects salient emotional stimuli in the environment and, therefore, is highly sensitive to social-environmental stressors (Anderson & Phelps, Reference Anderson and Phelps2001; Cunningham & Brosch, Reference Cunningham and Brosch2012; van Marle, Hermans, Qin, & Fernández, Reference van Marle, Hermans, Qin and Fernández2009). Alterations in amygdala function and structure are common both in patients with depression and in individuals with histories of life stressor exposure, especially during sensitive periods of development (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Jing, Yang, Tottenham, Lee and Casey2013; Frodl et al., Reference Frodl, Meisenzahl, Zetzsche, Bottlender, Born, Groll and Möller2002; Siegle, Steinhauer, Thase, Stenger, & Carter, Reference Siegle, Steinhauer, Thase, Stenger and Carter2002). Researchers have found volumetric alterations in the amygdala in the context of stress: whereas acute stress has been associated with greater basolateral amygdala spine density, chronic stress has been related to dendritic growth in this region in animal models (Roozendaal, McEwen, & Chattarji, Reference Roozendaal, McEwen and Chattarji2009). Conversely, lower perceived stress, measured using a self-report questionnaire following a mindfulness-based stress reduction intervention, was associated with reductions in gray matter density in the right basolateral amygdala (Hölzel et al., Reference Hölzel, Carmody, Evans, Hoge, Dusek, Morgan and Lazar2010).

Finally, the NAcc – part of the ventral striatum – receives dopaminergic inputs from the ventral tegmental area and is a core component of reward-learning and motivated behavior circuitry (Knutson, Adams, Fong, & Hommer, Reference Knutson, Adams, Fong and Hommer2001; Salamone, Correa, Mingote, & Weber, Reference Salamone, Correa, Mingote and Weber2005). In depression, reductions in NAcc activation have been found to be associated with symptoms of anhedonia (Wacker, Dillon, & Pizzagalli, Reference Wacker, Dillon and Pizzagalli2009); further, researchers have found smaller NAcc gray matter volume (GMV) in depressed individuals than in healthy controls (Zhang et al., Reference Zhang, Yang, Zhu, Zhu, Jia, Zhang and Wang2021). Recent research has posited that life stress drives symptoms of anhedonia in depression via reward pathways involving the NAcc (Pizzagalli, Reference Pizzagalli2014), which may be especially relevant for adolescent depression given the heightened sensitivity to reward during this developmental period (Auerbach, Admon, & Pizzagalli, Reference Auerbach, Admon and Pizzagalli2014; Galván, Reference Galván2010). Indeed, Lee et al. (Reference Lee, Yoo, Lee, Kim, Han, Hong and Brent2020) recently found that reduced NAcc volumes serve as an indirect pathway by which interpersonal stressors (e.g. peer conflict) lead to adolescent depression.

Goals of the present study

The research described above details several cortical and subcortical structures that are affected by stress, that undergo significant maturation during adolescence, and that are implicated in depression during this important developmental period. Although converging evidence suggests that these regions are susceptible to the effects of social stressors, the extent to which the morphology of these regions is associated with the severity of depression in adolescents is not clear.

To address this gap in our knowledge, we investigated the extent to which gray matter morphometry of fronto-cingulate-limbic regions are susceptible to stressors characterized by social threat severity over the life course and, further, the extent to which these regions are related to the severity of depression in adolescents with depressive disorders. Fifty-seven depressed adolescents were assessed at baseline and provided up to five reports of depression severity over the course of 9 months. We examined how lifetime exposure to different types of social stressors assessed by the Stress and Adversity Inventory for Adolescents (Adolescent STRAIN; Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields, & Auerbach, Reference Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields and Auerbach2019) was related to the morphometry of cortical and subcortical brain structures. We focused on the perceived severity of stressors occurring over the lifespan given evidence that perceived stress severity affects neurobiological development above and beyond the number of life events experienced (Ho et al., Reference Ho, King, Leong, Colich, Humphreys, Ordaz and Gotlib2017b; Ho & King, Reference Ho and King2021). Consistent with prior research, we hypothesized that greater cumulative severity of (i.e. experiencing more severe) social threat over the life course would be associated with greater depressive symptom severity at baseline and over the 9-month study period. We also hypothesized that experiencing more lifetime social threat severity would be related to lower cortical thickness and surface areas of rostral and caudal anterior cingulate cortex (rACC, cACC, respectively) and to smaller hippocampal, amygdalar, and NAcc GMVs. To further interrogate these findings, we conducted specificity analyses to test for lateralization effects for all significant associations with lifetime social threat severity and, in addition, tested whether these findings were specific to lifetime social threat severity or were associated with non-social threat severity as well. Finally, we tested whether fronto-cingulate-limbic morphology sensitive to lifetime social threat severity mediated the association between lifetime social threat severity and depressive symptom severity longitudinally over 9 months.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-six depressed adolescents between 13 and 18 years old were recruited from the San Francisco Bay Area community as part of a longitudinal study examining neurobiological mechanisms underlying adolescent stress and depression (see Walker et al., Reference Walker, Teresi, Weisenburger, Segarra, Ojha, Kulla and Ho2020, for a description of the study protocol). In accord with the Declaration of Helsinki, all study components were approved by the Institutional Review Boards (IRB) at Stanford University and the University of California, San Francisco.

Inclusion criteria at the baseline visit of the study included fluency in English and the presence of current threshold or subthreshold major depressive disorder (MDD), dysthymia, or depressive disorder not otherwise specified via child or parent report according to DSM-IV criteria as assessed with the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia – Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL; see Clinical assessments, below). See online Supplementary material for more information on inclusion/exclusion criteria. Participants were included in the present study if they completed the Adolescent STRAIN (see below) and provided usable structural MRI data (see below), resulting in a final sample of 57 participants.

Procedure

The present study is part of an ongoing three-wave longitudinal study (Walker et al., Reference Walker, Teresi, Weisenburger, Segarra, Ojha, Kulla and Ho2020). Eligible participants (based on a phone screen) were invited to participate in a behavioral session (T1V1) in which adolescents and their parent/legal guardian completed diagnostic interviews and self-report questionnaires. Eligibility was confirmed following completion of the diagnostic interviews (see Clinical assessments, below); those who met criteria for study participation (see online Supplementary material) were invited to participate in the remainder of the study procedure, including an MRI scan (T1V2; session interval: 10.75 ± 5.83 days; see MRI scanning acquisition, below). Following the MRI scan, participants were invited to complete self-report questionnaires about their symptoms of depression every other month from home (e.g. M3, M5, M7, etc.) in addition to a behavioral follow-up assessment at 9 months (T2). For the present study, we included participants who provided follow-up data up to T2 (as of 1 July 2021), for longitudinal analyses for a maximum of five assessments per participant (for additional protocol details, see Walker et al., Reference Walker, Teresi, Weisenburger, Segarra, Ojha, Kulla and Ho2020). See online Supplementary Fig. S1 for details on the study timeline, sample sizes, and intervals per follow-up assessment for the present investigation.

Clinical assessments

Trained research assistants administered the K-SADS-PL (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Rao, Flynn, Moreci and Ryan1997; Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Ryan, & Rao, Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Brent, Ryan and Rao2000) and the Children's Depressive Rating Scale – Revised (CDRS-R; Poznanski & Mokros, Reference Poznanski and Mokros1996) to adolescents and their parent/legal guardian at the initial behavioral session to assess eligibility.

Lifetime stressor exposure

The Adolescent STRAIN is an online, interview-based system for assessing cumulative lifetime exposure to stressors in adolescents ages 10–18 years (Slavich et al., Reference Slavich, Stewart, Esposito, Shields and Auerbach2019; see https://www.strainsetup.com/). For the purposes of this study, we focused on the total severity of social threat that occurred across the life course. This variable was computed by summing the severity scores for all of the acute and chronic stressors endorsed that involved interpersonal loss or humiliation. To test the specificity of these effects, we also computed a non-social threat severity score by subtracting each participant's lifetime social threat severity score from their total lifetime stressor severity score. The Adolescent STRAIN was administered to participants online within 3 weeks of their initial behavioral session (T1V1). The instrument has excellent psychometric properties and has been validated against psychological, behavioral, and clinical outcomes (Cazassa, Oliveira, Spahr, Shields, & Slavich, Reference Cazassa, Oliveira, Spahr, Shields and Slavich2020; Slavich & Shields, Reference Slavich and Shields2018; Sturmbauer, Shields, Hetzel, Rohleder, & Slavich, Reference Sturmbauer, Shields, Hetzel, Rohleder and Slavich2019). Because our goal was to empirically test associations between stressor exposure and behavioral and neurobiological outcomes described in life stress models of depression, we focused on the total severity of social threat that occurred across the entire life course.

Depression and anxiety symptom severity

Self-reported severity of depressive symptoms was assessed using the Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale (RADS-2; Reynolds, Reference Reynolds2010), a 30-item questionnaire designed to assess the severity of depressive symptoms in adolescents ages 11–20 years and that has been shown to have excellent validity and reliability (Reynolds, Reference Reynolds2010). RADS-2 items are rated on a four-point Likert scale from 1 (Almost never) to 4 (Most of the time), with total scores ranging from 30 to 120. The RADS-2 was administered to participants at all baseline and follow-up assessments. Cronbach's α was high at each assessment (T1 = 0.88, M3 = 0.91, M5 = 0.93, M7 = 0.91, T2 = 0.91; overall = 0.91).

Self-reported severity of anxiety symptoms was assessed using the Multidimensional Scale for Anxiety for Children (MASC-2; March, Reference March2012), a 39-item questionnaire designed to assess severity of anxiety symptoms in youth ages 8–19 years and that has been shown to have excellent validity and reliability (March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings, & Conners, Reference March, Parker, Sullivan, Stallings and Conners1997; Wei et al., Reference Wei, Hoff, Villabø, Peterman, Kendall and Piacentini2014). MASC-2 items are rated on a four-point Likert scale from 0 (Never true about me) to 3 (Often true about me), with total scores ranging from 0 to 117. The MASC-2 was administered to participants at all baseline and follow-up assessments. Cronbach's α was high at each assessment (T1 = 0.92, M3 = 0.92, M5 = 0.92, M7 = 0.93, T2 = 0.93; overall = 0.92).

MRI scanning acquisition

All MRI scans were acquired at the Stanford Center for Cognitive and Neurobiological Imaging (CNI) with a 3 T MRI scanner (General Electric Healthcare Systems) and 32-channel head coil (Nova Medical Inc., Wilmington, MA, USA). Forty-six participants were scanned on a Discovery MR750 and 11 participants were scanned on the SIGNA Ultra High Performance after COVID-19 mitigation procedures were put into place. Therefore, in all statistical analyses, we also included session type (pre-COVID, post-COVID) as a dichotomous covariate.

MRI quality control

MRI scans were visually inspected for motion artifacts before tissue segmentation and surface-based parcellation using FreeSurfer 6.0, as previously described (Ho et al., Reference Ho, Cichocki, Gifuni, Catalina Camacho, Ordaz, Singh and Gotlib2018, Reference Ho, Teresi, Ojha, Walker, Kirshenbaum, Singh and Gotlib2021). See Fig. 1 for representative segmentations of regions of interest. Bilateral GMVs for each structure were calculated by averaging across the two hemispheres. Given that we did not have hypotheses regarding laterality in the limbic structures of interest, we used bilateral regions of interest (ROIs) for these analyses. We followed up all analyses in which significant effects were identified for a given limbic ROI by conducting post-hoc analyses examining possible laterality effects (see next section for more details).

Fig. 1. Illustration of cortical and subcortical ROIs in standard space (for visualization purposes): rostral anterior cingulate cortex (dark purple), caudal anterior cingulate cortex (dark blue); nucleus accumbens (red), amygdala (green), hippocampus (teal). Segmentation of and estimation of gray matter for each ROI was performed within each individual. ROI, region of interest.

Statistical analyses

Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among study variables

Correlations between the main study variables and covariates were tested using Pearson's correlations. Student's t tests and χ2 tests were used to determine if the nine participants excluded from final analyses differed from those included in our final analytic sample.

Social threat and depression severity

We used linear regressions to test whether lifetime social threat severity was associated with RADS-2 scores cross-sectionally, and linear mixed effects (LME) modeling to test whether higher levels of lifetime social threat severity assessed at baseline predicted greater depression severity over 9 months (Kuznetsova, Brockhoff, & Christensen, Reference Kuznetsova, Brockhoff and Christensen2017). Covariates for these cross-sectional analyses included age and a dichotomous variable for antidepressant medication use at time of assessment, along with session type (for models that included morphometry metrics, as there was a hardware scanner upgrade post-COVID). All LME models included a subject-specific random intercept, a random slope based on time since baseline (T1V1) in years, and age at baseline, time-varying antidepressant medication use as a dichotomous variable, time since baseline in years, and a dichotomous variable for session type (where appropriate) as covariates. We also present results with sex as a covariate. Predictor and outcome variables were z-scored for all longitudinal statistical models.

Social threat and fronto-cingulate-limbic brain morphometry

We also used multiple linear regressions to investigate whether lifetime social threat severity was associated with subcortical GMV and cortical thickness and surface area for our regions of interest. Covariates for these cross-sectional analyses included age and a dichotomous variable for antidepressant medication use at time of assessment, along with session type (pre-COVID, post-COVID). We also present results with sex as an additional covariate. For all significant associations, in post-hoc analyses, we tested the two hemispheres separately to investigate whether there was lateralization specificity.

Specificity of the effects of social threat

To test the specificity of associations to lifetime social threat severity, we tested whether depression, as well as subcortical and cortical morphology, was associated with non-social threat severity.

Exploratory longitudinal mediation analyses

We conducted exploratory longitudinal analyses using fixed-effects modeling (for the path between lifetime social threat severity and brain morphometry metrics) and LMEs (for the paths predicting RADS-2 scores) to test whether fronto-cingulate-limbic structures statistically mediated the association between lifetime social threat severity and depressive symptom severity over 9 months. Specifically, for each brain metric, we used Monte Carlo simulations to estimate the 95% confidence intervals (CI) of the indirect effect of GMV on the association between lifetime social threat severity and RADS-2 scores.

Specificity of the effects to depression

To test whether these patterns were specific to depression, we also tested whether lifetime social threat and non-social threat severity, as well as subcortical and cortical morphology, were associated with anxiety as assessed with MASC-2 scores.

Exploratory whole-brain analyses

Although we took a hypothesis-driven approach to the present study, we also conducted a whole-brain surface-based voxel-wise analysis to complement our primary analyses and test whether other cortical regions were associated with lifetime social threat severity. See online Supplementary material for methodological details.

Statistical approach

We report the fixed-effect parameters of all cross-sectional and longitudinal statistical models (Lüdecke, Ben-Shachar, Patil, & Makowski, Reference Lüdecke, Ben-Shachar, Patil and Makowski2020). Given concerns about degrees of freedom, we included sex assigned at birth as an additional covariate in post-hoc sensitivity analyses. All reported β values are standardized. Finally, each of our statistical models met assumptions for linear modeling (i.e. normality of residuals, homoscedasticity). Significance was set at p < 0.05 with false discovery rate (FDR) correction (α = 0.05). All statistical analyses were conducted in R v. 4.0.3 (R Core Team, 2020). See online Supplementary material for methodological details.

Results

Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations among study variables

Demographic and clinical characteristics at baseline for the 57 participants are presented in Table 1. These participants did not differ on any demographic or clinical characteristics from the nine participants who were excluded due to missing data (all ps > 0.15). A correlation matrix of cross-sectional associations between the main variables of interest is presented in online Supplementary Fig. S2.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of participant demographic, clinical, and neural characteristics for the 57 participants with Adolescent STRAIN and structural fMRI data at baseline

Note: All continuous values are reported as mean ± s.d. (min–max). Numbers in brackets [ ] indicate the number of missing or unusable responses. Categorical variables are reported as percentage (count). Lifetime comorbidities (includes past and current reports integrated across parent and child interviews) are reported as percentage (count). Abbreviations: V1, visit 1; V2, visit 2; LGBTQ+, lesbian, gay, bisexual, transgender, queer; RADS-2, Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale, 2nd Edition; CDRS-R, Children's Depression Rating Scale–Revised; ICV, intracranial volume; R, right; L, left; GMV, gray matter volume; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; ACC, anterior cingulate cortex.

a Other medications taken by participants, with () indicating count, include gabapentin (1), lamotrigine (3), trazodone (4), prazosin (1), buspar (1), dextromethorphan (1), and cannabidiol (1).

b Therapy indicates the percentage (count) of participants who reported attending therapy sessions for their depression in the 2 months prior to their first visit.

c Lifetime comorbidity disorder groupings, with () indicating count for individual disorders: anxiety disorders: panic disorder (3), social phobia (12), simple phobia (5), agoraphobia (2), generalized anxiety disorder (21); eating disorders: anorexia nervosa (3), bulimia nervosa (0), eating disorder not otherwise specified (3); disruptive, impulse control, and conduct disorders: oppositional defiant disorder (2), conduct disorder (2); other disorders: autism spectrum disorder (1), disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (1).

Social threat and depression severity

Higher reported lifetime social threat severity was associated with greater depression severity both cross-sectionally at baseline (β = 0.37, p FDR = 0.005) and longitudinally over 9 months (β = 0.28, p FDR = 0.027). See Table 2 and Fig. 2 for more details. These results were significant at baseline when including sex as a covariate (β = 0.35, p = 0.015, uncorrected) but not longitudinally (β = 0.22, p = 0.065, uncorrected). See online Supplementary Tables S1A and S1B. Including sex as a covariate did not significantly improve model fit. See online Supplementary Tables S2A and S2B.

Fig. 2. Summary of primary results. Scatterplot and fitted regression line illustrating the association between (a) lifetime social threat severity and depression severity, (b) lifetime social threat severity and gray matter volumes of bilateral NAcc, (c) lifetime social threat severity and depression severity over the course of the study (intraindividual average RADS-2 score across all timepoints), and (d) gray matter volumes of bilateral NAcc and depression severity over the course of the study (intraindividual average RADS-2 score across all timepoints). NAcc, nucleus accumbens; RADS-2, Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale, 2nd Edition; T1, baseline.

Table 2. Fixed effects estimated from linear models testing for associations between social and non-social threat severity with depression symptoms at baseline (cross-sectional) and across nine months (longitudinal)

For the longitudinal model, fixed-effects are estimated from a linear mixed-effects model that includes random intercepts and slopes. β refers to the standardized partial regression coefficients. s.e., standard error; CI, confidence interval; FDR, false discovery rate. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Social threat and fronto-cingulate-limbic brain morphometry

Higher reported lifetime social threat severity was associated with smaller bilateral amygdala (β = −0.39, p FDR = 0.020) and NAcc GMVs (β = −0.35, p FDR = 0.020) but not with hippocampal GMV (β = −0.15, p FDR = 0.318). Lifetime social threat severity was not associated with either rACC or cACC cortical thickness (all p FDR > 0.475). In contrast, higher reported social threat severity was associated with smaller cACC and rACC surface areas (cACC: β = −0.34, p FDR = 0.048; rACC: β = −0.33, p FDR = 0.048). See Tables 3 and 4.

Table 3. Fixed effects estimated from linear models testing associations between social threat severity with subcortical gray matter volumes at baseline

Post-hoc lateralization analyses were only performed for structures exhibiting a significant bilateral effect. β refers to the standardized partial regression coefficients. CI, confidence interval; FDR, false discovery rate; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; s.e., standard error. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Table 4. Fixed effects estimated from linear models testing associations between social threat severity with cortical thickness and surface area at baseline

Post-hoc lateralization analyses were only performed for structures exhibiting a significant bilateral effect. β refers to the standardized partial regression coefficients. cACC, caudal anterior cingulate cortex; CI, confidence interval; FDR, false discovery rate; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; s.e., standard error. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

When including sex as a covariate, higher reported lifetime social threat remained significantly associated with both smaller amygdala and NAcc GMVs (amygdala: β = −0.29, p = 0.038, uncorrected; NAcc: β = −0.30, p = 0.036, uncorrected); however, neither rACC nor cACC surface area was associated with lifetime social threat severity after covarying for sex (all ps ⩾ 0.074, uncorrected). See online Supplementary Table S1C. Including sex as a covariate significantly improved model fit for associations between lifetime social threat severity and amygdala GMV, rACC surface area, and cACC surface area. See online Supplementary Table S2C.

Specificity of effects to social threat

We tested whether the associations described above were specific to lifetime social threat severity. As hypothesized, greater self-reported non-social threat severity (total lifetime stressor severity – total lifetime social threat severity) was associated with greater depression severity cross-sectionally (β = 0.45, p FDR = 0.002) and over 9 months (β = 0.27, p FDR = 0.027). See Table 2. Including sex as a covariate did not change the significance of the effect of non-social threat severity on RADS-2 at baseline (p = 0.005, uncorrected) but did attenuate the significance of the effect on RADS-2 longitudinally (p = 0.062, uncorrected). A summary of these analyses can be found in online Supplementary Tables S1A and S1B. Including sex as a covariate did not improve model fit. See online Supplementary Tables S2A and S2B. Conversely, as hypothesized, lifetime non-social threat severity was not associated with hippocampal, amygdalar, or NAcc GMV (all p FDR ⩾ 0.218) or with cACC or rACC surface areas or cortical thickness (all p FDR ⩾ 0.120), indicating that social threat severity specifically affects gray matter morphometry of these regions. Including sex as a covariate did not change the lack of a significant association for any of these effects (all ps ⩾ 0.109). See online Supplementary Table S1D. Including sex as a covariate significantly improved model fit for associations between lifetime non-social threat severity and amygdala GMV, cACC surface area, and rACC surface area. See online Supplementary Table S2D.

Exploratory longitudinal mediation analyses

We conducted exploratory longitudinal analyses to examine whether the subcortical and cortical regions that were significantly associated with lifetime social threat severity also predicted depressive symptom severity over 9 months. More specifically, we constructed LME models testing the associations between baseline rACC and cACC surface areas, NAcc and amygdala GMVs, and depression symptoms over time. All structures were negatively associated with depression symptoms over 9 months (all βs ⩽ −0.28, all p FDR < 0.014). See Table 5 for details. After including sex as an additional covariate, each of these structures were still significantly associated with depression symptoms at baseline (all ps < 0.018, uncorrected), with only NAcc GMVs and cACC surface areas associated with depression symptoms longitudinally (NAcc: β = −0.24, p = 0.037, uncorrected; cACC: β = −0.23, p = 0.045, uncorrected). See online Supplementary Tables S1E and S1F. Including sex as a covariate significantly improved model fit for associations between amygdala GMV, cACC surface area, and rACC surface area and RADS-2 at baseline but did not improve model fit for any of these models with RADS-2 longitudinally. See online Supplementary Tables S2B and S2C.

Table 5. Fixed effects estimated from linear mixed-effects models testing associations between rACC surface area, cACC surface area, NAcc GMV, and amygdala GMV with depressive symptoms (RADS-2 scores) longitudinally over 9 months

Post-hoc lateralization analyses were only performed for structures exhibiting a significant bilateral effect. β refers to the standardized partial regression coefficients. cACC, caudal anterior cingulate cortex; CI, confidence interval; FDR, false discovery rate; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; s.e., standard error. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

As a follow-up to these longitudinal analyses, we conducted post-hoc analyses to test whether the regions that demonstrated significant associations with lifetime social threat severity – bilateral baseline amygdala GMV, cACC surface area, NAcc GMV, rACC surface area – statistically mediated the association between lifetime social threat severity and depression symptom severity over 9 months. All of the indirect effects tested were significant [amygdala: β = 0.11, 95% CI (0.02–0.25); cACC: β = 0.10, 95% CI (0.008–0.23); NAcc: β = 0.10, 95% CI (0.01–0.23); rACC: β = 0.09, 95% CI (0.006–0.217)]. See Fig. 3 for more details.

Fig. 3. Summary of exploratory mediation analyses. Mediation model schematic illustrating associations between lifetime social threat severity, depression severity over the course of the study (intraindividual average RADS-2 score across all timepoints), and the indirect effect of (a) rACC SA, (b) cACC SA, (c) amygdala GMV, and (d) NAcc GMV. rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; cACC, caudal anterior cingulate cortex; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; SA, surface area; GMV, gray matter volume; ST, lifetime social threat severity.

Specificity of effects to depression

Next, we tested whether the associations described above were specific to depression severity by examining associations with self-reported anxiety (MASC-2). We found that the effect of lifetime social threat severity on MASC-2 scores at baseline and over 9 months was similar to that of RADS-2 (baseline: β = 0.43, p = 0.002, uncorrected; longitudinal: β = 0.37, p = 0.002, uncorrected) and, in addition, that these effects were specific to lifetime social threat severity in that all associations with non-social threat severity were non-significant (ps > 0.214). See online Supplementary Tables S3A and S3B.

Amygdala and NAcc GMVs were significantly correlated with MASC-2 scores at baseline (amygdala: βs = −0.27, p = 0.033, uncorrected; NAcc: β = −0.39, p = 0.002, uncorrected). See online Supplementary Table S3C. Similarly, amygdala and NAcc GMVs, as well as cACC surface area, were significantly correlated with MASC-2 scores over 9 months (all ps < 0.044, uncorrected). See online Supplementary Table S3D for more details.

In addition, we found that only NAcc and amygdala GMVs statistically mediated the association between lifetime social threat severity and MASC-2 scores over 9 months. See online Supplementary Table S3E for more details.

Exploratory whole-brain analyses

Finally, we conducted exploratory whole-brain analyses to further interrogate these data. We found that greater lifetime social threat severity was associated with lower cortical surface areas in the left fusiform gyrus, left inferior parietal region, left postcentral gyrus, and right superior frontal surface area, lower cortical volumes in the left fusiform gyrus, and left inferior parietal region. Consistent with our original analyses, cortical thickness was not associated with lifetime social threat severity. For more details, see online Supplementary Table S4 and Fig. S3.

Discussion

Although separate lines of evidence have implicated exposure to major life stressors in the onset and maintenance of depression in adolescents, it is unclear how various types of stressors occurring over the life course affect specific aspects of neural morphology to influence the pathophysiology of adolescent depression. We investigated this issue in the present study by examining how the perceived severity of social stressors occurring over the lifespan was associated with gray matter morphometry in the ACC, amygdala, hippocampus, and NAcc. We also investigated whether the lifetime social threat severity and GMVs in these fronto-cingulate-limbic regions were associated with severity of depression symptoms at baseline and their persistence over 9 months. As hypothesized, greater lifetime social threat severity was associated with more depression severity at baseline and over the course of 9 months. Also consistent with our hypotheses, greater lifetime social threat severity was associated with smaller bilateral amygdala and NAcc GMV. Surprisingly, neither lifetime social nor non-social threat severity was related to hippocampal GMV.

Lifetime social threat severity was not related to cACC or rACC cortical thickness but higher reported lifetime social threat severity was associated with smaller bilateral surface areas in both subregions. As hypothesized, NAcc and amygdala GMVs and cACC and rACC surface areas were all associated with lifetime social threat severity but were not related to non-social threat severity, suggesting that these regions are differentially sensitive to social threat severity. Finally, all fronto-cingulate-limbic structures that were associated with lifetime social threat severity were also related to severity of depression over the course of 9 months.

These findings are generally consistent with prior cross-sectional research examining associations among NAcc GMV, social stressors, and depression in adolescents. For example, a recent cross-sectional study of 78 depressed adolescents and 47 healthy controls found that NAcc GMV mediated the association between peer problems (e.g. victimization and/or bullying) – one common form of social stress experienced during adolescence – and adolescent depression (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Yoo, Lee, Kim, Han, Hong and Brent2020). Consistent with our findings regarding the specificity of the NAcc in linking social stressors to depression in adolescents, Lee et al. did not find such associations with amygdala or hippocampal GMVs. Notably, however, they reported larger NAcc GMV in the depressed adolescent group, which they interpreted as reflecting heightened sensitivity to adverse peer relationships. These discrepant findings between our study and Lee et al.'s investigation may be explained in part by the fact that Lee et al. operationalized ‘peer problems’ as the sum of peer victimization and bullying behaviors, obscuring both whether these findings were driven by the victims or the aggressors of adverse peer relationships, and the role of NAcc in the context of social stress and adolescent depression.

Although we are unable to make mechanistic inferences due to the correlational nature of our study, it is possible that the observation of smaller NAcc GMV as a function of lifetime social threat severity may also be due to the fact that our sample (mean age: 16.15 years) was older than Lee et al.'s sample (mean age: 14 years), suggesting that there is a loss in NAcc GMV experienced across adolescence that may reflect the downstream effects of stressor exposure, including inflammatory processes, on brain-derived neurotropic factors and neuronal integrity (Felger and Treadway, Reference Felger and Treadway2017) (for a more detailed overview of these processes, see Walker et al., Reference Walker, Teresi, Weisenburger, Segarra, Ojha, Kulla and Ho2020). Although they did not examine social threat severity specifically, Auerbach and colleagues (Reference Auerbach, Pagliaccio, Hubbard, Frosch, Kremens, Cosby and Pizzagalli2022) recently found smaller NAcc GMVs in adolescents with depression/anxiety relative to healthy controls and, furthermore, found that smaller NAcc GMVs predicted depressive symptoms over 6 months. Similarly, in a study using the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development (ABCD) study dataset (N = 11 876), investigators compared subcortical GMVs in 9- and 10-year-old children who were at high or low risk for depression based on parental history of depression and found that high-risk children had smaller right putamen and NAcc GMVs than did low-risk children, which was driven primarily by the effects of a maternal history of depression on striatal structures in the right hemisphere (Pagliaccio, Alqueza, Marsh, & Auerbach, Reference Pagliaccio, Alqueza, Marsh and Auerbach2020). In addition, in a prospective longitudinal study, smaller NAcc GMVs in girls ages 12–16 years predicted a diagnosis of MDD by age 18 (Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Lichter, Dennison, Vijayakumar, Schwartz, Byrne and Allen2014). Although we are unable to definitively demonstrate that stressor exposure leads to insufficient growth or sustained neuronal loss (as observed by reductions in gray matter morphometry as measured by MRI) as opposed to delayed development, our findings, while viewed in the context of these other studies, suggest that smaller NAcc volumes are already present prior to the onset of a depressive episode possibly due to stressor exposure, and that these smaller NAcc volumes persist throughout adolescence and may contribute to the maintenance of depression as well as, potentially, anxiety disorders.

Other studies have also reported associations between depression and gray matter in mesial temporal structures, with more nuanced associations between stress severity and these structures. For instance, Rosso et al. (Reference Rosso, Cintron, Steingard, Renshaw, Young and Yurgelun-Todd2005) reported smaller bilateral amygdalar GMVs in children with depression compared to healthy controls but no differences in hippocampal volumes. Similar to Pagliaccio et al.'s (Reference Pagliaccio, Alqueza, Marsh and Auerbach2020) findings described above, Saleh et al. (Reference Saleh, Carballedo, Lisiecka, Fagan, Connolly, Boyle and Frodl2012) found in an adult cohort (18–65 years of age) that familial risk for depression was associated with smaller amygdalar GMVs. The effect of adolescent depression on the hippocampus, however, remains inconsistent. Several researchers have found smaller hippocampal GMVs (and hippocampal subfields) in adolescents with depression as compared to healthy controls (e.g. MacMaster et al., Reference MacMaster, Mirza, Szeszko, Kmiecik, Easter, Taormina and Rosenberg2008; MacMaster & Kusumakar, Reference MacMaster and Kusumakar2004; Whittle et al., Reference Whittle, Lichter, Dennison, Vijayakumar, Schwartz, Byrne and Allen2014), as well as in adults with adolescent-onset depression (Ho et al., Reference Ho, Gutman, Pozzi, Grabe, Hosten, Wittfeld and Schmaal2022); however, others have not found these differences (e.g. Pannekoek et al., Reference Pannekoek, van der Werff, van den Bulk, van Lang, Rombouts, van Buchem and van der Wee2014; Rosso et al., Reference Rosso, Cintron, Steingard, Renshaw, Young and Yurgelun-Todd2005). In our study, lifetime stressor severity was not associated with hippocampal GMVs in depressed adolescents. It may be that the timing of stress affects hippocampal growth; indeed, there is evidence that stress exposure during childhood (prior to age 6) has a disproportionate impact on adolescent hippocampal volumes (Humphreys et al., Reference Humphreys, King, Sacchet, Camacho, Colich, Ordaz and Gotlib2019). As recent reviews have highlighted (Ho & King, Reference Ho and King2021; Slavich, Reference Slavich2019; Teicher, Samson, Anderson, & Ohashi, Reference Teicher, Samson, Anderson and Ohashi2016), stressor exposure timing as well as stressor type need to be considered when attempting to elucidate the effects by which life stressor exposure influences brain regions implicated in adolescent depression.

With respect to cortical structures, human neuroimaging research has demonstrated that ACC morphology, and specifically smaller surface area of the ACC, is a robust neurophenotype of adolescent depression (e.g. Pannekoek et al., Reference Pannekoek, van der Werff, van den Bulk, van Lang, Rombouts, van Buchem and van der Wee2014; Schmaal et al., Reference Schmaal, Yücel, Ellis, Vijayakumar, Simmons, Allen and Whittle2017). Although smaller rACC volumes have been found to be associated with depressive symptoms, including in subclinical children (e.g. Boes et al., Reference Boes, McCormick, Coryell and Nopoulos2008), most research has not considered cortical morphology in the context of specific types of stress (e.g. social threat) in adolescent depression. Our study advances this area of research by demonstrating that lifetime social threat severity in depressed adolescents is associated with smaller surface area in rACC and cACC. Moreover, our findings that rACC and cACC surface areas are significant mediators of associations between stressors and depression symptoms – but not anxiety symptoms – suggest there may be specificity with respect to cortical structures representing a pathway by which experiences of social threat severity lead to depression. Broadly, our findings are also consistent with previous studies demonstrating that morphological characteristics of the ACC represent biomarkers of depression that are predictive of treatment response (Boes et al., Reference Boes, Uitermarkt, Albazron, Lan, Liston, Pascual-Leone and Fox2018; Davey et al., Reference Davey, Cearns, Jamieson and Harrison2021; Jamieson et al., Reference Jamieson, Harrison, Razi and Davey2022; Webb et al., Reference Webb, Olson, Killgore, Pizzagalli, Rauch and Rosso2018).

Although rACC volumes have been found to be negatively associated with depression, including in children with subclinical symptoms (e.g. Boes et al., Reference Boes, McCormick, Coryell and Nopoulos2008), most research has not considered finer-grained aspects of cortical morphology in the context of adolescent stress and depression. This is critical, given that changes in volumes could be due to changes in cortical thickness or in surface area, or both. Whereas cortical thickness likely reflects dendritic arborization and pruning and has been shown to decrease linearly with age, surface area reflects cortical folding and gyrification and has a curvilinear association with age, with maximum area during adolescence and young adulthood (Storsve et al., Reference Storsve, Fjell, Tamnes, Westlye, Overbye, Aasland and Walhovd2014; Wierenga, Langen, Oranje, & Durston, Reference Wierenga, Langen, Oranje and Durston2014). Interestingly, cortical thickness and cortical surface area have been found to have distinct genetic bases (Panizzon et al., Reference Panizzon, Fennema-Notestine, Eyler, Jernigan, Prom-Wormley, Neale and Kremen2009), with the latter area exhibiting greater genetic heritability as well as stronger effects from early environmental factors (Fjell et al., Reference Fjell, Chen, Sederevicius, Sneve, Grydeland, Krogsrud and Walhovd2019). Researchers have speculated that lower surface area could therefore reflect pre-existing risk for depression in youth (Schmaal Reference Schmaal2019). Smaller ACC surface area specifically appears to be a neurophenotype of adolescent depression (e.g. Pannekoek et al., Reference Pannekoek, van der Werff, van den Bulk, van Lang, Rombouts, van Buchem and van der Wee2014; Schmaal et al., Reference Schmaal, Yücel, Ellis, Vijayakumar, Simmons, Allen and Whittle2017) but it is unclear if or how stressors may influence this. Our study advances this area of research by demonstrating that lifetime social threat severity in depressed adolescents is associated with smaller surface area in rACC and cACC.

Our findings should be interpreted in light of several study limitations. First, the sample size was modest, which may have limited our statistical power to find significant effects that we observed as non-significant. Second, given the disruption in in-person data collection due to the COVID-19 pandemic, we were only able to examine brain morphometry at a single timepoint, thereby limiting our ability to make inferences regarding the extent to which life stressors influence neurodevelopment of these structures and their sustained contributions to the severity and persistence of depressive symptoms. This is especially relevant for understanding the role of the NAcc as a potential mediator of the association between social stressors and depression, given evidence that the NAcc (as well as other striatal structures) continues to mature throughout adolescence (e.g. Ernst et al., Reference Ernst, Nelson, Jazbec, McClure, Monk, Leibenluft and Pine2005; Herting et al., Reference Herting, Johnson, Mills, Vijayakumar, Dennison, Liu and Tamnes2018; Parr et al., Reference Parr, Calabro, Larsen, Tervo-Clemmens, Elliot, Foran and Luna2021) and is especially sensitive to social stimuli (both positive and negative) during this period of development (Foulkes & Blakemore, Reference Foulkes and Blakemore2016). Third, for ethical reasons, we did not manipulate exposure to life stressors in these participants; consequently, the findings are correlational in nature, preventing us from drawing causal inferences. Indeed, subcortical regions (e.g. NAcc, amygdala) may already be smaller in individuals at familial risk for depression (e.g. Pagliaccio et al., Reference Pagliaccio, Alqueza, Marsh and Auerbach2020; Saleh et al., Reference Saleh, Carballedo, Lisiecka, Fagan, Connolly, Boyle and Frodl2012). It is noteworthy, however, that studies using animal models suggest causal relations among stress, these neural structures, outcomes related to depression and anxiety (e.g. Francis et al., Reference Francis, Chandra, Friend, Finkel, Dayrit, Miranda and Lobo2015; Heshmati et al., Reference Heshmati, Christoffel, LeClair, Cathomas, Golden, Aleyasin and Russo2020). Fourth, the heterogeneity of depressive symptoms in adolescents (Lamers et al., Reference Lamers, Burstein, He, Avenevoli, Angst and Merikangas2012) may reflect biological heterogeneity in fronto-cingulate-limbic circuitry across subtypes of depression (Buch & Liston, Reference Buch and Liston2021); future studies should examine large, diverse samples from which multimodal (e.g. neurobiological and behavioral) longitudinal data can be obtained to explore factors related to stress, brain development, and the onset and course of depression. Finally, although not studied in this sample, we wish to highlight the potential role of social support in potentially mitigating the negative effects of stress on depressive symptoms in adolescents (e.g. Rueger, Malecki, Pyun, Aycock, & Coyle, Reference Rueger, Malecki, Pyun, Aycock and Coyle2016; van Harmelen et al., Reference van Harmelen, Gibson, St Clair, Owens, Brodbeck, Dunn and Goodyer2016, Reference van Harmelen, Kievit, Ioannidis, Neufeld, Jones, Bullmore and Consortium2017, Reference van Harmelen, Blakemore, Goodyer and Kievit2021). To examine these effects, we encourage future studies to investigate the extent to which levels of social support – and which sources of social support – might attenuate the association between lifetime social stressor severity and depressive symptoms in adolescents.

Despite these limitations, the present study has several strengths. First, the longitudinal design of the study enabled us to investigate how lifetime stressor exposure predicted depression severity over the course of nine months and, in addition, how these associations were mediated by gray matter morphometry of stress-sensitive brain regions. Second, we assessed stressors that occurred over the entire life course, which has rarely been done (Slavich, Reference Slavich2019). Finally, our findings add to a growing literature indicating that associations between life stressors and neural correlates of adolescent depression differ depending on the specific types of stressors experienced (e.g. Andersen & Teicher, Reference Andersen and Teicher2008; Ho & King, Reference Ho and King2021; Sheridan & McLaughlin, Reference Sheridan, McLaughlin, Asmundson and Afifi2020; Slavich & Irwin, Reference Slavich and Irwin2014).

Looking forward, it will be important for future research to focus on understanding how social stressors specifically contribute to the onset, course, and maintenance of adolescent depression. Although non-social threat severity was also associated with depression severity in our sample, this form of stress severity was not associated with any of the brain regions we investigated, suggesting that different neurobiological circuitry or processes are relevant for encoding various forms of stressors. Delineating relations between life stressors and depression in adolescence is particularly important given that this is not only a developmental period of increased risk for MDD, but also one when lifelong risk for this burdensome disorder can still be effectively mitigated (Slavich & Sacher, Reference Slavich and Sacher2019; Twenge, Cooper, Joiner, Duffy, & Binau, Reference Twenge, Cooper, Joiner, Duffy and Binau2019). That said, depression is a clinically and biologically heterogeneous disorder that differs in its biological etiology and response to treatments, likely based in part on key facets of life stressors experienced preceding a depressive episode (Ho & King, Reference Ho and King2021). A critical future research direction will be to examine other critical aspects of stress exposures (e.g. acute v. chronic stressors, abuse v. neglect, frequency v. count of stressors) and to test the extent to which these stressors have differential neurobiological impacts based on the developmental stage of the individual.

In sum, the present findings elucidate how lifetime social threat severity is related to fronto-cingulate-limbic morphometry in a well-characterized sample of depressed adolescents. Consistent with emerging work in this area, the social threat severity-sensitive regions we identified may represent promising biomarkers associated with the severity and possibly the persistence of depression in adolescents (as well as with anxiety, a common comorbidity with depression). Studies aimed at characterizing mechanisms by which different types of life stressors alter neurodevelopment and explain clinical heterogeneity are promising strategies needed to understand MDD and inform the development of novel treatments and interventions for this prevalent and costly disorder.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722002239

Acknowledgements

We thank Johanna Walker, Michelle Sanabria, Rachel Weisenburger, Jillian Segarra, Alexess Sosa, Holly Pham, Lucinda Sisk, Anna Cichocki, and Miranda Edwards for assisting with data collection and organization. We thank Grant Shields for assistance with the STRAIN. Finally, we thank the participants and their families for their contribution to this study.

Financial support

This research was supported by grants from the National Institute of Health (K01MH117442 and R01MH127176 to TCH, R37MH101495 to IHG, T32GM081760 to AO), the Klingenstein Third Generation Foundation (to TCH), Stanford's Maternal Child Health Research Institute (Early Career Award and K Award Support Grant to TCH), the Ray and Dagmar Dolby Family Fund (to TCH), the UCSF Research Evaluation and Allocation Committee (to TCH), Raschen-Tiedeman Fund (to TCH), Moffitt Memorial Fund (to TCH), the California Initiative to Advance Precision Medicine (OPR21101 to GMS), and the Office of the California Surgeon General and California Department of Health Care Services (21-10317 to GMS). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Health or any other organization. The funding agencies played no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Andersen, S. L., & Teicher, M. H. (2008). Stress, sensitive periods and maturational events in adolescent depression. Trends in Neurosciences, 31(4), 183191. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2008.01.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411, 305309. doi: 10.1038/35077083CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auerbach, R. P., Admon, R., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Adolescent depression: Stress and reward dysfunction. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 22(3), 139148. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auerbach, R. P., Pagliaccio, D., Hubbard, N. A., Frosch, I., Kremens, R., Cosby, E.Pizzagalli, D. A. (2022). Reward-related neural circuitry in depressed and anxious adolescents: a human connectome project. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 61(2), 308320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basile, B. M., Schafroth, J. L., Karaskiewicz, C. L., Chang, S. W., & Murray, E. A. (2020). The anterior cingulate cortex is necessary for forming prosocial preferences from vicarious reinforcement in monkeys. PLoS Biology, 18(6), e3000677. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000677CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S. J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 187207. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boes, A. D., McCormick, L. M., Coryell, W. H., & Nopoulos, P. (2008). Rostral anterior cingulate cortex volume correlates with depressed mood in normal healthy children. Biological Psychiatry, 63(4), 391397. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.07.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boes, A. D., Uitermarkt, B. D., Albazron, F. M., Lan, M. J., Liston, C., Pascual-Leone, A., … Fox, M. D. (2018). Rostral anterior cingulate cortex is a structural correlate of repetitive TMS treatment response in depression. Brain Stimulation, 11(3), 575581. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.01.029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breslau, J., Gilman, S. E., Stein, B. D., Ruder, T., Gmelin, T., & Miller, E. (2017). Sex differences in recent first-onset depression in an epidemiological sample of adolescents. Translational Psychiatry, 7(5), e1139. doi: 10.1038/tp.2017.105CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buch, A. M., & Liston, C. (2021). Dissecting diagnostic heterogeneity in depression by integrating neuroimaging and genetics. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(1), 156175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cazassa, M. J., Oliveira, M. D. S., Spahr, C. M., Shields, G. S., & Slavich, G. M. (2020). The Stress and Adversity Inventory for Adults (Adult STRAIN) in Brazilian Portuguese: Initial validation and links with executive function, sleep, and mental and physical health. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 3083. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03083.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Claessens, S. E., Daskalakis, N. P., van Der Veen, R., Oitzl, M. S., de Kloet, E. R., & Champagne, D. L. (2011). Development of individual differences in stress responsiveness: an overview of factors mediating the outcome of early life experiences. Psychopharmacology, 214(1), 141154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, M. M., Jing, D., Yang, R. R., Tottenham, N., Lee, F. S., & Casey, B. J. (2013). Early-life stress has persistent effects on amygdala function and development in mice and humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 110(45), 1827418278. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310163110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cunningham, W. A., & Brosch, T. (2012). Motivational salience: Amygdala tuning from traits, needs, values, and goals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 5459. doi: 10.1177/0963721411430832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davey, C. G., Cearns, M., Jamieson, A., & Harrison, B. J. (2021). Suppressed activity of the rostral anterior cingulate cortex as a biomarker for depression remission. Psychological Medicine, 18. doi: 10.1017/S0033291721004323Google ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An fMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302(5643), 290292. doi: 10.1126/science.1089134CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberger, N. I., Taylor, S. E., Gable, S. L., Hilmert, C. J., & Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Neural pathways link social support to attenuated neuroendocrine stress responses. NeuroImage, 35(4), 16011612. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, M., Nelson, E. E., Jazbec, S., McClure, E. B., Monk, C. S., Leibenluft, E., … Pine, D. S. (2005). Amygdala and nucleus accumbens in responses to receipt and omission of gains in adults and adolescents. NeuroImage, 25(4), 12791291. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.12.038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felger, J. C., & Treadway, M. T. (2017). Inflammation effects on motivation and motor activity: Role of dopamine. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(1), 216241. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.143CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fjell, A. M., Chen, C. H., Sederevicius, D., Sneve, M. H., Grydeland, H., Krogsrud, S. K.Walhovd, K. B. (2019). Continuity and discontinuity in human cortical development and change from embryonic stages to old age. Cerebral Cortex, 29(9), 38793890.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flynn, M., & Rudolph, K. D. (2011). Stress generation and adolescent depression: Contribution of interpersonal stress responses. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 39(8), 11871198. doi: 10.1007/s10802-011-9527-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foulkes, L., & Blakemore, S. J. (2016). Is there heightened sensitivity to social reward in adolescence? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 40, 8185. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.06.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, T. C., Chandra, R., Friend, D. M., Finkel, E., Dayrit, G., Miranda, J., … Lobo, M. K. (2015). Nucleus accumbens medium spiny neuron subtypes mediate depression-related outcomes to social defeat stress. Biological Psychiatry, 77(3), 212222. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.07.021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedrich, M. J. (2017). Depression is the leading cause of disability around the world. JAMA, 317(15), 1517. doi: 10.1001/jama.2017.3826Google ScholarPubMed
Frodl, T., Meisenzahl, E., Zetzsche, T., Bottlender, R., Born, C., Groll, C., … Möller, H. J. (2002). Enlargement of the amygdala in patients with a first episode of major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 51(9), 708714. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01359-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galván, A. (2010). Adolescent development of the reward system. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 6. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.006.2010Google ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C. (2005). Stress and depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 293319. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.1.102803.143938CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C. (2006). Stress generation in depression: Reflections on origins, research, and future directions. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 62(9), 10651082. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20293CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hammen, C. (2009). Adolescent depression: Stressful interpersonal contexts and risk for recurrence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 200204. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01636.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herting, M. M., Johnson, C., Mills, K. L., Vijayakumar, N., Dennison, M., Liu, C., … Tamnes, C. K. (2018). Development of subcortical volumes across adolescence in males and females: A multisample study of longitudinal changes. NeuroImage, 172, 194205. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heshmati, M., Christoffel, D. J., LeClair, K., Cathomas, F., Golden, S. A., Aleyasin, H., … Russo, S. J. (2020). Depression and social defeat stress are associated with inhibitory synaptic changes in the nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience, 40(32), 62286233. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2568-19.2020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C. (2019). Stress and neurodevelopment in adolescent depression. Biological Psychiatry, 86(10), e33e35. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.09.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C. (2021). Editorial: Toward neurobiological-based treatments of depression and anxiety: A potential case for the nucleus accumbens. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, S0890–8567(21), 00417–2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2021.06.013Google Scholar
Ho, T. C., Cichocki, A. C., Gifuni, A. J., Catalina Camacho, M., Ordaz, S. J., Singh, M. K., & Gotlib, I. H. (2018). Reduced dorsal striatal gray matter volume predicts implicit suicidal ideation in adolescents. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 13(11), 12151224. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsy089CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C., Gutman, B., Pozzi, E., Grabe, H. J., Hosten, N., Wittfeld, K., … Schmaal, L. (2022). Subcortical shape alterations in major depressive disorder: Findings from the ENIGMA major depressive disorder working group. Human Brain Mapping, 43(1), 341–351. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24988CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C., & King, L. S. (2021). Mechanisms of neuroplasticity linking adversity to depression: Developmental considerations. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 517. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01639-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C., King, L. S., Leong, J. K., Colich, N. L., Humphreys, K. L., Ordaz, S. J., & Gotlib, I. H. (2017b). Effects of sensitivity to life stress on uncinate fasciculus segments in early adolescence. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(9), 14601469. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C., Sacchet, M. D., Connolly, C. G., Margulies, D. S., Tymofiyeva, O., Paulus, M. P., … Yang, T. T. (2017a). Inflexible functional connectivity of the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex in adolescent major depressive disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology, 42(12), 24342445. doi: 10.1038/npp.2017.103CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ho, T. C., Teresi, G. I., Ojha, A., Walker, J. C., Kirshenbaum, J. S., Singh, M. K., & Gotlib, I. H. (2021). Smaller caudate gray matter volume is associated with greater implicit suicidal ideation in depressed adolescents. Journal of Affective Disorders, 278, 650657. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.09.046CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hölzel, B. K., Carmody, J., Evans, K. C., Hoge, E. A., Dusek, J. A., Morgan, L., … Lazar, S. W. (2010). Stress reduction correlates with structural changes in the amygdala. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(1), 1117. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp034CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Humphreys, K. L., King, L. S., Sacchet, M. D., Camacho, M. C., Colich, N. L., Ordaz, S. J., … Gotlib, I. H. (2019). Evidence for a sensitive period in the effects of early life stress on hippocampal volume. Developmental Science, 22(3), e12775. doi: 10.1111/desc.12775CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jamieson, A. J., Harrison, B. J., Razi, A., & Davey, C. G. (2022). Rostral anterior cingulate network effective connectivity in depressed adolescents and associations with treatment response in a randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychopharmacology, 47(6), 12401248. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-01214-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U. M. A., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., … Ryan, N. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D. A., Ryan, N. D., & Rao, U. (2000). K-SADS-PL. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(10), 1208. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200010000-00002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Knutson, B., Adams, C. M., Fong, G. W., & Hommer, D. (2001). Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectivity recruits nucleus accumbens. Journal of Neuroscience, 21(16), RC159. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-j0002.2001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(1), 126. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamers, F., Burstein, M., He, J. P., Avenevoli, S., Angst, J., & Merikangas, K. R. (2012). Structure of major depressive disorder in adolescents and adults in the US general population. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 201(2), 143150. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.098079CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, B., & Luna, B. (2018). Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 94, 179195. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lee, K. H., Yoo, J. H., Lee, J., Kim, S. H., Han, J. Y., Hong, S. B., … Brent, D. A. (2020). The indirect effect of peer problems on adolescent depression through nucleus accumbens volume alteration. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 19. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-69769-3Google ScholarPubMed
Lemoult, J., Humphreys, K. L., Tracy, A., Hoffmeister, J. A., Ip, E., & Gotlib, I. H. (2020). Meta-analysis: Exposure to early life stress and risk for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(7), 842855. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.10.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewinsohn, P. M., Allen, N. B., Seeley, J. R., & Gotlib, I. H. (1999). First onset versus recurrence of depression: Differential processes of psychosocial risk. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 108(3), 483489. doi: 10.1037//0021-843x.108.3.483CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lichenstein, S. D., Verstynen, T., & Forbes, E. E. (2016). Adolescent brain development and depression: A case for the importance of connectivity of the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 271287. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüdecke, D., Ben-Shachar, M. S., Patil, I., & Makowski, D. (2020). Extracting, computing and exploring the parameters of statistical models using R. Journal of Open Source Software, 5(53), 2445. doi: 10.21105/joss.02445CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacMaster, F. P., & Kusumakar, V. (2004). Hippocampal volume in early onset depression. BMC Medicine, 2(2), 16. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-2-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacMaster, F. P., Mirza, Y., Szeszko, P. R., Kmiecik, L. E., Easter, P. C., Taormina, S. P., … Rosenberg, D. R. (2008). Amygdala and hippocampal volumes in familial early onset major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 63(4), 385390. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.05.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
March, J. S. (2012). Multidimensional anxiety scale for children (2nd Edn.). North Tonawanda, NY: Multi-Health Systems.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
March, J. S., Parker, J. D. A., Sullivan, K., Stallings, P., & Conners, C. K. (1997). The multidimensional anxiety scale for children (MASC): Factor structure, reliability, and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 554565. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199704000-00019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mirsu-Paun, A., & Oliver, J. A. (2017). How much does love really hurt? A meta-analysis of the association between romantic relationship quality, breakups and mental health outcomes in adolescents and young adults. Journal of Relationships Research, 8, e5. doi: 10.1017/jrr.2017.6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monroe, S. M., & Harkness, K. L. (2005). Life stress, the ‘kindling’ hypothesis, and the recurrence of depression: Considerations from a life stress perspective. Psychological Review, 112(2), 417445. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.2.417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naicker, K., Galambos, N. L., Zeng, Y., Senthilselvan, A., & Colman, I. (2013). Social, demographic, and health outcomes in the 10 years following adolescent depression. Journal of Adolescent Health, 52(5), 533538. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.12.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pagliaccio, D., Alqueza, K. L., Marsh, R., & Auerbach, R. P. (2020). Brain volume abnormalities in youth at high risk for depression: Adolescent brain and cognitive development study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 59(10), 11781188. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2019.09.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panizzon, M. S., Fennema-Notestine, C., Eyler, L. T., Jernigan, T. L., Prom-Wormley, E., Neale, M., … Kremen, W. S. (2009). Distinct genetic influences on cortical surface area and cortical thickness. Cerebral Cortex, 19(11), 27282735. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pannekoek, J. N., van der Werff, S. J., van den Bulk, B. G., van Lang, N. D., Rombouts, S. A., van Buchem, M. A., … van der Wee, N. J. (2014). Reduced anterior cingulate gray matter volume in treatment-naive clinically depressed adolescents. NeuroImage: Clinical, 4, 336342. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2014.01.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parr, A. C., Calabro, F., Larsen, B., Tervo-Clemmens, B., Elliot, S., Foran, W., … Luna, B. (2021). Dopamine-related striatal neurophysiology is associated with specialization of frontostriatal reward circuitry through adolescence. Progress in Neurobiology, 201, 101997. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2021.101997CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pizzagalli, D. A. (2014). Depression, stress, and anhedonia: Toward a synthesis and integrated model. Annual of Clinical Psychology, 10, 393423. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185606CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Platt, B., Kadosh, K. C., & Lau, J. Y. (2013). The role of peer rejection in adolescent depression. Depression and Anxiety, 30(9), 809821. doi: 10.1002/da.22120CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poznanski, E. O., & Mokros, H. B. (1996). Children's depression rating scale, revised (CDRS-R). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2020). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/.Google Scholar
Reynolds, W. M. (2010). Reynolds adolescent depression scale. Atlanta, GA, USA: The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology, 1–1. doi: 10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0798CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roozendaal, B., McEwen, B. S., & Chattarji, S. (2009). Stress, memory and the amygdala. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 423433. doi: 10.1038/nrn2651CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosso, I. M., Cintron, C. M., Steingard, R. J., Renshaw, P. F., Young, A. D., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2005). Amygdala and hippocampus volumes in pediatric major depression. Biological Psychiatry, 57(1), 2126. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rueger, S. Y., Malecki, C. K., Pyun, Y., Aycock, C., & Coyle, S. (2016). A meta-analytic review of the association between perceived social support and depression in childhood and adolescence. Psychological Bulletin, 142(10), 10171067. doi: 10.1037/bul0000058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salamone, J., Correa, M., Mingote, S., & Weber, S. (2005). Beyond the reward hypothesis: Alternative functions of nucleus accumbens dopamine. Current Opinion in Pharmacology, 5(1), 3441. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2004.09.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saleh, K., Carballedo, A., Lisiecka, D., Fagan, A. J., Connolly, G., Boyle, G., & Frodl, T. (2012). Impact of family history and depression on amygdala volume. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 203(1), 2430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmaal, L. (2019). Cortical surface area: a potential biological marker for depression onset and poor clinical outcomes. The Lancet Psychiatry, 6(4), 277279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmaal, L., Yücel, M., Ellis, R., Vijayakumar, N., Simmons, J. G., Allen, N. B., & Whittle, S. (2017). Brain structural signatures of adolescent depressive symptom trajectories: A longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(7), 593601. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheets, E. S., & Craighead, W. E. (2014). Comparing chronic interpersonal and noninterpersonal stress domains as predictors of depression recurrence in emerging adults. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 63, 3642. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.09.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheline, Y. I., Liston, C., & McEwen, B. S. (2019). Parsing the hippocampus in depression: Chronic stress, hippocampal volume, and major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 85(6), 436438. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.01.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shenhav, A., Botvinick, M. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2013). The expected value of control: an integrative theory of anterior cingulate cortex function. Neuron, 79(2), 217240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheridan, M. A., & McLaughlin, K. A. (2020). Neurodevelopmental mechanisms linking ACEs with psychopathology. In Asmundson, J. G. G. & Afifi, T. O. (Eds.), Adverse childhood experiences (pp. 265285). Cambridge, MA, USA: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-816065-7.00013-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Siegle, G. J., Steinhauer, S. R., Thase, M. E., Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2002). Can't shake that feeling: Event-related fMRI assessment of sustained amygdala activity in response to emotional information in depressed individuals. Biological Psychiatry, 51(9), 693707. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01314-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M. (2019). Stressnology: The primitive (and problematic) study of life stress exposure and pressing need for better measurement. Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 75, 35. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2018.08.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., & Irwin, M. R. (2014). From stress to inflammation and major depressive disorder: A social signal transduction theory of depression. Psychological Bulletin, 140(3), 774815. doi: 10.1037/a0035302CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., O'Donovan, A., Epel, E. S., & Kemeny, M. E. (2010a). Black sheep get the blues: A psychobiological model of social rejection and depression. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 3945. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.01.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slavich, G. M., & Sacher, J. (2019). Stress, sex hormones, inflammation, and major depressive disorder: Extending social signal transduction theory of depression to account for sex differences in mood disorders. Psychopharmacology, 236(10), 30633079. doi: 10.1007/s00213-019-05326-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., & Shields, G. S. (2018). Assessing lifetime stress exposure using the Stress and Adversity Inventory for Adults (Adult STRAIN): An overview and initial validation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 80(1), 1727. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000534CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., Stewart, J. G., Esposito, E. C., Shields, G. S., & Auerbach, R. P. (2019). The Stress and Adversity Inventory for Adolescents (Adolescent STRAIN): Associations with mental and physical health, risky behaviors, and psychiatric diagnoses in youth seeking treatment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(9), 9981009. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13038Google ScholarPubMed
Slavich, G. M., Way, B. M., Eisenberger, N. I., & Taylor, S. E. (2010b). Neural sensitivity to social rejection is associated with inflammatory responses to social stress. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 107(33), 1481714822. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1009164107CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M., & Casey, B. J. (2010). A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain Cognition, 72(1), 124133. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.07.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9(2), 6974. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storsve, A. B., Fjell, A. M., Tamnes, C. K., Westlye, L. T., Overbye, K., Aasland, H. W., & Walhovd, K. B. (2014). Differential longitudinal changes in cortical thickness, surface area and volume across the adult life span: regions of accelerating and decelerating change. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(25), 84888498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturmbauer, S. C., Shields, G. S., Hetzel, E. L., Rohleder, N., & Slavich, G. M. (2019). The Stress and Adversity Inventory for Adults (Adult STRAIN) in German: An overview and initial validation. PLoS ONE, 14(5), e0216419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Anderson, C. M., & Ohashi, K. (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(10), 652666. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tottenham, N., & Galván, A. (2016). Stress and the adolescent brain: Amygdala-prefrontal cortex circuitry and ventral striatum as developmental targets. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 217227. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.07.030CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Twenge, J. M., Cooper, A. B., Joiner, T. E., Duffy, M. E., & Binau, S. G. (2019). Age, period, and cohort trends in mood disorder indicators and suicide-related outcomes in a nationally representative dataset, 2005–2017. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 128(3), 185. doi: 10.1037/abn0000410CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Harmelen, A. L., Blakemore, S. J., Goodyer, I. M., & Kievit, R. A. (2021). The interplay between adolescent friendship quality and resilient functioning following childhood and adolescent adversity. Adversity and Resilience Science, 2(1), 3750. doi: 10.1007/s42844-020-00027-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Harmelen, A. L., Gibson, J. L., St Clair, M. C., Owens, M., Brodbeck, J., Dunn, V., … Goodyer, I. M. (2016). Friendships and family support reduce subsequent depressive symptoms in at-risk adolescents. PLoS ONE, 11(5), e0153715. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0153715CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Harmelen, A. L., Kievit, R. A., Ioannidis, K., Neufeld, S., Jones, P. B., Bullmore, E., … Consortium, N. S. P. N. (2017). Adolescent friendships predict later resilient functioning across psychosocial domains in a healthy community cohort. Psychological Medicine, 47(13), 23122322. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717000836CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Marle, H. J. F., Hermans, E. J., Qin, S., & Fernández, G. (2009). From specificity to sensitivity: How acute stress affects amygdala processing of biologically salient stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 66(7), 649655. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vrshek-Schallhorn, S., Stroud, C. B., Mineka, S., Hammen, C., Zinbarg, R. E., Wolitzky-Taylor, K., & Craske, M. G. (2015). Chronic and episodic interpersonal stress as statistically unique predictors of depression in two samples of emerging adults. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(4), 918. doi: 10.1037/abn0000088CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wacker, J., Dillon, D. G., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2009). The role of the nucleus accumbens and rostral anterior cingulate cortex in anhedonia: Integration of resting EEG, fMRI, and volumetric techniques. NeuroImage, 46(1), 327337. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.058CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wager, T. D., van Ast, V. A., Hughes, B. L., Davidson, M. L., Lindquist, M. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2009). Brain mediators of cardiovascular responses to social threat, part II: Prefrontal-subcortical pathways and relationship with anxiety. NeuroImage, 47(3), 836851. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.044CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, J. C., Teresi, G. I., Weisenburger, R. L., Segarra, J. R., Ojha, A., Kulla, A., … Ho, T. C. (2020). Study protocol for teen inflammation glutamate emotion research (TIGER). Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, 585512. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.585512CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, H., Braun, C., & Enck, P. (2017). How the brain reacts to social stress (exclusion) – a scoping review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 8088. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, C. A., Olson, E. A., Killgore, W. D., Pizzagalli, D. A., Rauch, S. L., & Rosso, I. M. (2018). Rostral anterior cingulate cortex morphology predicts treatment response to internet-based cognitive behavioral therapy for depression. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 3(3), 255262. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.08.005Google ScholarPubMed
Wei, C., Hoff, A., Villabø, M. A., Peterman, J., Kendall, P. C., Piacentini, J. … March, J. (2014). Assessing anxiety in youth with the multidimensional anxiety scale for children. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 43, 566578. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.814541CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whittle, S., Lichter, R., Dennison, M., Vijayakumar, N., Schwartz, O., Byrne, M. L., … Allen, N. B. (2014). Structural brain development and depression onset during adolescence: A prospective longitudinal study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(5), 564571. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070920CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wierenga, L. M., Langen, M., Oranje, B., & Durston, S. (2014). Unique developmental trajectories of cortical thickness and surface area. Neuroimage, 87, 120126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization. (2017). Depression and other common mental disorders: global health estimates. Retrieved from https://apps.who.int/iris/bitstream/handle/10665/254610/W?sequence=1.Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Yang, Y., Zhu, L., Zhu, Q., Jia, Y., Zhang, L., … Wang, J. (2021). Volumetric deficit within the fronto-limbic-striatal circuit in first-episode drug naïve patients with major depression disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 600583. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.600583CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zisook, S., Rush, A. J., Lesser, I., Wisniewski, S. R., Trivedi, M., Husain, M. M., … Fava, M. (2007). Preadult onset vs. adult onset of major depressive disorder: A replication study. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 115(3), 196205. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2006.00868.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Illustration of cortical and subcortical ROIs in standard space (for visualization purposes): rostral anterior cingulate cortex (dark purple), caudal anterior cingulate cortex (dark blue); nucleus accumbens (red), amygdala (green), hippocampus (teal). Segmentation of and estimation of gray matter for each ROI was performed within each individual. ROI, region of interest.

Figure 1

Table 1. Descriptive statistics of participant demographic, clinical, and neural characteristics for the 57 participants with Adolescent STRAIN and structural fMRI data at baseline

Figure 2

Fig. 2. Summary of primary results. Scatterplot and fitted regression line illustrating the association between (a) lifetime social threat severity and depression severity, (b) lifetime social threat severity and gray matter volumes of bilateral NAcc, (c) lifetime social threat severity and depression severity over the course of the study (intraindividual average RADS-2 score across all timepoints), and (d) gray matter volumes of bilateral NAcc and depression severity over the course of the study (intraindividual average RADS-2 score across all timepoints). NAcc, nucleus accumbens; RADS-2, Reynolds Adolescent Depression Scale, 2nd Edition; T1, baseline.

Figure 3

Table 2. Fixed effects estimated from linear models testing for associations between social and non-social threat severity with depression symptoms at baseline (cross-sectional) and across nine months (longitudinal)

Figure 4

Table 3. Fixed effects estimated from linear models testing associations between social threat severity with subcortical gray matter volumes at baseline

Figure 5

Table 4. Fixed effects estimated from linear models testing associations between social threat severity with cortical thickness and surface area at baseline

Figure 6

Table 5. Fixed effects estimated from linear mixed-effects models testing associations between rACC surface area, cACC surface area, NAcc GMV, and amygdala GMV with depressive symptoms (RADS-2 scores) longitudinally over 9 months

Figure 7

Fig. 3. Summary of exploratory mediation analyses. Mediation model schematic illustrating associations between lifetime social threat severity, depression severity over the course of the study (intraindividual average RADS-2 score across all timepoints), and the indirect effect of (a) rACC SA, (b) cACC SA, (c) amygdala GMV, and (d) NAcc GMV. rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; cACC, caudal anterior cingulate cortex; NAcc, nucleus accumbens; SA, surface area; GMV, gray matter volume; ST, lifetime social threat severity.

Supplementary material: File

Ojha et al. supplementary material

Ojha et al. supplementary material

Download Ojha et al. supplementary material(File)
File 659.9 KB