Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-dsjbd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T23:03:26.995Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Specificity in genetic and environmental risk for prescription opioid misuse and heroin use

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2023

Genevieve F. Dash*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
Ian R. Gizer
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211, USA
Nicholas G. Martin
Affiliation:
QIMR Berghofer, Brisbane, Queensland 4006, Australia
Wendy S. Slutske
Affiliation:
Department of Family Medicine and Community Health and Center for Tobacco Research and Intervention, University of Wisconsin, Madison, WI 53711, USA
*
Author for correspondence: Genevieve F. Dash, E-mail: genevievedash@mail.missouri.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Many studies aggregate prescription opioid misuse (POM) and heroin use into a single phenotype, but emerging evidence suggests that their genetic and environmental influences may be partially distinct.

Methods

In total, 7164 individual twins (84.12% complete pairs; 59.81% female; mean age = 30.58 years) from the Australian Twin Registry reported their lifetime misuse of prescription opioids, stimulants, and sedatives, and lifetime use of heroin, cannabis, cocaine/crack, illicit stimulants, hallucinogens, inhalants, solvents, and dissociatives via telephone interview. Independent pathway models (IPMs) and common pathway models (CPMs) partitioned the variance of drug use phenotypes into general and drug-specific genetic (a), common environmental (c), and unique environmental factors (e).

Results

An IPM with one general a and one general e factor and a one-factor CPM provided comparable fit to the data. General factors accounted for 55% (a = 14%, e = 41%) and 79% (a = 64%, e = 15%) of the respective variation in POM and heroin use in the IPM, and 25% (a = 12%, c = 8%, e = 5%) and 80% (a = 38%, c = 27%, e = 15%) of the respective variation in POM and heroin use in the CPM. Across both models, POM emerged with substantial drug-specific genetic influence (26–39% of total phenotypic variance; 69–74% of genetic variance); heroin use did not (0% of total phenotypic variance; 0% of genetic variance in both models). Prescription sedative misuse also demonstrated significant drug-specific genetic variance.

Conclusions

Genetic variation in POM, but not heroin use, is predominantly drug-specific. Misuse of prescription medications that reduce experiences of subjective distress may be partially influenced by sources of genetic variation separate from illicit drug use.

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Opioid use and related mortality remain a major public health concern (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, 2021). Despite substantial effort to explicate the genetic etiology of opioid use and associated sequalae (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Yang, Zhou, Nunez, Kranzler and Gelernter2020; Deak et al., Reference Deak, Zhou, Galimberti, Levey, Wendt, Sanchez-Roige and Demontis2022; Jensen, Reference Jensen2016; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Rentsch, Cheng, Kember, Nunez, Sherva and Polimanti2020), many twin and genomic studies have struggled to identify significant and/or replicable findings (Crist, Reiner, & Berrettini, Reference Crist, Reiner and Berrettini2019; Polimanti et al., Reference Polimanti, Walters, Johnson, McClintick, Adkins, Adkins and Brown2020; Reed & Kreek, Reference Reed and Kreek2021). Heritability estimates of opioid use derived from twin studies have been varied, ranging from 0% to 79% (Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey, & Slutske, Reference Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey and Slutske2022; Karkowski, Prescott, & Kendler, Reference Karkowski, Prescott and Kendler2000; Kendler, Aggen, Tambs, & Reichborn-Kjennerud, Reference Kendler, Aggen, Tambs and Reichborn-Kjennerud2006; Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott, & Neale, Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott and Neale2003; Kendler, Karkowski, Neale, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Karkowski, Neale and Prescott2000a; Kendler, Karkowski, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999; Tsuang et al., Reference Tsuang, Lyons, Meyer, Doyle, Eisen, Goldberg and Eaves1998; Van den Bree, Johnson, Neale, & Pickens, Reference Van den Bree, Johnson, Neale and Pickens1998). Such discrepancies in findings regarding the relative influence of genes and environment on opioid use may be at least partially attributable to the operationalization of opioid use as a single behavior encompassing both prescription opioid misuse (POM) and heroin use: emerging evidence suggests that POM and heroin use may be differentially influenced by genes and environment, such that POM is more strongly influenced by genes and heroin use is more strongly influenced by common environment (Dash et al., Reference Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey and Slutske2022; Gillespie et al., Reference Gillespie, Bates, Hickie, Medland, Verhulst, Kirkpatrick and Benotsch2019).

Multivariate twin studies have demonstrated that liability for drug use can be accounted for by both a general propensity for any drug use and some amount of drug-specific influence, though most, if not all, such studies have utilized an aggregated opioid use phenotype. Genetic and common environmental influence on opioid use appear to be attributable to liability shared with cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogen, sedative, and stimulant use, though the magnitude of genetic and common environmental influence on opioid use identified in these studies is varied (3–37% and 17–40%, respectively); there also appears to be opioid-specific unique environmental influence (13–29% of the phenotypic variance) (Karkowski et al., Reference Karkowski, Prescott and Kendler2000; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott and Neale2003). Conversely, a DSM-III opioid abuse phenotype was found to have substantial drug-specific genetic influence (38% of the phenotypic variance) as well as drug-specific unique environmental influence (12% of the phenotypic variance) (Tsuang et al., Reference Tsuang, Lyons, Meyer, Doyle, Eisen, Goldberg and Eaves1998). It remains unclear whether these patterns of findings would hold for POM and heroin use individually.

Multivariate twin studies have also identified two distinct sets of genetic risk factors for substance use, with one predisposing to licit use (alcohol, caffeine, nicotine), the other to illicit use (cannabis, cocaine), and some degree of shared genetic risk across these two factors (Kendler, Myers, & Prescott, Reference Kendler, Myers and Prescott2007). It is not immediately obvious whether prescription misuse behaviors are more aptly operationalized as ‘licit’ or ‘illicit’ use given that prescription drugs are conditionally legal to obtain and possess, but can be acquired illegally and/or used in a manner more consistent with illicit drug use behavior (e.g. via insufflation, injection). Substantial evidence indicates that individuals reporting POM tend to misuse other prescription drugs at higher rates, whereas individuals reporting heroin use tend to use other illicit drugs at higher rates (Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey, & Slutske, Reference Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey and Slutske2021a; Rigg & Monnat, Reference Rigg and Monnat2015; Wu, Woody, Yang, & Blazer, Reference Wu, Woody, Yang and Blazer2011). In other words, POM behavior tends to ‘cluster’ more closely with other prescription misuse behaviors (sedative, tranquilizer), while heroin use tends to ‘cluster’ more closely with use of other illicit drugs (cocaine, inhalants, hallucinogens), supporting the notion of distinct but correlated licit/prescription and illicit (mis)use factors.

Present study

To date, twin studies that have parsed POM and heroin use have only done so within univariate and bivariate frameworks (Dash et al., Reference Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey and Slutske2022; Gillespie et al., Reference Gillespie, Bates, Hickie, Medland, Verhulst, Kirkpatrick and Benotsch2019). The present study aimed to expand on this emergent literature by modeling POM and heroin use as distinct phenotypes within a multivariate framework as a means of explicating the degree to which their genetic and environmental influences are shared both with each other and with other drug (mis)use more broadly. Further, we aimed to explore the notion of separable prescription and illicit (mis)use factors by modeling POM and heroin use on distinct genetic and environmental factors alongside other forms of prescription misuse and illicit drug use, respectively. It was hypothesized that such a configuration would provide a better explanatory model than a single factor model, thereby replicating prior findings of differentiable sources of genetic influence across subcategories of substances (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Myers and Prescott2007).

Methods

Participants and procedure

Data were drawn from Australian Twin Registry (ATR) cohorts II and III, which include Australian twins of primarily European ancestry born between 1964 and 1979. ATR cohort II data were collected between 1996 and 2000 via a telephone interview; ATR cohort III data were collected between 2005 and 2009 via computer-assisted telephone interview. The combined sample was comprised of 7164 individual twins [mean age = 30.58 years (s.d. = 2.64), range = 22–43] from both complete and incomplete same-sex pairs [monozygotic (MZ) male = 1555; MZ female = 2405; dizygotic (DZ) male = 1324; DZ female = 1880]. Of the full sample, 6026 individual twins (84.12%) were part of a complete twin pair (MZ male = 1264; MZ female = 2122; DZ male = 1030; DZ female = 1610).

Measures

Assessments were conducted within the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism, adapted for the Australian population (SSAGA-OZ) (Bucholz et al., Reference Bucholz, Cadoret, Cloninger, Dinwiddie, Hesselbrock, Nurnberger and Schuckit1994). Ahead of their interview, participants were provided with a respondent booklet containing lists of specific drugs described by name and by common slang terms, where relevant. Lists were grouped by drug class, and included cannabis/hashish, cocaine/crack, amphetamine-based stimulants, opioids, sedatives, hallucinogens, dissociatives, solvents, and inhalants. Participants were asked ‘Have you ever used any of the items in List [X]?’ A positive response prompted a query as to which drug(s) on the list they had used, with instruction for reporting misuse of medically indicated drugs (‘when not prescribed or more than prescribed’). Responses were coded as binary (yes/no) variables.

Analytic plan

Analyses were conducted in Mplus Version 8 (Muthén, Reference Muthén2017). All models were fitted by the method of robust weighted least squares directly to the raw twin data, which uses data from incomplete as well as complete twin pairs, and bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals were estimated. A liability-threshold model, which assumes that there is a latent liability continuum underlying the binary drug use variables, was employed (Neale & Cardon, Reference Neale and Cardon2013). Thresholds and variances were constrained to equality across twins and zygosity groups but were permitted to differ across men and women after testing against saturated models determined that these specifications fit the data well. Univariate biometric models were run to partition the variation in drug use liability into additive genetic (a), common environmental (c), and unique (individual-specific) environmental (e) influences; the latter also includes measurement error. Quantitative sex differences were examined via Wald tests to determine if the magnitude of a, c, and e influences differed across men and women. A series of bivariate biometric models were then fit to estimate the phenotypic and cross-twin cross-trait correlations between POM and heroin use, POM and all other drug use phenotypes, and heroin use and all other drug use phenotypes.

Next, a series of independent pathway models (IPMs) and common pathway models (CPMs) were run. IPMs estimate general a, c, and e factors that influence all phenotypes, as well as drug-specific (residual) a, c, and e effects. The IPM assumes that a, c, and e factors operate independently of one another to influence manifest phenotypes (see online Supplementary Fig. S1). Congruent with other studies applying IPMs to substance use phenotypes (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott and Neale2003), and determined to be appropriate after testing against a saturated model, [χ2(399) = 424.56, p = 0.18], we began with a model comprised of two general a factors, two general c factors, and two general e factors, as well as drug-specific a, c, and e factors for each phenotype, here referred to as the ‘2-2-2 model’ wherein each number corresponds to the number of general a-c-e factors in the model. To test the hypothesis of separable genetic and environmental influences on prescription misuse and illicit use, constraints were imposed such that prescription opioid, sedative, and stimulant use loaded on one of the two factors for a given variance component, while heroin, cannabis, cocaine/crack, illicit stimulant, hallucinogens, inhalant, solvent, and dissociative use loaded on the other. This configuration was imposed iteratively upon each variance component in combination and alone (i.e. ace, ac, ae, ce, a, c, e) within the full 2-2-2 model, and model comparison was conducted via Wald tests. We subsequently conducted further testing of reduced models to identify the optimal number of factors and determine the most parsimonious solution. The CPM was used as a comparator against the IPM. The CPM assumes that the covariation between phenotypes is attributable to the influence of an intermediate, latent general phenotype that combines the effects of a, c, and e such that they do not act independently (see online Supplementary Fig. S2). As such, the a, c, and e effects mediated via the general phenotype are proportionally equivalent across all observed phenotypes in the model. In addition to the general phenotype, drug-specific a, c, and e influences were modeled within the CPM. Model comparison was conducted to identify the best-fitting and most parsimonious model via Wald tests. Sex-specific effects were not explored in the IPMs and CPMs to enhance power by utilizing the full combined sample, though models with sex included as a covariate were run. Model fit was evaluated via the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) (Steiger, Reference Steiger1990), the non-normed fit index (i.e. Tucker–Lewis Index or TLI) (Tucker & Lewis, Reference Tucker and Lewis1973) and the comparative fit index (CFI) (Bentler, Reference Bentler1990). Cutoffs were applied as follows: RMSEA < 0.05, TLI > 0.95, and CFI > 0.95 (Chen, Curran, Bollen, Kirby, & Paxton, Reference Chen, Curran, Bollen, Kirby and Paxton2008; Hooper, Coughlan, & Mullen, Reference Hooper, Coughlan and Mullen2008; Hu & Bentler, Reference Hu and Bentler1999; Yu, Reference Yu2002).

Results

Descriptive findings

Lifetime POM and heroin use were endorsed by 7.79% and 1.31% of the sample, respectively. Cannabis was the most commonly used drug (61.81%), followed by prescription stimulants (17.46%), hallucinogens (16.85%), illicit stimulants (15.05%), inhalants (13.31%), cocaine/crack (11.96%), prescription sedatives (8.94%), solvents (3.41%), and dissociatives (1.86%). Prevalence of (mis)use for each drug by zygosity group is presented in online Supplementary Table S1. Men misused prescription stimulants [χ2(1) = 88.78, p < 0.001] and used heroin [χ2(1) = 20.23, p < 0.001], cannabis [χ2(1) = 145.84, p < 0.001], cocaine/crack [χ2(1) = 52.60, p < 0.001], illicit stimulants [χ2(1) = 33.74, p < 0.001], hallucinogens [χ2(1) = 114.45, p < 0.001], inhalants [χ2(1) = 130.07, p < 0.001], solvents [χ2(1) = 22.80, p < 0.001], and dissociatives [χ2(1) = 19.38, p < 0.001] at higher rates than women; only prescription opioid and sedative misuse did not differ in rates of use across men and women (ps = 0.41–0.57).

Variance estimates from univariate models are depicted in Fig. 1 (standardized coefficients, confidence intervals, and model fit statistics are available in online Supplementary Table S2). Estimates for a, c, and e components could be constrained to equality across men and women for all drug use phenotypes except cannabis use. POM was attributable to a (37%), c (10%, ns), and e (53%); heroin use was attributable to a (8%, ns), c (74%), and e (17%). For the remaining drugs, the proportion of a influence ranged from 24% (ns) for dissociative use to 60% for prescription sedative misuse; the proportion of c influence ranged from 0% for prescription sedative misuse to 53% for dissociative use; and the proportion of e influence ranged from 20% for illicit stimulant use to 40% for prescription sedative misuse.

Fig. 1. Proportion of variance in lifetime drug use attributable to additive genetic (a), common environmental (c), and unique environmental (e) factors in univariate models.

Note: Parameters could be constrained to equality across men and women in all models except cannabis use, for which a and c parameters could be individually, but not simultaneously, constrained [Wald χ2(2) = 8.02, p = 0.02]; a, c, and e estimates from the freely estimated model were 31, 33, and 37% for men and 51, 27, and 22% for women (estimates from the constrained cannabis use model are presented for consistency).

Bivariate correlations are presented in Table 1. Cannabis use represented the weakest association for both opioid types in the phenotypic (within-twin) correlations. POM was most strongly associated with prescription sedative misuse and heroin use among both men (r = 0.70–0.65) and women (r = 0.55–0.64). Heroin use was most strongly associated with cocaine/crack use (r = 0.81) and hallucinogen use among men (r = 0.75) and hallucinogen use (r = 0.81), cocaine/crack use (r = 0.80), and dissociative use (r = 0.80) among women. For the cross-twin cross-trait correlations, MZ twin correlations were generally larger in magnitude than the DZ correlations, implying some degree of genetic influence on the overlap between POM, heroin use, and other drug use. Cross-twin cross-trait correlations were substantially larger in magnitude for heroin and other drug use than for POM and other drug use.

Table 1. Cross-trait correlations for prescription opioid misuse, heroin use, and other drug (mis)use derived from bivariate biometric models

MZM, monozygotic male; MZF, monozygotic female; DZM, dizygotic male; DZF, dizygotic female.

Note: Bold font indicates significant correlation, p < 0.001; italic font indicates significant correlation, p < 0.05.

Multivariate models

Model selection

Results of model comparison are presented in Table 2. The full 2-2-2 IPM, comprised of two general factors for each a, c, and e as well as drug-specific a, c, and e factors, provided good fit to the data [χ2(399) = 424.58; RMSEA = 0.006 (95% CI 0.000–0.010); TLI = 0.999; CFI = 0.999]. Although the prescription-illicit factor configuration could be imposed upon each variance component alone and in combination without worsening model fit (see online Supplementary Table S3), further model comparison indicated that this was likely due to two general factors per variance component being unnecessary. That is, the second general a, c, and e factors could all be dropped without significant decrement in model fit [Wald χ2(30) = 13.16, p = 0.99] to create a 1-1-1 model with only one general factor for each a, c, and e, suggesting that variance shared across drugs is common to all drug types rather than separable clusters of drugs (i.e. prescription and illicit). While dropping any one of the general factors from the resulting 1-1-1 IPM did not significantly worsen model fit, the model with no general a factor (0-1-1 model) and the model with no general e factor (1-1-0 model) did not converge, implying poor fit of these models to the data. As such, we proceeded with a model with one general a and one general e factor (1-0-1 model), which did not worsen model fit compared to either the 2-2-2 [Wald χ2(41) = 15.01, p = 0.99] or 1-1-1 IPMs [Wald χ2(11) = 0.69, p > 0.99]. This 1-0-1 model with one general a and one general e factor could be further reduced by dropping non-significant parameters, which included the loading for cannabis use on the general e factor; drug-specific a factors for prescription stimulant misuse and all illicit drug use phenotypes; and drug-specific c factors for all prescription misuse phenotypes and for heroin, cocaine/crack, hallucinogen, and dissociative use [Wald χ2(17) = 19.80, p = 0.28]. However, drug-specific a and c factors could not be completely dropped from the model [Wald χ2(22) = 98.42, p < 0.001], confirming appreciable magnitude of drug-specific effects. Drug-specific e factors were retained for all phenotypes, as it is unlikely that they were measured without error. The reduced 1-0-1 model provided good fit to the data [χ2(457) = 794.97; RMSEA = 0.019 (95% CI 0.017–0.021); TLI = 0.990; CFI = 0.990].

Table 2. Fix indices for independent pathway (IP) and common pathway (CP) models of drug (mis)use

Note: Numbers in IP model names correspond to the model's number of general a-c-e factors; numbers in CP model names correspond to the number of intermediate, latent general factors.

A one-factor CPM also provided good fit to the data [χ2(438) = 761.05; RMSEA = 0.019 (95% CI 0.017–0.021); TLI = 0.991; CFI = 0.990]. All non-significant parameters could be dropped without significant decrement in model fit [Wald χ2(11) = 1.64, p = 0.99]; this included drug-specific a factors for heroin, cocaine/crack, illicit stimulant, hallucinogen, solvent, and dissociative use, and drug-specific c factors for prescription opioid, stimulant, and sedative misuse, cannabis use, and inhalant use. Drug-specific a and c factors could not be completely dropped from the model [Wald χ2(22) = 405.27, p < 0.001], again confirming appreciable magnitude of drug-specific effects. As in the IPM, drug-specific e factors were retained for all phenotypes. The reduced CPM fit the data well [χ2(449) = 766.42; RMSEA = 0.018 (95% CI 0.016–0.021); TLI = 0.991; CFI = 0.990].

Overall, the reduced 1-0-1 IPM and the reduced one-factor CPM provided comparable fit to the data, with nearly identical RMSEA, TLI, and CFI values. Model preference in this case is not clear. It has been argued that the IPM is the superior model, given that it makes fewer assumptions (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott and Neale2003); conversely, it has been argued that the CPM is the superior model, as it estimates fewer parameters and is therefore more parsimonious (Tsuang et al., Reference Tsuang, Lyons, Meyer, Doyle, Eisen, Goldberg and Eaves1998). As such, both models are presented below.

Model results

Independent pathway model. Variance estimates from the best-fit IPM (reduced 1-0-1), directly interpretable as the proportion of variance in each manifest trait attributable to each factor, are depicted in Fig. 2 (standardized coefficients and confidence intervals are available in online Supplementary Table S4). For POM, 55% of the variance was attributable to general factors, primarily through the general e factor (41% of the phenotypic variance); for heroin use, 79% of the variance was attributable to general factors, primarily through the general a factor (64% of the phenotypic variance). The general a factor contributed significantly to the total variance in all drug use phenotypes (14–80%), most weakly for POM (14%) and most strongly for prescription stimulant misuse (80%). Drug-specific a influence emerged only for POM (39% of the phenotypic variance) and prescription sedative misuse (30% of the phenotypic variance); the drug-specific a parameter for all other phenotypes, including heroin use, could be constrained to 0. Drug-specific c influences ranged from 9% of the phenotypic variance for illicit stimulant use to 51% of the phenotypic variance for solvent use. The general e factor contributed substantially to the total variance in POM (41%) and sedative misuse (41%), and modestly to the total variance in remaining phenotypes (3–15%). Drug-specific e influences (and error) also contributed modestly to all phenotypes (6–22%) except prescription sedative misuse (<1%).

Fig. 2. Proportion of variance in lifetime drug use attributable to general and drug-specific additive genetic (a), common environmental (c), and unique environmental (e) factors in the best-fit independent pathway model.

Note: Bold font indicates significant parameter estimate, p < 0.001; variance components may not sum to 1 due to rounding error; variances of residual components were set to 1 (not depicted).

Common pathway model. Variance estimates from the best-fit one-factor CPM, again directly interpretable as the proportion of variance accounted for, are depicted in Fig. 3 (standardized coefficients and confidence intervals are available in online Supplementary Table S5). Variation in the general phenotypic factor was 47% attributable to a, 34% attributable to c, and 19% attributable to e. Variation in drug (mis)use phenotypes attributable to the general phenotypic factor ranged from 25% for POM to 84% for cocaine/crack and illicit stimulant use; 80% of the phenotypic variance in heroin use was attributable to the general factor. The remaining variance in POM was attributable to drug-specific a (26%) and e (50%) influences; the remaining variance in heroin use was attributable to modest drug-specific effects of c (13%) and e (7%). Drug-specific a influences also emerged for prescription sedative misuse (22%), cannabis use (28%), and inhalant use (24%). Consistent with the IPM, drug-specific c influences only emerged for illicit drug use phenotypes (8–48%), including a modest contribution for heroin use (13%). Drug-specific e influences again contributed modestly to most drug use (2–23%), including heroin use (7%), but quite substantially for POM (50%) and prescription sedative misuse (33%).

Fig. 3. Proportion of variance in lifetime drug use attributable to a latent general factor and drug-specific additive genetic (a), common environmental (c), and unique environmental (e) factors in the best-fit common pathway model.

Note: Bold font indicates significant parameter at p < 0.001; italic font indicates significant parameter at p < 0.05; variance components may not sum to 1 due to rounding error; variances of residual components were set to 1 (not depicted).

Supplemental models

Because prevalence rates across most drug (mis)use phenotypes differed across men and women, supplementary IPM and CPM analyses with sex included as a covariate were run. Variance estimates are presented in online Supplementary Table S6. Sex accounted for as little as 0% of the variance (POM, prescription sedative misuse) and at most 5% of the variance (inhalant use) in drug (mis)use phenotypes across models. The overall pattern of results was essentially unchanged.

Discussion

The present study aimed to explore specificity in genetic and environmental risk for POM and heroin use within a multivariate twin framework. Most twin studies using this approach have utilized an aggregate opioid use phenotype that combines POM and heroin use, potentially obscuring unique influences on each opioid type (Dash et al., Reference Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey and Slutske2022; Karkowski et al., Reference Karkowski, Prescott and Kendler2000; Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Karkowski and Prescott1999, Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott and Neale2003; Tsuang et al., Reference Tsuang, Lyons, Meyer, Doyle, Eisen, Goldberg and Eaves1998). Here, we aimed to identify whether and to what degree POM and heroin use are influenced by distinct etiologic influences and to explore how genetic and environmental influences on POM and heroin use are situated within a broader constellation of drug (mis)use behaviors. Results did not provide definitive support for the IPM v. the CPM, which is consistent with mixed findings regarding the relative fit of these models in multivariate twin studies of drug use (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott and Neale2003; Tsuang et al., Reference Tsuang, Lyons, Meyer, Doyle, Eisen, Goldberg and Eaves1998). However, the conclusions regarding general and specific genetic and environmental influences on POM and heroin use were quite similar across the IPM and CPM. Though their assumptions regarding the nature of shared influences differ, both models identified substantial drug-specific additive genetic variance in POM (26–39% of the total phenotypic variance; 69–71% of the total genetic variance) that was not similarly present for heroin use (0% of the total phenotypic variance in both models; 0% of the total genetic variance in both models), the latter of which was primarily accounted for by variance shared with other drug use (79–80% of the total phenotypic variance). This supports past findings of smaller genetic and environmental correlations between POM and heroin use than would perhaps be expected (Dash et al., Reference Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey and Slutske2022).

Also notable were the patterns of effect for prescription sedative misuse, which largely mirrored those of POM. Both POM and prescription sedative misuse emerged with relatively low loadings on general factors shared across drugs and more robust drug-specific influences (45–76% and 30–55% of the total phenotypic variance in POM and prescription sedative misuse, respectively). Such findings suggest that, despite a lack of support for differentiable prescription and illicit use factors, there is something unique about the etiology of prescription misuse behaviors, at least for drugs that act as ‘downers.’ It is not clear why prescription stimulant misuse did not adhere to this pattern, with only 18–21% of the phenotypic variance being drug-specific and predominantly attributable to unique environment. One reason may be differences in subjective drug effects that result in distinct instrumental reasons for misuse of prescription ‘downers’ such as opioids and sedatives v. prescription ‘uppers’ such as stimulants. For example, individual differences such as personality may facilitate use of particular drugs; studies have shown that neuroticism and hopelessness predict opioid and sedative use, while extraversion and sensation-seeking predict stimulant use (Dash, Martin, & Slutske, Reference Dash, Martin and Slutske2021b; Mahu et al., Reference Mahu, Conrod, Barrett, Sako, Swansburg, Lawrence and Nogueira-Arjona2019). Prescription stimulants are also used more frequently in a manner consistent with illicit drug use (e.g. intranasal administration) (Butler et al., Reference Butler, Faraone, Rostain, Newcorn, Antshel, Robbins and Green2021; Wilens et al., Reference Wilens, Adler, Adams, Sgambati, Rotrosen, Sawtelle and Fusillo2008), which may indicate that prescription stimulant misuse aligns more closely with use of other illicit drugs than with other prescription misuse behaviors.

More broadly, the finding that variance shared across drugs could be modeled as a single factor, as opposed to smaller, separable clusters, is consistent with phenotypic and genotypic findings on the factor structure of substance use and use disorder – and psychopathology more broadly – that have identified an overarching factor accounting for overlap across phenotypes (Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel and Poulton2014; Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2018; Hatoum et al., Reference Hatoum, Johnson, Colbert, Polimanti, Zhou, Walters and Agrawal2022; Hicks, Schalet, Malone, Iacono, & McGue, Reference Hicks, Schalet, Malone, Iacono and McGue2011; Sanchez-Roige, Kember, & Agrawal, Reference Sanchez-Roige, Kember and Agrawal2022). These findings are also consistent with a past twin study of drug use that found genetic variance shared between opioids, sedatives, cannabis, cocaine, stimulants, and hallucinogens to be captured by a single factor (Kendler et al., Reference Kendler, Jacobson, Prescott and Neale2003). Though a twin study testing licit and illicit factors conducted by Kendler et al. (Reference Kendler, Myers and Prescott2007) identified a licit factor comprised of alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine dependence symptoms and an illicit factor comprised of cannabis and cocaine dependence symptoms, there are plausible explanations as to why this two-factor pattern did not replicate for the prescription-illicit configuration tested here. Alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine are more widely available, more commonly used, and, among adults, universally legal in the place and time of data collection (Australian Department of Health and Aged Care, 2021; Australia Department of Health and Aged Care, 2022; SAMHSA, 2021). These characteristics of ‘licit use’ as operationalized by Kendler et al. (Reference Kendler, Myers and Prescott2007) may have contributed to a starker contrast between licit and illicit use than would be observed between prescription misuse and illicit use. That is, differences between prescription misuse and illicit use may be more subtle so as to be insufficient to form clearly separable factors. However, the pattern of results for POM and prescription sedative misuse suggests that the present study may have been underpowered to detect the uniqueness of the prescription misuse phenotypes. Future research in larger datasets may be equipped to shed further light on this pattern.

Overall, the finding that POM may share little genetic influence with heroin use has several implications for opioid research and policy. First, such a pattern suggests that disaggregating POM and heroin use could increase the replicability of findings across samples with varying proportions of POM v. heroin use (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Zhou, Sherva, Farrer, Kranzler and Gelernter2018; Dash et al., Reference Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey and Slutske2022). Second, the presence of POM-specific genetic influence unshared with heroin use has important implications for case–control selection in genomic studies of opioid use disorder (OUD), which typically do not differentiate POM-exposed individuals from heroin-exposed individuals, nor POM- v. heroin-based OUD (Cheng et al., Reference Cheng, Zhou, Sherva, Farrer, Kranzler and Gelernter2018; Dash et al., Reference Dash, Martin, Agrawal, Lynskey and Slutske2022; Gelernter et al., Reference Gelernter, Kranzler, Sherva, Koesterer, Almasy, Zhao and Farrer2014; Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Rentsch, Cheng, Kember, Nunez, Sherva and Polimanti2020). This is also salient considering that heroin-exposed individuals tend to progress to OUD at higher rates than POM-exposed individuals (Wu et al., Reference Wu, Woody, Yang and Blazer2011). Thus, POM- and heroin-involved individuals could be differentially represented among cases and exposed controls, thereby introducing potential uncontrolled confounds into such studies. Third, if POM and heroin use have little etiologic overlap, public health efforts seeking to mitigate the impact of heroin and other illicit opioid use by reducing opioid prescribing rates are unlikely to be an optimal approach. Indeed, overdose deaths due to opioids have not dropped in parallel with declines in opioid prescribing, and there are increasing calls to shift efforts toward addressing the root social and economic causes of the opioid crisis (Dasgupta, Beletsky, & Ciccarone, Reference Dasgupta, Beletsky and Ciccarone2018; Mattson et al., Reference Mattson, Tanz, Quinn, Kariisa, Patel and Davis2021).

Limitations

The results of the present study should be interpreted in light of limitations. First, it is not clear how findings from this Australian sample will generalize to other countries and cultures. The sample was comprised primarily of individuals of European ancestry and data on self-identified race and ethnicity were not available. Additionally, the data collection period for a subset of the present sample occurred very early in the rise of the global opioid crisis, potentially limiting generalizability to the contemporary public health landscape. It is also important to note here that heritability estimates are specific to a particular population at a given point in time; it will be important to collect novel data so as to examine this topic in more recent samples, as evidence suggests that heritability of substance use can vary over time as a function of environmental factors such as changes in drug accessibility (Boardman, Reference Boardman2009; Slutske, Deutsch, & Piasecki, Reference Slutske, Deutsch and Piasecki2019a; Slutske, Piasecki, Deutsch, Statham, & Martin, Reference Slutske, Piasecki, Deutsch, Statham and Martin2019b) and social acceptability (Kendler, Thornton, & Pedersen, Reference Kendler, Thornton and Pedersen2000b; Mezquita et al., Reference Mezquita, Sánchez-Romera, Ibáñez, Morosoli, Colodro-Conde, Ortet and Ordoñana2018). Second, we were unable to conduct multivariate models in sex-specific zygosity groups due to sparse endorsement of the phenotypes of interest, though supplemental models revealed minimal differences between models with and without sex included as a covariate. Similarly, given low endorsement of heroin use in particular, the present analyses may have been underpowered. There were more illicit use phenotypes than prescription misuse phenotypes and therefore an imbalance in the number of indicators per factor in models with a two-factor configuration, which may have also impacted model fitting. Finally, we were restricted to binary indicators of lifetime drug use. Despite these limitations, the present study provides novel insight into the specificity (or lack thereof) in genetic and environmental influences on POM and heroin use, as well as several other drug (mis)use behaviors.

Conclusions

POM and heroin use are often aggregated into a single ‘opioid use’ variable, but the global health burden of opioid consumption has highlighted the potential need for examining these behaviors as distinct phenotypes. The present study suggests that POM is non-negligibly attributable to additive genetic influences not shared with heroin use or use of other illicit drugs. Behavior genetic research, and opioid research more broadly, may consider implementing finer-grained assessment and operationalization of opioid use, including deep phenotyping, that can more effectively and consistently differentiate POM and heroin use behaviors, as well as different manifestations of such behaviors (e.g. more than prescribed v. when not prescribed), so as to avoid overlooking potentially important characteristics unique to each opioid form and the manner in which they are used. Doing so may have public health implications of critical importance germane to effectively addressing the opioid crisis via public policy, and may help to explain the underwhelming effectiveness of opioid prescribing restrictions on reducing the population-level impact of opioid use and related mortality.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172300034X.

Financial support

This research was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse grant numbers F31DA054701 (PI: Genevieve F. Dash) and R01DA18267 (PI: Michael T. Lynskey) and the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism grant number R37AA007728 (PI: Andrew C. Heath).

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 107(2), 238246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boardman, J. D. (2009). State-level moderation of genetic tendencies to smoke. American Journal of Public Health, 99(3), 480486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bucholz, K. K., Cadoret, R., Cloninger, C. R., Dinwiddie, S. H., Hesselbrock, V., Nurnberger, J. Jr., … Schuckit, M. A. (1994). A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: A report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 55(2), 149158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, S. F., Faraone, S. V., Rostain, A. L., Newcorn, J. H., Antshel, K. M., Robbins, R. S., & Green, J. L. (2021). Non-medical use of prescription stimulants among college students: Non-oral routes of administration, risk factors, motivations, and pathways. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 667118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., … Poulton, R. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2018). All for one and one for all: Mental disorders in one dimension. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(9), 831844.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, F., Curran, P. J., Bollen, K. A., Kirby, J., & Paxton, P. (2008). An empirical evaluation of the use of fixed cutoff points in RMSEA test statistic in structural equation models. Sociological Methods & Research, 36(4), 462494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, Z., Yang, B. Z., Zhou, H., Nunez, Y., Kranzler, H. R., & Gelernter, J. (2020). Genome-wide scan identifies opioid overdose risk locus close to MCOLN1. Addiction Biology, 25(2), e12811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, Z., Zhou, H., Sherva, R., Farrer, L. A., Kranzler, H. R., & Gelernter, J. (2018). Genome-wide association study identifies a regulatory variant of RGMA associated with opioid dependence in European Americans. Biological Psychiatry, 84(10), 762770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crist, R. C., Reiner, B. C., & Berrettini, W. H. (2019). A review of opioid addiction genetics. Current Opinion in Psychology, 27, 3135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dasgupta, N., Beletsky, L., & Ciccarone, D. (2018). Opioid crisis: No easy fix to its social and economic determinants. American Journal of Public Health, 108(2), 182186.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dash, G. F., Martin, N. G., Agrawal, A., Lynskey, M. T., & Slutske, W. S. (2021a). Typologies of illicit drug use in mid-adulthood: A quasi-longitudinal latent class analysis in a community-based sample of twins. Addiction, 116(5), 11011112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dash, G. F., Martin, N. G., Agrawal, A., Lynskey, M. T., & Slutske, W. S. (2022). Are prescription misuse and illicit drug up etiologically distinct? A genetically-informed analysis of opioids and stimulants. Psychological Medicine, 54(14), 31763183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dash, G. F., Martin, N. G., & Slutske, W. S. (2021b). Big Five personality traits and illicit drug use: Specificity in trait–drug associations. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/adb0000793.Google ScholarPubMed
Deak, J. D., Zhou, H., Galimberti, M., Levey, D. F., Wendt, F. R., Sanchez-Roige, S., … Demontis, D. (2022). Genome-wide association study in individuals of European and African ancestry and multi-trait analysis of opioid use disorder identifies 19 independent genome-wide significant risk loci. Molecular Psychiatry, 27, 39703979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Department of Health and Aged Care. (2021, August 11). Smoking and tobacco laws in Australia. Retrieved from https://www.health.gov.au/health-topics/smoking-and-tobacco/about-smoking-and-tobacco/smoking-and-tobacco-laws-in-australia.Google Scholar
Department of Health and Aged Care. (2022, March 1). Alcohol laws in Australia. Retrieved from https://www.health.gov.au/health-topics/alcohol/about-alcohol/alcohol-laws-in-australia.Google Scholar
Gelernter, J., Kranzler, H. R., Sherva, R., Koesterer, R., Almasy, L., Zhao, H., & Farrer, L. A. (2014). Genome-wide association study of opioid dependence: Multiple associations mapped to calcium and potassium pathways. Biological Psychiatry, 76(1), 6674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillespie, N. A., Bates, T. C., Hickie, I. B., Medland, S. E., Verhulst, B., Kirkpatrick, R. M., … Benotsch, E. G. (2019). Genetic and environmental risk factors in the non-medical use of over-the-counter or prescribed analgesics, and their relationship to major classes of licit and illicit substance use and misuse in a population-based sample of young adult twins. Addiction, 114(12), 22292240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatoum, A. S., Johnson, E. C., Colbert, S., Polimanti, R., Zhou, H., Walters, R. K., … Agrawal, A. (2022). The addiction risk factor: A unitary genetic vulnerability characterizes substance use disorders and their associations with common correlates. Neuropsychopharmacology, 47(10), 17391745.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hicks, B. M., Schalet, B. D., Malone, S. M., Iacono, W. G., & McGue, M. (2011). Psychometric and genetic architecture of substance use disorder and behavioral disinhibition measures for gene association studies. Behavior Genetics, 41(4), 459475.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. (2008). Evaluating model fit: A synthesis of the structural equation modelling literature. Paper presented at the 7th European Conference on research methodology for business and management studies.Google Scholar
Hu, L. t., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6(1), 155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, K. P. (2016). A review of genome-wide association studies of stimulant and opioid use disorders. Molecular Neuropsychiatry, 2(1), 3745.Google ScholarPubMed
Karkowski, L. M., Prescott, C. A., & Kendler, K. S. (2000). Multivariate assessment of factors influencing illicit substance use in twins from female-female pairs. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 96(5), 665670.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Aggen, S. H., Tambs, K., & Reichborn-Kjennerud, T. (2006). Illicit psychoactive substance use, abuse and dependence in a population-based sample of Norwegian twins. Psychological Medicine, 36(7), 955.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Jacobson, K. C., Prescott, C. A., & Neale, M. C. (2003). Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for use and abuse/dependence of cannabis, cocaine, hallucinogens, sedatives, stimulants, and opiates in male twins. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(4), 687695.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L., & Prescott, C.A. (1999). Hallucinogen, opiate, sedative and stimulant use and abuse in a population-based sample of female twins. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 99(5), 368376.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Karkowski, L. M., Neale, M. C., & Prescott, C. A. (2000a). Illicit psychoactive substance use, heavy use, abuse, and dependence in a US population-based sample of male twins. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(3), 261269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., Myers, J., & Prescott, C. A. (2007). Specificity of genetic and environmental risk factors for symptoms of cannabis, cocaine, alcohol, caffeine, and nicotine dependence. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(11), 13131320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2000b). Tobacco consumption in Swedish twins reared apart and reared together. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57(9), 886892.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mahu, I., Conrod, P., Barrett, S., Sako, A., Swansburg, J., Lawrence, M., … Nogueira-Arjona, R. (2019). Specificity of personality relationships to particular forms of concurrent substance use among methadone maintenance therapy clients. Addictive Behaviors, 98, 106056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mattson, C. L., Tanz, L. J., Quinn, K., Kariisa, M., Patel, P., & Davis, N. L. (2021). Trends and geographic patterns in drug and synthetic opioid overdose deaths – United States, 2013–2019. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 70(6), 202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mezquita, L., Sánchez-Romera, J. F., Ibáñez, M. I., Morosoli, J. J., Colodro-Conde, L., Ortet, G., & Ordoñana, J. R. (2018). Effects of social attitude change on smoking heritability. Behavior Genetics, 48(1), 1221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muthén, L. (2017). Mplus users guide (version 8). Los Angeles, CA, USA: Muthén & Muthén.Google Scholar
Neale, M., & Cardon, L. R. (2013). Methodology for genetic studies of twins and families, Vol. 67). Dordrecht: Springer Science & Business Media.Google Scholar
Polimanti, R., Walters, R. K., Johnson, E. C., McClintick, J. N., Adkins, A. E., Adkins, D. E., … Brown, S. (2020). Leveraging genome-wide data to investigate differences between opioid use vs. opioid dependence in 41,176 individuals from the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Molecular Psychiatry, 25, 16731687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, B., & Kreek, M. J. (2021). Genetic vulnerability to opioid addiction. Cold Spring Harbor Perspectives in Medicine, 11, a039735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rigg, K. K., & Monnat, S. M. (2015). Comparing characteristics of prescription painkiller misusers and heroin users in the United States. Addictive Behaviors, 51, 106112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanchez-Roige, S., Kember, R. L., & Agrawal, A. (2022). Substance use and common contributors to morbidity: A genetics perspective. Ebiomedicine, 83, 104212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slutske, W. S., Deutsch, A. R., & Piasecki, T. M. (2019a). Neighborhood alcohol outlet density and genetic influences on alcohol use: Evidence for gene–environment interaction. Psychological Medicine, 49(3), 474482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slutske, W. S., Piasecki, T. M., Deutsch, A. R., Statham, D. J., & Martin, N. G. (2019b). Potential causal influence of neighborhood disadvantage on disordered gambling: Evidence from a multilevel discordant twin design. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(3), 582596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steiger, J. H. (1990). Structural model evaluation and modification: An interval estimation approach. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 25(2), 173180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. (2021). Key substance use and mental health indicators in the United States: Results from the 2020 National Survey on Drug Use and Health (HHS Publication No. PEP21-07-01-003, NSDUH Series H-56). Rockville, MD: Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality, Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration. Retrieved from https://www.samhsa.gov/data/.Google Scholar
Tsuang, M. T., Lyons, M. J., Meyer, J. M., Doyle, T., Eisen, S. A., Goldberg, J., … Eaves, L. (1998). Co-occurrence of abuse of different drugs in men: The role of drug-specific and shared vulnerabilities. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55(11), 967972.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucker, L. R., & Lewis, C. (1973). A reliability coefficient for maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika, 38(1), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Bree, M. B., Johnson, E. O., Neale, M. C., & Pickens, R. W. (1998). Genetic and environmental influences on drug use and abuse/dependence in male and female twins. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 52(3), 231241.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilens, T. E., Adler, L. A., Adams, J., Sgambati, S., Rotrosen, J., Sawtelle, R., … Fusillo, S. (2008). Misuse and diversion of stimulants prescribed for ADHD: A systematic review of the literature. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(1), 2131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, L. T., Woody, G. E., Yang, C., & Blazer, D. G. (2011). How do prescription opioid users differ from users of heroin or other drugs in psychopathology: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Journal of Addiction Medicine, 5(1), 2835.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, C.-Y. (2002). Evaluating cutoff criteria of model fit indices for latent variable models with binary and continuous outcomes. Los Angeles: University of California.Google Scholar
Zhou, H., Rentsch, C. T., Cheng, Z., Kember, R. L., Nunez, Y. Z., Sherva, R. M., … Polimanti, R. (2020). Association of OPRM1 functional coding variant with opioid use disorder: A genome-wide association study. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(10), 10721080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Proportion of variance in lifetime drug use attributable to additive genetic (a), common environmental (c), and unique environmental (e) factors in univariate models.Note: Parameters could be constrained to equality across men and women in all models except cannabis use, for which a and c parameters could be individually, but not simultaneously, constrained [Wald χ2(2) = 8.02, p = 0.02]; a, c, and e estimates from the freely estimated model were 31, 33, and 37% for men and 51, 27, and 22% for women (estimates from the constrained cannabis use model are presented for consistency).

Figure 1

Table 1. Cross-trait correlations for prescription opioid misuse, heroin use, and other drug (mis)use derived from bivariate biometric models

Figure 2

Table 2. Fix indices for independent pathway (IP) and common pathway (CP) models of drug (mis)use

Figure 3

Fig. 2. Proportion of variance in lifetime drug use attributable to general and drug-specific additive genetic (a), common environmental (c), and unique environmental (e) factors in the best-fit independent pathway model.Note: Bold font indicates significant parameter estimate, p < 0.001; variance components may not sum to 1 due to rounding error; variances of residual components were set to 1 (not depicted).

Figure 4

Fig. 3. Proportion of variance in lifetime drug use attributable to a latent general factor and drug-specific additive genetic (a), common environmental (c), and unique environmental (e) factors in the best-fit common pathway model.Note: Bold font indicates significant parameter at p < 0.001; italic font indicates significant parameter at p < 0.05; variance components may not sum to 1 due to rounding error; variances of residual components were set to 1 (not depicted).

Supplementary material: File

Dash et al. supplementary material

Tables S1-S6 and Figures S1-S2

Download Dash et al. supplementary material(File)
File 176 KB