Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T14:35:51.615Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Virtual reality in the assessment, understanding, and treatment of mental health disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 March 2017

D. Freeman*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
S. Reeve
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
A. Robinson
Affiliation:
Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
A. Ehlers
Affiliation:
Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
D. Clark
Affiliation:
Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
B. Spanlang
Affiliation:
Event Lab, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
M. Slater
Affiliation:
Event Lab, Department of Clinical Psychology and Psychobiology, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain Institució Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avançats (ICREA), Barcelona, Spain
*
*Address for correspondence: D. Freeman, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK. (Email: Daniel.Freeman@psych.ox.ac.uk)
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Mental health problems are inseparable from the environment. With virtual reality (VR), computer-generated interactive environments, individuals can repeatedly experience their problematic situations and be taught, via evidence-based psychological treatments, how to overcome difficulties. VR is moving out of specialist laboratories. Our central aim was to describe the potential of VR in mental health, including a consideration of the first 20 years of applications. A systematic review of empirical studies was conducted. In all, 285 studies were identified, with 86 concerning assessment, 45 theory development, and 154 treatment. The main disorders researched were anxiety (n = 192), schizophrenia (n = 44), substance-related disorders (n = 22) and eating disorders (n = 18). There are pioneering early studies, but the methodological quality of studies was generally low. The gaps in meaningful applications to mental health are extensive. The most established finding is that VR exposure-based treatments can reduce anxiety disorders, but there are numerous research and treatment avenues of promise. VR was found to be a much-misused term, often applied to non-interactive and non-immersive technologies. We conclude that VR has the potential to transform the assessment, understanding and treatment of mental health problems. The treatment possibilities will only be realized if – with the user experience at the heart of design – the best immersive VR technology is combined with targeted translational interventions. The capability of VR to simulate reality could greatly increase access to psychological therapies, while treatment outcomes could be enhanced by the technology's ability to create new realities. VR may merit the level of attention given to neuroimaging.

Type
Invited Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2017

Introduction

A technological revolution in mental health care is approaching. At the forefront may be virtual reality (VR), a powerful tool for individuals to make new learning for the benefit of their psychological well-being. Immersive VR creates interactive computer-generated worlds, which substitute real-world sensory perceptions with digitally generated ones, producing the sensation of actually being in life-sized new environments. VR allows such tight control over the stimuli presented that therapeutic strategies can be precisely implemented; VR can produce situations that can be therapeutically helpful if used in the right way but near impossible to recreate in real life; VR allows repeated, immediately available and greater treatment input; and VR can reduce inconsistency of treatment delivery. With high-quality VR devices reaching the consumer market for the first time, the future is suddenly imminent. The affordability makes it feasible for the technology to break out of the laboratory and enter the home – and forward-thinking mental health clinics too.

VR

The basic elements of VR – a computer generating an image, a display system presenting the sensory information, and a tracker feeding back the user's position and orientation in order to update the image – have existed for 50 years. The hardware recognizable today emerged in the 1980s but has been largely confined to specialist laboratories (Slater & Sanchez-Vives, Reference Slater and Sanchez-Vives2016). Systems vary greatly. For example, the Cave Automatic Virtual Environment (CAVE) projects computer images onto the walls of a room and the participant wears tracked shutter glasses to view the scene three-dimensionally (Cruz-Neira et al. Reference Cruz-Neira, Sandin and DeFanti1993). Sometimes described as VR are much lower-specification systems that use displays on computer monitors or large projector screens, but the limited levels of immersion and interaction make it questionable whether these are truly VR. The current excitement relates to the new generation of head-mounted display (HMD) and associated equipment that have emerged as affordable consumer products due to the investment of global companies. Smartphones, laptops or desktop computers can run the software. VR is moving out of specialist laboratories. The transformation in what the hardware and software can now realize compared with even a few years ago is great.

An HMD displays images, one for each eye, forming an overall stereo scene. Each image is computed and rendered separately with correct perspective from the position of each eye with respect to a mathematical description of a three-dimensional (3D) virtual scene. The HMD is typically tracked, with continuous capturing of the position and orientation of the participant's head and therefore head-based gaze direction. As participants turn or move their head to look around, the computer updates at a very high frame rate – typically 60 frames per second – the images displayed. Therefore participants see a surrounding 3D stereo scene that can change dynamically. Although much VR programming has omitted it, one particular object in the scene can have special status – the virtual body of the participant. At its simplest, this visual substitution of the person's real body can be aligned to the head tracking. But if the participant wears a motion-tracking capture suit with an HMD, then the data from this, continuously streamed to the computer, will effectively substitute the body of the participant by a life-sized virtual body that fully moves in correspondence with their own movements, leading to the illusion of body ownership (Slater et al. Reference Slater, Spanlang, Sanchez-Vives and Blanke2010; Spanlang et al. Reference Spanlang, Normand, Borland, Kilteni, Giannopoulos, Pomés, González-Franco, Perez-Marcos, Arroyo-Palacios, Muncunill and Slater2014).

Perception through natural movement is the key element of an immersive VR system. Immersion reflects the system's technical capabilities; the subjective experience delivered is termed ‘presence’, which is the illusion of being in the place rendered by VR. Presence comprises two concepts: place illusion (PI) and plausibility illusion (Psi) (Slater, Reference Slater2009). PI is the sense of being in the virtual place. A necessary condition for PI is that the VR is perceived through natural sensorimotor contingencies, based on the active vision paradigm (Noë, Reference Noë2004). The idea of this paradigm is that we perceive through using our whole body, via a set of implicit rules involving head turning, leaning, reaching, looking around and so on. The illusion of ‘being there’ is generated to the extent that the VR system affords perception through such contingencies. If what we see matches our movements then the brain's conclusion is that these are our surroundings. The Psi is the sense that the events experienced in VR are happening (e.g. that there are people walking about, that a ball is flying through the air), even though, of course, individuals consciously know that these are not real. Psi requires that the virtual environment responds to actions of the participants, generates spontaneous actions towards them, and is ecologically valid when real-life events are depicted. For example, when the environment includes virtual human characters, these avatars should respond to the presence and actions of the participants (e.g. by gaze and maintaining appropriate interpersonal distances). When both PI and Psi operate, participants will be likely to behave realistically in VR.

VR and mental health

VR has extraordinary potential to help people overcome mental health problems if high levels of presence are achieved for situations that trouble them. Difficulties interacting in the world are at the heart of mental health issues [e.g. becoming highly anxious near spiders in arachnophobia, having intense flashbacks with reminders of past trauma in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), fearing attack from people in persecutory delusions, resisting the urge to take another drink in alcohol abuse disorders]. Therefore recovery concerns thinking, reacting and behaving differently in these situations. The most successful interventions are those that enable people to make such changes in real-world situations. With VR, individuals can enter simulations of the difficult situations and be coached in the appropriate responses, based upon the best theoretical understanding of the specific disorder. The simulations can be graded in difficulty and repeatedly experienced until the right learning is made. Problematic situations difficult to find in real life can be realized at the flick of a switch. And the great advantage of VR is that individuals know that a computer environment is not real but their minds and bodies behave as if it is real; hence, people will much more easily face difficult situations in VR than in real life and be able to try out new therapeutic strategies. The learning can then transfer to the real world. For some disorders it may be possible to eradicate the need for any therapist input, while for other disorders the time required of skilled therapists could be greatly reduced. Thus VR could help improve access to the most effective psychological treatments. It may become the method of choice for psychological treatment: out with the couch, on with the headset.

There are also many other potential uses of VR in mental health. We originally set out seven purposes (Freeman, Reference Freeman2008): symptom assessment, identification of symptom markers or correlates, establishment of factors predictive of disorders, tests of putative causal factors, investigation of the differential prediction of symptoms, determination of toxic elements in the environment, and the development of treatment. For instance, standard mental health diagnosis chiefly comprises retrospective recall using clinician interview and validated questionnaires. Inevitably, human beings tend to be very subjective in their views. Memory, moreover, is notoriously fallible. In the clinic of the future, it is possible that problems could also be assessed live in VR. The technology could also help make substantial inroads into understanding the causes of mental health disorders, for example, pinpointing the environmental characteristics that raise the risk of adverse psychological reactions in the context of individual differences.

The aim of this paper is to highlight for clinicians and researchers in mental health the potential of VR technology. This includes a review of what has been learned empirically from the first generation of studies about the use of VR in assessing, understanding and treating the main adult mental health disorders. We wished to identify established findings, obvious areas of neglect and new directions of interest. These are the studies that have been conducted in specialist laboratories over the past 20 years, before the current transformation in availability and capabilities of the technology. It is the ambition of these pioneering studies that we aim to capture, as new hardware and software are dramatically altering what can be created in VR and the ease of use.

Method

The literature on VR in mental health was searched up to the end of 2016. The inclusion criteria were: a specific focus on the assessment, theory, or treatment of adult mental health disorders; published in a peer-reviewed journal; was an empirical study (including case studies with data); used a form of immersive VR (HMD, CAVE, large projection screen, screen with 3D glasses); and in the English language. The exclusion criteria were: non-immersive VR method [e.g. personal computer (PC) screen only, websites such as Second Life]; qualitative data or review; unable to obtain the paper; use of VR but no specific focus on a mental health symptom or condition. We did not look at health psychology, cognitive disorders, personality disorders, childhood-onset disorders, or at the effects of VR games that were not designed as interventions.

PubMed was used for the searches, which were conducted separately by major disorder types. The general search terms were: [([Virtual reality OR Immersive virtual reality] AND [Assessment OR treatment OR research OR study OR experiment OR understanding]) AND (disorder-specific terms inserted here)] AND English (language). For anxiety disorders, the search terms added were: (Anx* OR obsessive-compulsive OR post-traumatic OR panic OR social phobia OR social anxiety OR phob*OR GAD OR OCD OR PTSD OR SAD). For depression, the search terms added were: (depression OR depress*). For psychosis the search terms added were: (Delus*OR Hallucinat* OR Psychosis OR Psychotic OR Schizophren* OR Schizotyp* OR Bipolar OR Mania OR Manic). For substance disorders, the search terms added were: (substance disorder OR substance abuse OR substance OR abuse OR cannabis OR tobacco OR alcohol OR amphetamine OR hallucinogens OR heroin). For eating disorders, the search terms added were: AND (anorexia nervosa OR bulimia nervosa OR eating disorders OR binge eating). For sleep disorders, the added terms were: (insomnia OR sleep OR nightmares OR circadian). For sexual disorders, the added search terms were: (sexual OR orgasm OR desire OR erectile OR ejaculation OR dyspareunia). Titles and abstracts were read, and, if appropriate, the whole paper, in order to determine whether the inclusion and exclusion criteria were met.

Results

A total of 1096 studies were identified from the literature searches, of which 285 met the review inclusion criteria. Summaries of the searches by each of the disorder types are displayed in the online Supplementary Figs S1–S7. Descriptions of the individual VR studies are available in the online Supplementary Tables S1–S7. The most common reason for an empirical study to be excluded from review was the use of non-immersive technology (e.g. participants simply viewed a standard computer screen for presentation) or empirical data were not reported.

Anxiety disorders

Overwhelmingly, VR studies have concerned the treatment of anxiety disorders (n = 127 intervention reports). Even the assessment studies (n = 46) have mainly been conducted for the purpose of validating VR environments for treatment. The use of VR to investigate the causes of anxiety has been more rarely conducted (n = 19). The focus of the treatment studies has typically been on specific phobias (e.g. Rothbaum et al. Reference Rothbaum, Hodges, Smith, Lee and Price2000; Emmelkamp et al. Reference Emmelkamp, Krijn, Hulsbosch, de Vries, Schuemie and van der Mast2002; Garcia-Palacios et al. Reference Garcia-Palacios, Hoffman, Carlin, Furness and Botella2002; Botella et al. Reference Botella, Osma, Garcia-Palacios, Quero and Baños2004) or social anxiety (e.g. Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Price, Edwards, Obasaju, Schmertz, Zimand and Calamaras2013; Bouchard et al. Reference Bouchard, Dumoulin, Robillard, Guitard, Klinger, Forget, Loranger and Roucaut2016) or post-traumatic stress disorder (e.g. Difede et al. Reference Difede, Cukor, Jayasinghe, Patt, Jedel, Spielman, Giosan and Hoffman2007; Rizzo et al. Reference Rizzo, Difede, Rothbaum, Johnston, McLay, Reger, Gahm, Parsons, Graap and Pair2009), with many fewer investigations for obsessive– compulsive disorder (OCD), which is surprising given that treatment often requires change in fears about external stimuli, and generalized anxiety disorder, which is less surprising given the internal focus of the disorder. The principal intervention technique has been exposure, with a therapist present to guide the person in most of the intervention studies. The treatment studies have undoubtedly been pioneering in recognizing the potential for the technology in this treatment area especially (e.g. Hodges et al. Reference Hodges, Kooper, Meyer, Rothbaum, Opdyke, de Graaff, Williford and North1995; Rothbaum et al. Reference Rothbaum, Hodges, Watson, Kessler and Opdyke1996; Botella et al. Reference Botella, Baños, Perpiñá, Villa, Alcañiz and Rey1998). Case study reports or small randomized controlled trials have dominated the field. The quality of studies has too often been low (Meyerbröker & Emmelkamp, Reference Meyerbröker and Emmelkamp2010; McCann et al. Reference McCann, Armstrong, Skopp, Edwards-Stewart, Smolenski, June, Metzger-Abamukong and Reger2014). There are too few convincing randomized controlled trials, although this is beginning to change (e.g. Anderson et al. Reference Anderson, Price, Edwards, Obasaju, Schmertz, Zimand and Calamaras2013; Bouchard et al. Reference Bouchard, Dumoulin, Robillard, Guitard, Klinger, Forget, Loranger and Roucaut2016; Reger et al. Reference Reger, Koenen-Woods, Zetocha, Smolenski, Holloway, Rothbaum, Difede, Rizzo, Edwards-Stewart, Skopp, Mishkind, Reger and Gahm2016), too few experimental studies of potential treatment mediators (e.g. Shiban et al. Reference Shiban, Fruth, Pauli, Kinateder, Reichenberger and Mühlberger2016), and comparisons between different techniques have typically been underpowered. Overall, however, VR treatments seem to perform comparably in efficacy to face-to-face equivalent interventions. With the caveat concerning the quality of the studies, the treatment efficacy has been shown in meta-analyses to be large (e.g. Opriş et al. Reference Opriş, Pintea, García-Palacios, Botella, Szamosközi and David2012), with evidence that the beneficial effects transfer to the real world (Morina et al. Reference Morina, Ijntema, Meyerbröker and Emmelkamp2015). When long-term follow-ups have been included, treatment effects for these short-term therapies have strikingly been shown to persist over a number of years (e.g. Rothbaum et al. Reference Rothbaum, Hodges, Anderson, Price and Smith2002; Wiederhold & Wiederhold, Reference Wiederhold and Wiederhold2003). There are indications that drop-out rates may be lower with VR treatments but that may simply reflect a problem of quality control with face-to-face therapy delivery. The range of VR-type methods used has been wide, varying from large projection screens to the computer-assisted research environment (CAREN) system, in which the person has a walking platform surrounded by a 360° display, to CAVEs, flight simulators, and to HMDs. Not all reports make clear which type of technology was used. The greater the sense of presence in VR achieved then the more likely anxiety will occur (Ling et al. Reference Ling, Nefs, Morina, Heynderickx and Brinkman2014). The importance of sound in achieving presence in VR should not be overlooked (e.g. Taffou et al. Reference Taffou, Guerchouche, Drettakis and Viaud-Delmon2013). Detailed studies of how best to present stimuli in VR are warranted but in our opinion have been far too rare (e.g. Shiban et al. Reference Shiban, Schelhorn, Pauli and Mühlberger2015).

Depression

Surprisingly, we identified only two studies that clearly used immersive VR in relation to depression. These feasibility studies tested out single treatment techniques in small case series with no control conditions (Shah et al. Reference Shah, Torres, Kannusamy, Chng, He and Klainin-Yobas2015; Falconer et al. Reference Falconer, Rovira, King, Gilbert, Antley, Fearon, Ralph, Slater and Brewin2016), with levels of depression found to decrease with time. There have also been two studies of (non-immersive) VR-type tasks assessing spatial navigation memory in patients with depression (e.g. Gould et al. Reference Gould, Holmes, Fantie, Luckenbaugh, Pine, Gould, Burgess, Manji and Zarate2007).

Psychosis

There have been 44 VR studies about schizophrenia and related problems, with 23 concerning theory development, 15 concerning assessment, and six testing treatment. The types of VR studies here were probably the most heterogeneous compared with the other mental health conditions, reflecting the complexity of the clinical problem and the different perspectives taken towards diagnosing and understanding psychosis. The studies have predominately used VR to assess psychotic experiences in order to understand the causes. VR has been of particular use in assessing paranoia because the presentation of neutral social situations enables unfounded, rather than genuine, hostility to be detected. It is clear that VR can safely assess psychotic experiences in patients with schizophrenia and related diagnoses. Our group pioneered the work on VR in relation to paranoia. We have used VR to: assess paranoia (e.g. Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Slater, Bebbington, Garety, Kuipers, Fowler, Met, Read, Jordan and Vinayagamoorthy2003); understand the individual characteristics predictive of paranoia (e.g. Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Pugh, Antley, Slater, Bebbington, Gittins, Dunn, Kuipers, Fowler and Garety2008); manipulate psychological factors in order to determine the causes of paranoia (e.g. Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Evans, Lister, Antley, Dunn and Slater2014); and, most recently, treat persecutory delusions in the context of schizophrenia (Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Bradley, Antley, Bourke, DeWeever, Evans, Černis, Sheaves, Waite, Dunn, Slater and Clark2016). A rare use of VR to create a situation that cannot be achieved in real life is provided by our manipulation of a person's height in order to affect self-esteem and hence paranoia (Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Evans, Lister, Antley, Dunn and Slater2014). The small treatment study with 30 patients with persecutory delusions showed that VR cognitive therapy is potentially much more efficacious than VR exposure therapy both in terms of reducing delusions and lessening distress in real-world situations. The controlled effect size (d = 1.3) for VR cognitive therapy was large, which is notable given that it was compared with another credible treatment approach. VR has also been used to study environmental factors that make an impact on paranoia, by altering variables such as population density and ethnicity (e.g. Valmaggia et al. Reference Valmaggia, Day, Garety, Freeman, Antley, Slater, Swapp, Myin-Germeys and McGuire2015; Veling et al. Reference Veling, Pot-Kolder, Counotte, van Os and van der Gaag2016). There is also a strand of VR work assessing cognitive and social functioning in schizophrenia (e.g. Sorkin et al. Reference Sorkin, Weinshall, Modai and Peled2006) and consequent intervention (e.g. Rus-Calafell et al. Reference Rus-Calafell, Gutiérrez-Maldonado and Ribas-Sabaté2014). Treatment studies are generally very few in number and small in size but the results are very encouraging. No studies related to mania were identified.

Substance disorders

VR has the potential to present individuals with simulations of the cues that lead to the cravings that drive subsequent problematic behaviours such as drug misuse, alcohol abuse or excessive gambling. There have been 22 VR studies on substance disorders, with 15 concerning assessment, five treatment, and two theory development. The overwhelming majority of the studies have simply shown that appropriate VR environments can trigger cravings. Misuse of a range of substances has been studied, including alcohol (e.g. Lee et al. Reference Lee, Namkoong, Ku, Cho, Park, Choi, Kim, Kim, Kim and Jung2008) and cocaine (e.g. Saladin et al. Reference Saladin, Brady, Graap and Rothbaum2006). However, the majority of the work has concerned smoking (e.g. Bordnick et al. Reference Bordnick, Graap, Copp, Brooks and Ferrer2005) and it is evident that VR environments can produce strong cravings for cigarettes (Pericot-Valverde et al. Reference Pericot-Valverde, Germeroth and Tiffany2016). The elicitation of cravings means that VR has the potential to be successfully used in treatment, though this has not yet been rigorously demonstrated. Uncontrolled studies do indicate that VR might be able to help reduce cravings for cigarette smoking (e.g. Pericot-Valverde et al. Reference Pericot-Valverde, Secades-Villa, Gutiérrez-Maldonado and García-Rodríguez2014). Even crushing virtual cigarettes has been found to be helpful when added to standard treatment (Girard et al. Reference Girard, Turcotte, Bouchard and Girard2009). For smoking cessation, a randomized controlled trial testing cognitive–behavioural therapy (CBT) with VR cue exposure is underway (Giovancarli et al. Reference Giovancarli, Malbos, Baumstarck, Parola, Pélissier, Lançon, Auquier and Boyer2016).

Eating disorders

There are a number of obvious mechanistic targets for VR in the treatment of eating disorders: reducing food cravings, improving body image, and enhancing emotion regulation skills. A total of 18 empirical studies were identified, 10 concerning treatment, seven assessment, and one theory development. Despite an early use of VR for eating disorders (e.g. Riva, Reference Riva1998), it has been recognized that the field has very few methodologically strong studies (Riva, Reference Riva2011; Ferrer-García & Gutiérrez-Maldonado, Reference Ferrer-García and Gutiérrez-Maldonado2012). Suitable VR environments can bring on food cravings (e.g. Ferrer-García et al. Reference Ferrer-Garcia, Gutierrez-Maldonado, Treasure and Vilalta-Abella2015), with responses to VR food comparable with real food (Gorini et al. Reference Gorini, Griez, Petrova and Riva2010), and there has even been an initial test of high-calorie food presented using augmented reality (Pallavicini et al. Reference Pallavicini, Serino, Cipresso, Pedroli, Chicchi Giglioli, Chirico, Manzoni, Castelnuovo, Molinari and Riva2016). The preliminary trial evidence is that VR techniques added to standard CBT help to improve body image (Riva et al. Reference Riva, Bacchetta, Cesa, Conti and Molinari2003; Cesa et al. Reference Cesa, Manzoni, Bacchetta, Castelnuovo, Conti, Gaggioli, Mantovani, Molinari, Cárdenas-López and Riva2013; Marco et al. Reference Marco, Perpiñá and Botella2013). In an intriguing VR experimental study, Keizer et al. (Reference Keizer, van Elburg, Helms and Dijkerman2016) helped patients with anorexia nervosa to experience ownership of a healthy-body mass index (BMI) body, which led afterwards, for at least 2 h, to a reduction in body size overestimation. New research on understanding the body ownership illusion in VR is likely to enhance eating disorder treatments (Normand et al. Reference Normand, Giannopoulos, Spanlang and Slater2011; Maselli & Slater, Reference Maselli and Slater2013).

Additional disorders

VR could have potential uses in the understanding and treatment of sexual disorders concerning desire, arousal and orgasm. This work has not been carried out. The literature search revealed four reports describing a series of uncontrolled studies in which a form of psychodynamic therapy for erectile dysfunction or premature ejaculation included a VR element (e.g. Optale et al. Reference Optale, Marin, Pastore, Nasta and Pianon2003). Another notable area is sleep disorder, which is very common in the general population, but VR has not been used to study causes or treatments. Three studies have used a VR paradigm (road crossing) to assess the adverse effects of sleep disorders for the daytime safety of children (e.g. Avis et al. Reference Avis, Gamble and Schwebel2014).

Discussion

We conclude from the early studies that VR environments can elicit psychiatric symptoms, manipulation of VR can inform the understanding of disorders, and simpler psychological treatments can be successfully administered in VR. This is highly encouraging for the future application of VR to mental health. However, our inspection of the older literature warns that the technology of VR is not an answer in and of itself: the content delivered will matter for outcomes (e.g. Freeman et al. Reference Freeman, Bradley, Antley, Bourke, DeWeever, Evans, Černis, Sheaves, Waite, Dunn, Slater and Clark2016; Reger et al. Reference Reger, Koenen-Woods, Zetocha, Smolenski, Holloway, Rothbaum, Difede, Rizzo, Edwards-Stewart, Skopp, Mishkind, Reger and Gahm2016). Across a breadth of disorders there are instances of real innovations in the interaction between the technology and insights into mental health problems. This has largely been unheralded, perhaps because the methodological quality has been limited and the potential for wider dissemination hitherto constrained. The studies have typically been small, negative results are less likely to have been reported, and, in most places, the literature has been distinctly piecemeal. Progress has been understandably slow because hardware and software have been expensive and expertise limited. This is about to change (Wiederhold, Reference Wiederhold2016).

The gaps in the literature are astonishingly large. This technology has simply not been applied enough to mental health. Psychiatric symptoms can be assessed in VR, but robust tests of reliability and validity have been very few; compared with retrospective self-report, VR has the potential to prove a ‘gold standard’ assessment method for many mental health problems but this has not remotely been tested. VR has been used to develop the understanding of too few disorders, although even when used as an investigative tool it has principally been used to assess symptoms rather than provide firmer causal conclusions via manipulation tests (Cook & Campbell, Reference Cook and Campbell1979). Treatment trials have been small in size, rarely pre-registered, and seldom conducted to the standards now expected in clinical research. Of the range of treatment techniques available it is the simpler ones, such as exposure, that have been used. A therapist has nearly always still been engaged in the VR interventions. Numerous other important treatment techniques remain to be implemented in VR, especially for more complex disorders. There is an intriguing programme of research to be conducted concerning the degree to which therapies can be delivered without a therapist present for each type of presenting problem, and whether avatars can compensate for the important human presence fundamental to traditional psychological interventions. Many common disorders, for example, depression, have barely received any VR research attention. VR also has obvious, but untested, use in psychiatric settings such as hospital wards or forensic units where contact with the outside world is highly restricted.

We believe that there are three overarching treatment questions that need to be addressed: (1) What is the best way to immerse individuals in VR so that learning most readily transfers to the real world, balancing the need to use affordable equipment? (2) Can key theory-driven psychological treatment techniques (beyond simple exposure) be successfully delivered in VR? (3) Do engaging, personalized, theory-driven treatments implemented in affordable VR, with limited use of clinicians, produce large real-world benefits for patients? This work will need to be carried out with the user experience put at the centre of design. Given its use in gaming, VR could be made a highly appealing treatment approach for patients. There is also the issue of how related technologies, notably augmented reality and wearable devices, could dovetail with the new approaches.

Our review offers, perhaps, a glimpse of the future of mental health care. It is, however, still relatively early days with VR for mental health: scenarios are limited, as is the degree of social interaction, for example conversation, that is possible. Specialist programming expertise is still required to create suitable environments that lead to presence. Simulator sickness may occur in poorly realized scenarios and systems. Multi-sensory presentation of stimuli is most likely to induce presence, but generalized touch feedback, that is tactile stimulation on any part of the body contingent on collision with a virtual object, is not feasible at present. The potential therapeutic power of body ownership manipulation remains confined to specialist laboratories. But the technology is developing fast: these are likely to be short-term concerns. Psychological research and clinical practice have made huge strides in recent years too (Layard & Clark, Reference Layard and Clark2015). We now have a much clearer picture of which therapeutic techniques are most effective, but suitably trained therapists are in short supply, and quality control remains a concern. VR and related technologies could help in solving this problem, making the best therapy available to many more people. Yet the power of VR is such that it promises much more than an improved delivery method for psychological therapies. VR allows us to try things that are not easily practical in the real world. That means it could potentially generate the kind of results that even a course of standard treatment could not produce. ‘Revolutionary’ is an overused word; for VR and mental health care, it may actually be justified over the coming years.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171700040X

Acknowledgements

D.F. is supported by a National Institute of Health Research (NIHR) Research Professorship (RP-2014-05-003). A.E., D.C. and D.F. are supported by the Oxford NIHR Biomedical Research Centre for mental health and dementia. A.E. and D.C. are supported by the Wellcome Trust.

Declaration of Interest

Oxford VR is a University of Oxford spin-out company for developing VR treatments for mental health. Co-founders include D.F., M.S. and B.S.

References

Anderson, PL, Price, M, Edwards, SM, Obasaju, MA, Schmertz, SK, Zimand, E, Calamaras, MR (2013). Virtual reality exposure therapy for social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 81, 751760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avis, KT, Gamble, KL, Schwebel, DC (2014). Does excessive daytime sleepiness affect children's pedestrian safety? Sleep 37, 283287.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bordnick, PS, Graap, KM, Copp, HL, Brooks, J, Ferrer, M (2005). Virtual reality cue reactivity assessment in cigarette smokers. CyberPsychology and Behavior 8, 487492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botella, C, Baños, RM, Perpiñá, C, Villa, H, Alcañiz, M, Rey, A (1998). Virtual reality treatment of claustrophobia: a case report. Behaviour Research and Therapy 36, 239246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botella, C, Osma, J, Garcia-Palacios, A, Quero, S, Baños, RM (2004). Treatment of flying phobia using virtual reality: data from a 1-year follow-up using a multiple baseline design. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy 11, 311323.Google Scholar
Bouchard, S, Dumoulin, S, Robillard, G, Guitard, T, Klinger, É, Forget, H, Loranger, C, Roucaut, FX (2016). Virtual reality compared with in vivo exposure in the treatment of social anxiety disorder: a three-arm randomised controlled trial. British Journal of Psychiatry. Published online 15 December 2016. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.116.184234.Google Scholar
Cesa, GL, Manzoni, GM, Bacchetta, M, Castelnuovo, G, Conti, S, Gaggioli, A, Mantovani, F, Molinari, E, Cárdenas-López, G, Riva, GV (2013). Virtual reality for enhancing the cognitive behavioral treatment of obesity with binge eating disorder: randomized controlled study with one-year follow-up. Journal of Medical Internet Research 15, e113.Google Scholar
Cook, TD, Campbell, DT (1979). Quasi-Experimentation: Design and Analysis Issues for Field Settings. Rand-McNally: Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Cruz-Neira, C, Sandin, DJ, DeFanti, TA (1993). Surround-screen projection-based virtual reality: the design and implementation of the CAVE. ACM Computer Graphics (SIGGRAPH) Proceedings 27, 135142.Google Scholar
Difede, J, Cukor, J, Jayasinghe, N, Patt, I, Jedel, S, Spielman, L, Giosan, C, Hoffman, HG (2007). Virtual reality exposure therapy for the treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder following September 11, 2001. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 68, 16391647.Google Scholar
Emmelkamp, PMG, Krijn, M, Hulsbosch, AM, de Vries, S, Schuemie, MJ, van der Mast, CAPG (2002). Virtual reality treatment versus exposure in vivo: a comparative evaluation in acrophobia. Behaviour Research and Therapy 40, 509516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falconer, CJ, Rovira, A, King, JA, Gilbert, P, Antley, A, Fearon, P, Ralph, N, Slater, M, Brewin, CR (2016). Embodying self-compassion within virtual reality and its effects on patients with depression. British Journal of Psychiatry Open 2, 7480.Google Scholar
Ferrer-García, M, Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J (2012). The use of virtual reality in the study, assessment, and treatment of body image in eating disorders and nonclinical samples: a review of the literature. Body Image 9, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrer-Garcia, M, Gutierrez-Maldonado, J, Treasure, J, Vilalta-Abella, F (2015). Craving for food in virtual reality scenarios in non-clinical sample: analysis of its relationship with body mass index and eating disorder symptoms. European Eating Disorders Review 23, 371378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D (2008). Studying and treating schizophrenia using virtual reality (VR): a new paradigm. Schizophrenia Bulletin 34, 605610.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D, Bradley, J, Antley, A, Bourke, E, DeWeever, N, Evans, N, Černis, E, Sheaves, B, Waite, F, Dunn, G, Slater, M, Clark, D (2016). Virtual reality in the treatment of persecutory delusions. British Journal of Psychiatry 209, 6267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D, Evans, N, Lister, R, Antley, A, Dunn, G, Slater, M (2014). Height, social comparison, and paranoia: an immersive virtual reality experimental study. Psychiatry Research 30, 348352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D, Pugh, K, Antley, A, Slater, M, Bebbington, P, Gittins, M, Dunn, G, Kuipers, E, Fowler, D, Garety, PA (2008). A virtual reality study of paranoid thinking in the general population. British Journal of Psychiatry 192, 258263.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, D, Slater, M, Bebbington, PE, Garety, PA, Kuipers, E, Fowler, D, Met, A, Read, C, Jordan, J, Vinayagamoorthy, V (2003). Can virtual reality be used to investigate persecutory ideation? Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease 191, 509514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia-Palacios, A, Hoffman, H, Carlin, A, Furness, TA, Botella, C (2002). Virtual reality in the treatment of spider phobia: a controlled study. Behaviour Research and Therapy 40, 983993.Google Scholar
Giovancarli, C, Malbos, E, Baumstarck, K, Parola, N, Pélissier, MF, Lançon, C, Auquier, P, Boyer, L (2016). Virtual reality cue exposure for the relapse prevention of tobacco consumption: a study protocol for a randomized controlled trial. Trials 17, 96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girard, B, Turcotte, V, Bouchard, S, Girard, B (2009). Crushing virtual cigarettes reduces tobacco addiction and treatment discontinuation. Cyberpsychology and Behavior 12, 477483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorini, A, Griez, E, Petrova, A, Riva, G (2010). Assessment of the emotional responses produced by exposure to real food, virtual food and photographs of food in patients affected by eating disorders. Annals of General Psychiatry 9, 30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gould, NF, Holmes, MK, Fantie, BD, Luckenbaugh, DA, Pine, DS, Gould, TD, Burgess, N, Manji, HK, Zarate, CA Jr. (2007). Performance on a virtual reality spatial memory navigation task in depressed patients. American Journal of Psychiatry 164, 516519.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodges, LF, Kooper, R, Meyer, TC, Rothbaum, BO, Opdyke, D, de Graaff, JJ, Williford, JS, North, MM (1995). Virtual environments for treating the fear of heights. Computer 28, 2734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keizer, A, van Elburg, A, Helms, R, Dijkerman, HC (2016). A virtual reality full body illusion improves body image disturbance in anorexia nervosa. PLOS ONE 11, e0163921.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Layard, R, Clark, DM (2015). Thrive: The Power of Psychological Therapy. Penguin: London.Google Scholar
Lee, JS, Namkoong, K, Ku, J, Cho, S, Park, JY, Choi, YK, Kim, JJ, Kim, IY, Kim, SI, Jung, YC (2008). Social pressure-induced craving in patients with alcohol dependence: application of virtual reality to coping skill training. Psychiatry Investigations 5, 239243.Google Scholar
Ling, Y, Nefs, HT, Morina, N, Heynderickx, I, Brinkman, W-P (2014). A meta-analysis on the relationship between self-reported presence and anxiety in virtual reality exposure therapy for anxiety disorders. PLOS ONE 9, e96144.Google Scholar
Marco, JH, Perpiñá, C, Botella, C (2013). Effectiveness of cognitive behavioral therapy supported by virtual reality in the treatment of body image in eating disorders: one year follow-up. Psychiatry Research 209, 619625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maselli, A, Slater, M (2013). The building blocks of the full body ownership illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCann, R, Armstrong, C, Skopp, N, Edwards-Stewart, A, Smolenski, D, June, J, Metzger-Abamukong, M, Reger, G (2014). Virtual reality exposure therapy for the treatment of anxiety disorders: an evaluation of research quality. Journal of Anxiety Disorders 28, 625631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyerbröker, K, Emmelkamp, P (2010). Virtual reality exposure therapy in anxiety disorders: a systematic review of process-and-outcome studies. Depression and Anxiety 27, 933944.Google Scholar
Morina, N, Ijntema, H, Meyerbröker, K, Emmelkamp, P (2015). Can virtual reality exposure therapy gains be generalised to real-life? A meta-analysis of studies applying behavioural assessments. Behaviour Research and Therapy 74, 1824.Google Scholar
Noë, A (2004). Action in Perception. MIT Press: Cambridge, MA.Google Scholar
Normand, J-M, Giannopoulos, E, Spanlang, B, Slater, M (2011). Multisensory stimulation can induce an illusion of larger belly size in immersive virtual reality. PLoS ONE 6, e16128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opriş, D, Pintea, S, García-Palacios, A, Botella, C, Szamosközi, S, David, D (2012). Virtual reality exposure therapy in anxiety disorders: a quantitative meta-analysis. Depression and Anxiety 29, 8593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Optale, G, Marin, S, Pastore, M, Nasta, A, Pianon, C (2003). Male sexual dysfunctions and multimedia immersion therapy (follow-up). Cyberpsychology and Behavior 6, 289294.Google Scholar
Pallavicini, F, Serino, S, Cipresso, P, Pedroli, E, Chicchi Giglioli, IA, Chirico, A, Manzoni, GM, Castelnuovo, G, Molinari, E, Riva, G (2016). Testing augmented reality for cue exposure in obese patients: an exploratory study. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 19, 107114.Google Scholar
Pericot-Valverde, I, Germeroth, L, Tiffany, S (2016). The use of virtual reality in the production of cue-specific craving for cigarettes: a meta-analysis. Nicotine and Tobacco Research 18, 538546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pericot-Valverde, I, Secades-Villa, R, Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J, García-Rodríguez, O (2014). Effects of systematic cue exposure through virtual reality on cigarette craving. Nicotine and Tobacco Research 16, 14701477.Google Scholar
Reger, GM, Koenen-Woods, P, Zetocha, K, Smolenski, DJ, Holloway, KM, Rothbaum, BO, Difede, J, Rizzo, AA, Edwards-Stewart, A, Skopp, NA, Mishkind, M, Reger, MA, Gahm, GA (2016). Randomized controlled trial of prolonged exposure using imaginal exposure vs. virtual reality exposure in active duty soldiers with deployment-related posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 84, 946959.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riva, G (1998). Virtual environment for body image modification: virtual reality system for the treatment of body image disturbances. Computers in Human Behavior 14, 477490.Google Scholar
Riva, G (2011). The key to unlocking the virtual body: virtual reality in the treatment of obesity and eating disorders. Journal of Diabetes Science and Technology 5, 283292.Google Scholar
Riva, G, Bacchetta, M, Cesa, G, Conti, S, Molinari, E (2003). Six-month follow-up of in-patient experiential cognitive therapy for binge eating disorders. Cyberpsychology and Behavior 6, 251258.Google Scholar
Rizzo, AA, Difede, J, Rothbaum, BO, Johnston, S, McLay, RN, Reger, G, Gahm, G, Parsons, T, Graap, K, Pair, J (2009). VR PTSD exposure therapy results with active duty OIF/OEF combatants. Studies in Health Technology and Informatics 142, 277282.Google Scholar
Rothbaum, B, Hodges, L, Anderson, P, Price, L, Smith, S (2002). Twelve-month follow-up of virtual reality and standard exposure therapies for the fear of flying. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 70, 428432.Google Scholar
Rothbaum, BO, Hodges, L, Smith, S, Lee, JH, Price, L (2000). A controlled study of virtual reality exposure therapy for the fear of flying. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 68, 10201026.Google Scholar
Rothbaum, BO, Hodges, L, Watson, BA, Kessler, CD, Opdyke, D (1996). Virtual reality exposure therapy in the treatment of fear of flying: a case report. Behaviour Research and Therapy 34, 477481.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rus-Calafell, M, Gutiérrez-Maldonado, J, Ribas-Sabaté, J (2014). A virtual reality-integrated program for improving social skills in patients with schizophrenia: a pilot study. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 45, 8189.Google Scholar
Saladin, M, Brady, K, Graap, K, Rothbaum, B (2006). A preliminary report on the use of virtual reality technology to elicit craving and cue reactivity in cocaine dependent individuals. Addictive Behaviors 31, 18811894.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, LB, Torres, S, Kannusamy, P, Chng, CM, He, HG, Klainin-Yobas, P (2015). Efficacy of the virtual reality-based stress management program on stress-related variables in people with mood disorders: the feasibility study. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing 29, 613.Google Scholar
Shiban, Y, Fruth, MB, Pauli, P, Kinateder, M, Reichenberger, J, Mühlberger, A (2016). Treatment effect on biases in size estimation in spider phobia. Biological Psychology 121, 146152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shiban, Y, Schelhorn, I, Pauli, P, Mühlberger, A (2015). Effect of combined multiple contexts and multiple stimuli exposure in spider phobia: a randomized clinical trial in virtual reality. Behaviour Research and Therapy 71, 4553.Google Scholar
Slater, M (2009). Place illusion and plausibility can lead to realistic behaviour in immersive virtual environments. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences 364, 35493557.Google Scholar
Slater, M, Sanchez-Vives, M (2016). Enhancing our lives with immersive virtual reality. Frontiers in Robotics and AI. Published online 19 December 2016. doi:10.3389/frobt.2016.00074.Google Scholar
Slater, M, Spanlang, B, Sanchez-Vives, M, Blanke, O (2010). First person experience of body transfer in virtual reality. PLoS ONE 5, e10564.Google Scholar
Sorkin, A, Weinshall, D, Modai, I, Peled, A (2006). Improving the accuracy of the diagnosis of schizophrenia by means of virtual reality. American Journal of Psychiatry 163, 512520.Google Scholar
Spanlang, B, Normand, J-M, Borland, D, Kilteni, K, Giannopoulos, E, Pomés, A, González-Franco, M, Perez-Marcos, D, Arroyo-Palacios, J, Muncunill, XN, Slater, M (2014). How to build an embodiment lab: achieving body representation illusions in virtual reality. Frontiers in Robotics and AI. Published online 27 November 2014. doi:10.3389/frobt.2014.00009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taffou, M, Guerchouche, R, Drettakis, G, Viaud-Delmon, I (2013). Auditory–visual aversive stimuli modulate the conscious experience of fear. Multisensory Research 26, 347370.Google Scholar
Valmaggia, LR, Day, F, Garety, P, Freeman, D, Antley, A, Slater, M, Swapp, D, Myin-Germeys, I, McGuire, P (2015). Social defeat predicts paranoid appraisals in people at high risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research 168, 1622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Veling, W, Pot-Kolder, R, Counotte, J, van Os, J, van der Gaag, M (2016). Environmental social stress, paranoia and psychosis liability: a virtual reality study. Schizophrenia Bulletin 42, 13631371.Google Scholar
Wiederhold, B (2016). Lessons learned as we begin the third decade of virtual reality. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking 19, 577578.Google Scholar
Wiederhold, B, Wiederhold, M (2003). Three-year follow-up for virtual reality exposure for fear of flying. CyberPsychology and Behavior 6, 441445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Supplementary material: File

Freeman supplementary material

Freeman supplementary material 1

Download Freeman supplementary material(File)
File 584.2 KB
Supplementary material: File

Freeman supplementary material

Freeman supplementary material 2

Download Freeman supplementary material(File)
File 98.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Freeman supplementary material

Freeman supplementary material 3

Download Freeman supplementary material(File)
File 183.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Freeman supplementary material

Freeman supplementary material 4

Download Freeman supplementary material(File)
File 103.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Freeman supplementary material

Freeman supplementary material 5

Download Freeman supplementary material(File)
File 99.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Freeman supplementary material

Freeman supplementary material 6

Download Freeman supplementary material(File)
File 101.4 KB
Supplementary material: File

Freeman supplementary material

Freeman supplementary material 7

Download Freeman supplementary material(File)
File 100.4 KB