Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T01:58:39.520Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Relationship between Self-Focused Attention and Mindfulness in Peoplewith and without Hallucination Proneness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 April 2014

Salvador Perona-Garcelán*
Affiliation:
Hospital Universitario “Virgen del Rocío” (Spain)
José M. García-Montes
Affiliation:
Universidad de Almería (Spain)
Ana Mª López-Jiménez
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla (Spain)
Juan Francisco Rodríguez-Testal
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla (Spain)
Miguel Ruiz-Veguilla
Affiliation:
Hospital Universitario “Virgen del Rocío” (Spain)
María Jesús Ductor-Recuerda
Affiliation:
Hospital Universitario “Virgen del Rocío” (Spain)
María del Mar Benítez-Hernández
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla (Spain)
Mª Ángeles Arias-Velarde
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla (Spain)
María Teresa Gómez-Gómez
Affiliation:
Universidad de Sevilla (Spain)
Marino Pérez-Álvarez
Affiliation:
Universidad de Oviedo (Spain).
*
*Correspondence concerning this articleshould be addressed to Salvador Perona-Garcelán. HospitalUniversitario “Virgen del Rocío”. ServicioAndaluz de Salud. Avda. Jerez, s/n. 41013. Seville (Spain). Phone:+34–954787414. E-mail: sperona@us.es

Abstract

The purpose of this work was to study the relationship between self-focusedattention and mindfulness in participants prone to hallucinations and others whowere not. A sample of 318 healthy participants, students at the universities ofSevilla and Almería, was given the Launay-Slade HallucinationsScale-revised (LSHS-R, Bentall & Slade, 1985). Based on this sample, two groups were formed:participants with high (n = 55) and low proneness(n = 28) to hallucinations. Participants with ascore higher than a standard deviation from the mean in the LSHS-R were includedin the high proneness group, participants with a score lower than a standarddeviation from the mean in the LSHR-R were included in the second one. Allparticipants were also given the Self-Absorption Scale (SAS, McKenzie& Hoyle, 2008) and theSouthampton Mindfulness Questionnaire (SMQ, Chadwick et al., 2008). The results showed thatparticipants with high hallucination proneness had significantly higher levelsof public (t(80) = 6.81, p< .001) and private (t(77) = 7.39,p < .001) self-focused attention and lower levelsof mindfulness (t(81) = -4.56, p< .001) than participants in the group with low hallucinationproneness. A correlational analysis showed a negative association betweenself-focused attention (private and public) and mindfulness (r= -0.23, p < .001; r= -0.38, p < .001 respectively). Finally,mindfulness was found to partly mediate between self-focused attention andhallucination proneness. The importance of self-focused attention andmindfulness in understanding the etiology of hallucinations discussed andsuggest some approaches to their treatment.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Universidad Complutense de Madrid and Colegio Oficial de Psicólogos de Madrid 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, P., Freeman, D., McGuire, P., Garety, P., Kuipers, E., Fowler, D., … Ray, K. (2005). The prediction of hallucination proneness in non-clinical individuals: Examining the contribution of emotion and reasoning. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44, 127132. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466504X20044 Google Scholar
Baer, R. A. (2009). Self-focused attention and mechanisms of change in mindfulness-based treatment. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 38, 1520. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/16506070902980703 Google Scholar
Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietmeyer, J., & Toney, L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 2745. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/1073191105283504 Google Scholar
Bentall, R. P., & Slade, P. (1985). Reliability of a scale measuring disposition towards hallucination: A brief report. Personality and Individual Differences, 6, 527529. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0191-8869(85)90151-5 Google Scholar
Bishop, S. R., Lau, M., Shapiro, S., & Carlson, L., (2004). Mindfulness: A proposed operational definition. Clinical psychology: Science and practice, 11, 230241. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/clipsy/bph077 Google Scholar
Chadwick, P. D. J. (2006). Person-based cognitive therapy for distressing psychosis. Chichester, UK: Wiley.Google Scholar
Chadwick, P. D. J., Barnbrook, E., & Newman Taylor, K. (2007). Responding mindfully to distressing voices: Links with meaning, affect and relationship with voice. Journal of the Norwegian Psychological Association, 44, 581588.Google Scholar
Chadwick, P., & Birchwood, M. (1994) The omnipotence of voices. A cognitive approach to auditory hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 190201. http://dx.doi.org/10.1192/bjp.164.2.190 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chadwick, P. D. J., Hember, M., Symes, J., Peters, E., Kuipers, E., & Dagnan, D., (2008). Responding mindfully to distressing thoughts and images: Reliabity and validity of the Southampton mindfulness questionnaire (SMQ). British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 47, 451455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1348/014466508X314891 Google Scholar
Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objetive self-awareness. New York: NY. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ensum, I., & Morrison, A. P. (2003). The effects of focus attention on attributional bias in patients experiencing auditory hallucinations. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41, 895907. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00102-X Google Scholar
Exner, J. E. (1973). The self-focus sentence completion: A study of egocentricity. Journal of Personality Assessment, 37, 437455. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223891.1973.10119902 Google Scholar
Fenigstein, A., Scheier, M. F., & Buss, A. H. (1975). Public and private self-consciousness: Assessment and theory. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 43, 522527. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0076760 Google Scholar
Fetterman, A. K., Robinson, M. D., Ode, S., & Gordon, K. H. (2010). Neuroticism as a risk factor for behavioral dysregulation: A mindfulness-mediation perspective. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 29, 301321. http://dx.doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2010.29.3.301 Google Scholar
Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Lemos-Giráldez, S., Paino, M., Sierra-Baigrie, S., Villazón-García, U., García-Portilla González, M. P., & Muñiz, J. (2010). Dimensionality of hallucination proneness: Confirmatory factor analysis of the Launay-Slade Hallucination Scale-revised in college students. Anales de Psicología, 26, 4148.Google Scholar
Ingram, R. E. (1990). Self-focused attention in clinical disorders: Review and a conceptual model. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 156176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.156 Google Scholar
Kabat-Zinn, J. (1990). Full catastrophe living: Using the wisdom of your body and mind to face stress, pain, and illness. New York, NY: Delta.Google Scholar
Ma, S. H., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Mindfulness-based cognitive therapy for depression: Replication and exploration of differential relapse prevention effects. Journal of consulting and clinical Psychology, 72, 3141. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.72.1.31 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKenzie, K. S., & Hoyle, R. H. (2008). The self-absorption scale: Reliability and validity in non-clinical samples. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 726731. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2008.07.020 Google Scholar
Mor, N., & Winquist, J. (2002). Self-focused attention and negative affect: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 638662. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638 Google Scholar
Morrison, A. P., & Haddock, G. (1997). Self-focused attention in schizophrenic patients with and without auditory hallucinations and normal subjects: A comparative study. Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 937941. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00130-X Google Scholar
Muñiz, J., & Hambleton, R. K. (1996). Directrices para la traducción y adaptación de los test [Indications for translating and adapting psychological tests]. Papeles del Psicólogo, 66, 6370.Google Scholar
Perona-Garcelán, S., Cuevas-Yust, C., García-Montes, J. M., Pérez-Álvarez, M., Ductor-Recuerda, M. J., Salas-Azcona, R., … Rodríguez-Martin, B. (2008). Relationship between self-focused attention and dissociation in patients with and without auditory hallucinations. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 196, 190197.Google Scholar
Perona-Garcelán, S., García-Montes, J. M., Ductor-Recuerda, M. J., Vallina-Fernández, O., Cuevas-Yust, C., Pérez-Álvarez, M., … Gómez-Gómez, M. T. (2012). Relationship of metacognition, absorption and depersonalization in patients with auditory hallucinations. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 100118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8260.2011.02015.x Google Scholar
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 717731. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03206553 Google Scholar
Sedikides, C. (1992). Mood as a determinant of attentional focus. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 129148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02699939208411063 Google Scholar
Startup, M., Startup, S., & Sedgman, A. (2008). Immediate source-monitoring, self-focused attention and the positive symptoms of schizophrenia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 11761180. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2008.07.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995) How does cognitive therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) training help? Behaviour Research and therapy, 33, 2539. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(94)E0011-7 Google Scholar
Varese, F., Barkus, E., & Bentall, R. P. (2011). Dissociative and metacognitive factors in hallucinations-proneness when controlling for comorbid symptoms. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 16, 193217. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13546805.2010.495244 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 163206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163 Google Scholar
Wells, A., & Cartwright-Hatton, S. (2004). A short form of the Metacognition Questionnaire: Properties of the MCQ-30. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 385396. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00147-5 CrossRefGoogle Scholar