Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-xbtfd Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T10:29:21.027Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Evolutionary Biology and Psychiatry

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

C. U. M. Smith*
Affiliation:
Unit of Clinical Neurophysiology, Department of Vision Sciences, Aston University, Birmingham B4 7ET

Extract

“Every one of the twenty-eight bones of the human skull has been inherited in an unbroken succession from the air-breathing fishes of the pre-Devonian seas.” (Gregory, 1967)

Type
Annotation
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 1993 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alexander, R. D. (1987) The Biology of Moral Systems. New York: Aldine.Google Scholar
Allen, C. (1992) Mental content and evolutionary explanation. Biology and Philosophy, 7, 112.Google Scholar
Burglin, T. R., Finney, M., Coulson, A., et al (1989) Caenorhabditis elegans has scores of homeobox containing genes. Nature, 341, 239243.Google Scholar
Chance, M. R. A., (ed.) (1988) Social Fabrics of the Mind. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. M. (1988) Matter and Consciousness. Cambridge: Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1838) B Notebook, pp. 231232.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872) The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals. London: Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Driscoll, M. & Chalfie, M. (1991) The mec-4 gene is a member of a family of Caenorhabditis elegans genes that can mutate to induce neuronal degeneration. Nature, 349, 588593.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1992) Facial expressions of emotion: an old conundrum and new findings. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 335, 6369.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. & Friesman, W. V. (1971) Constants across culture in face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124129.Google Scholar
Feigl, H. (1967) The ‘Mental’ and the ‘Physical’, p. 79. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983) The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Friedrich, P. (1991) Protein structure: the primary substrate of memory. Neuroscience, 35, 18.Google Scholar
Garcia, J., Walter, G. H. & Rusniak, K. W. (1974) Behavioural regularities in the milieu interne in man and rat. Science, 185, 824831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, R. (1988) Psychiatric syndromes as infrastructure for intraspecific communication. In Social Fabrics of the Mind (ed. Chance, M. R. A.). Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gregory, W. K. (1967) Our Face from Fish to Man. New York: Hafner.Google Scholar
Griffiths, P. E. (1990) Modularity, and the psychoevolutionary theory of emotions. Biology and Philosophy, 5, 175196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hinde, T. (1982) Ethology. New York: Fontana Books.Google Scholar
Humphrey, N. K. (1979) Nature's psychologists. In Consciousness and the Physical World (eds Josephson, B. & Ramamchandran, B. S.). New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Huxley, T. H. (1874) On the hypothesis that animals are automata, and its history. Method and Results, p. 236. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Jacob, F. (1977) Evolution and tinkering. Science, 196, 11611166.Google Scholar
Kollar, E. & Fisher, C. (1980) Tooth inductions in chick epithelium: expression of quiescent genes for enamel synthesis. Science, 207, 993995.Google Scholar
Laughlin, C., McManus, J. & d'Aquili, E. G. (1990) Brain, Symbol and Experience. Boston: New Science Library.Google Scholar
MacLean, P. D. (1989) The Triune Brain in Evolution: Role in Palaeocerebral Functions. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
McGinn, C. (1989) Can we solve the mind-body problem. Mind, 98, 349366.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974) What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83, 435450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, R. (1848) On the Archetype and Homologies of the Vertebrate Skeleton. London: John von Voorst.Google Scholar
Rapoport, S. I. (1990) Integrated phylogeny of the primate brain, with special reference to humans and their disease. Brain Research Review, 15, 267294.Google Scholar
Sartre, J.-P. (1936) Translated (1957) as The Transcendence of the Ego, by Williams, F. & Kirkpatrick, R. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Schjelderup-Ebbe, T. (1935) Social behaviour in birds. In Handbook of Social Psychology (ed Murchison, C.). Worcester, Mass.: Clarke University Press. (Quoted in Chance (ed.) (1988), p. 177.)Google Scholar
Sherrington, C. S. (1947) The Integrative Action of the Nervous System, p. 22. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, C. U. M. (1978) Charles Darwin, the origin of consciousness and panpsychism. Journal of the History of Biology, 11, 245267.Google Scholar
Smith, C. U. M. (1982) Evolution and the problem of mind: 2. John Hughlings Jackson. Journal of the History of Biology, 15, 241262.Google Scholar
Smith, C. U. M. (1989) Elements of Molecular Neurobiology. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Smith, C. U. M. (1991) Kant and Darwin. Journal of Social and Biological Structures, 14, 3550.Google Scholar
Smith, C. U. M. (1993) Existentialism and neurobiology. Intersezione: Rivista di storia della idee (in press).Google Scholar
Tinbergen, N. (1951) The Study of Instinct. London: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1952) Philosophical Investigations, section 19 etc. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.