Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T14:38:38.861Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Pressures to adhere to treatment (‘leverage’) in English mental healthcare

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Tom Burns*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Social Psychiatry Unit, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Ksenija Yeeles
Affiliation:
Social Psychiatry Unit, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Andrew Molodynski
Affiliation:
Social Psychiatry Unit, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Helen Nightingale
Affiliation:
Social Psychiatry Unit, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, UK
Maria Vazquez-Montes
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, University of Oxford, UK
Kathleen Sheehan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Canada
Louise Linsell
Affiliation:
Department of Public Health and Primary Care, University of Oxford, UK
*
Tom Burns, MD, DSc, FRCPsych, Department of Psychiatry, University of Oxford, Warneford Hospital, Oxford OX3 7JX, UK. Email: tom.burns@psych.ox.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Coercion has usually been equated with legal detention. Non-statutory pressures to adhere to treatment, ‘leverage’, have been identified as widespread in US public mental healthcare. It is not clear if this is so outside the USA.

Aims

To measure rates of different non-statutory pressures in distinct clinical populations in England, to test their associations with patient characteristics and compare them with US rates.

Method

Data were collected by a structured interview conducted by independent researchers supplemented by data extraction from case notes.

Results

We recruited a sample of 417 participants from four differing clinical populations. Lifetime experience of leverage was reported in 35% of the sample, 63% in substance misusers, 33% and 30% in the psychosis samples and 15% in the non-psychosis sample. Leverage was associated with repeated hospitalisations, substance misuse diagnosis and lower insight as measured by the Insight and Treatment Attitudes Questionnaire. Housing leverage was the most frequent form (24%). Levels were markedly lower than those reported in the USA.

Conclusions

Non-statutory pressure to adhere to treatment (leverage) is common in English mental healthcare but has received little clinical or research attention. Urgent attention is needed to understand its variation and place in community practice.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2011 

Compulsory treatment is integral to mental healthcare and originally restricted to in-patient settings. Reference Kjellin, Hoyer, Engberg, Kaltiala-Heino and Sigurjonsdottir1 Several legislations now permit coerced treatment in the community. Reference Dawson2 Many ‘voluntary’ patients experience their cooperation with mental healthcare as coerced. Reference Hoge, Lidz, Eisenberg, Gardner, Monahan and Mulvey3 A study of public mental health service patients in five US cities found that half the sample reported experiencing specific pressures to promote treatment adherence. Reference Monahan, Redlich, Swanson, Robbins, Appelbaum and Petrila4 These pressures are referred to as ‘informal coercion’ or more commonly ‘leverages’. Leverages included access to accommodation, financial assistance and reduction or waiving of judicial sanctions. It is not clear if these high levels of leverage are restricted to the USA. The US mental healthcare system has a relatively fragmented community provision, limited availability of adequate benefits Reference Clark Robbins, Petrila, LeMelle and Monahan5 and some availability of ‘mental health courts’. Reference Redlich, Steadman, Clark Robbins and Swanson6 Leverage may be less common in a European setting where accommodation and welfare payments are basic entitlements and cannot be easily linked to treatment. We measured the prevalence and patterns of leverage across four differing clinical samples and tested for patient and treatment characteristics associated with them.

Method

Ethical approval was given by Oxfordshire Research Ethics Committee A (22 February 2006, Ref. No. 05/Q1604/180). An independent researcher explained the study and obtained written informed consent for an interview and examination of case notes. Participants were paid £10 (US$15) for the 50 min interview. This comprised demographic and historical data plus a series of structured questions (Appendix) on four forms of leverage adapted from Monahan and colleagues’ US study. Reference Monahan, Redlich, Swanson, Robbins, Appelbaum and Petrila4

Sample

Participants were recruited from the Oxford and Buckinghamshire Mental Health Trust that serves a mixed urban and rural population of 1 million. Recruitment procedures varied between the different teams. We aimed to recruit 100 assertive outreach team (AOT) patients with a diagnosis of psychosis, 100 community mental health team (CMHT) patients with a diagnosis of psychosis, 100 CMHT patients with a diagnosis of non-psychosis and lastly 100 people with heroin-dependent substance misuse currently treated with substitution medication (methadone or subutex). There were no exclusion criteria apart from a duration of care of less than 6 months.

Assertive outreach teams target hard to engage people with psychosis and have small case-loads (1:12), whereas CMHTs have higher, diagnostically mixed case-loads (usually 1:25). The participants with substance misuse either attended dispersed local clinics run by the mental health services or attended their general practitioner as ‘shared-care’ patients for prescribing and follow-up.

All patients on the case-loads of the Trust’s four AOTs were approached and CMHT participants were recruited in randomly selected blocks of 25 with either psychosis or non-psychosis diagnoses from the case-loads of eight teams. Oversampling was undertaken in all four samples because the duration of contact (required to confirm eligibility) could not always be identified reliably from the case notes. Comprehensive case-loads were not available in the substance misuse services. A consecutive convenience sample was obtained from the four methadone clinics in the Oxfordshire County and the Oxford City shared-care methadone services.

A series of established, validated questionnaires and rating instruments were conducted by the researcher at the interview between January 2008 and April 2009. Training was provided by an experienced psychiatrist for the clinical rating scales. The first 20 participants were jointly rated by both researchers. Interrater reliability for Global Assessment of Functioning (GAF) Reference Endicott, Spitzer, Fleiss and Cohen7 was fair (intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) = 0.489) and for Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS) Reference Overall and Gorham8 was moderate (ICC = 0.789). Refresher sessions were conducted every 6 months.

Instruments

  1. (a) The GAF rates impairment in functioning ranging from 0 to 100, with given anchors at 10-point levels.

  2. (b) BPRS Reference Overall and Gorham8 rates 18 symptoms each for severity from one to seven.

  3. (c) The Insight and Treatment Attitudes Questionnaire (ITAQ) Reference McEvoy, Freter, Everett, Geller, Appelbaum and Apperson9 is a self-reported, semi-structured 11-item questionnaire assessing awareness of illness and need for treatment.

  4. (d) The Conjoint CAGE Reference Erwing10 is an 8-item questionnaire for screening both alcohol (four items) and drug misuse (four items). Two or more positive responses on either scale indicate pathology.

  5. (e) Leverage questions are detailed in the Appendix and are taken from the Monahan study. Reference Monahan, Redlich, Swanson, Robbins, Appelbaum and Petrila4 Questions test for both access to and potential withdrawal of benefits. Questions are repeated for each stage of the criminal justice system. Any positive response within a specific leverage area counts as ‘reported’.

Statistical analysis

Comparisons across three or more sample groups were carried out using Kruskall–Wallis non-parametric tests. A variety of tests were used when only two groups were compared. These were: the Mann–Whitney test for non-normally distributed continuous variables; chi-squared test for binary variables; and ANOVA for categorical variables. To compare our outcomes with the US sample, overall percentages and pooled means and standard deviations were calculated for the latter. A t-test was used when comparing means and a two-proportion Z-test when comparing percentages. Analyses were run in SPSS 15.0 for Windows.

Univariate and multivariate multilevel logistic regressions were used to investigate the association of sociodemographic and clinical characteristics with types of leverage in the whole sample. As the sample comprised participants from four different groups, it was treated as a stratified sample with two levels or strata. The four subgroups occupy the upper level and the participants form the lower level. It is expected that participants in the same sample group are more similar to each other, thus breaking the independence assumption needed for standard statistical methods. Multilevel models were used to analyse variables measured at different levels simultaneously, employing a statistical model that includes the various dependencies. Reference Hox11 Stata 9.0 command xtlogit on Windows was used for this analysis.

Results

We recruited 417 participants: 101 in the CMHT psychosis group, 101 in the substance misuse group, 102 in the AOT (with psychosis) group and 113 in the CMHT non-psychosis group. Of 912 identified patients, 308 were uncontactable, discharged or ineligible. Of the 604 approached 188 refused involvement.

Baseline characteristics

Table 1 outlines the sociodemographic and main historical and clinical features of the four samples using the same presentation format and variables as Monahan and colleagues Reference Monahan, Redlich, Swanson, Robbins, Appelbaum and Petrila4 for ease of comparison. The group was predominantly male (60%), White (89%) and born in the UK (89%). Most patients were single (60%) and most (83%) lived in independent accommodation. Service contact ranged from a mean of 17 years for the AOT group to 11.5 years for the substance misuse group with repeated hospitalisations in the mental illness sample (AOT and CMHT groups combined). The BPRS mean total score of 33.0 and GAF of 56.9 indicate a relatively poorly functioning group.

Differences in baseline characteristics between the clinical samples

There were statistically significant differences between the four clinical samples on a number of variables not included in Table 1. General education was shorter in the substance misuse group; employment was lowest in the AOT group who also were much more likely to be single. The proportion of those with children under 18 who were living with them was lowest in the AOT group at 15% in contrast to 65% of those in the CMHT non-psychosis group and 54% of those in the CMHT psychosis group. The number of past hospitalisations ranged from a mean of 6.5 in the AOT group to 0.2 in the substance misuse group.

Differences between the mental illness and substance misuse samples

The substance misuse sample appears to have contributed most to the variation in baseline characteristics and so it was compared individually with the mental illness group. The substance misuse sample was significantly younger than the mental illness sample (mean 34.7 v. 42.0 years) with more men (76% v. 55%) and fewer participants from ethnic minorities (3% v. 13%). Significant differences were found for all psychiatric history variables. Age at first contact with psychiatric services was significantly lower for the substance misuse group (mean 23.3 v. 25.9), number of psychiatric hospitalisations was lower (mean 0.2 v. 4.0) and number of previous involuntary hospitalisations lower (4% v. 52%). The substance misuse sample had more criminal convictions (82% v. 26%) and previous imprisonments (48% v. 14%).

Differences within the mental illness groups

The three mental illness groups (AOT and CMHTs) demonstrated greater homogeneity. There were no significant differences in years of general education, proportion of those with children younger than 18 years and proportion with criminal convictions. All other differences identified across the four samples retained their significance.

Lifetime experience of leverage

Table 2 displays the rates of lifetime experience of reported leverage types across the four groups. Overall, 145 (35%) participants reported any leverage, of whom 51 (12%) reported more than one type and seven more than two. Multiple leverages are predominantly reported in the substance misuse group (33%). Housing leverage is by far the most common (24%). The criminal justice system leverage of 15% overall is accounted for almost entirely by the substance misuse and AOT groups. Financial leverage was only reported by ten participants (2%) but childcare access leverage was reported for 34 (8%) participants; reflecting 26% of the 130 with children under 18 years of age.

Table 3 shows the univariate associations of individual leverages and Table 4 the univariate and multivariate associations of overall experience of leverages and of the experience of more than one type of leverage. In multivariate analysis all the significances are lost apart from ethnicity. Reporting more than one type of leverage (Table 4) remains significantly associated in

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics across the four samplesa

Assertive outreach group (n = 102) CMHT psychosis group (n = 101) CMHT non-psychosis group (n = 113) Substance misuse group (n = 101) Total sample (n = 417) P b US sample (Monahan et al)4 (n = 1011) P c
Age, years: mean (s.d.) 40.1 (10.37) 42.2 (10.67) 43.7 (10.69) 34.7 (8.56) 40.3 (10.65) <0.001 43.4 (10) <0.001
Ethnicity, n (%)
    White d 80 (78) 86 (85) 108 (95) 98 (97) 372 (89) <0.001 341 (34)
    Other e 22 (22) 15 (15) 5 (5) 3 (3) 45 (11) 662 (65)
Male, n (%) 77 (76) 55 (55) 43 (38) 77 (76) 252 (60) 509 (50) <0.001
Diagnosis (ICD-10),12 n (%) <0.001
    Schizophrenia 93 (91) 75 (74) 0 (0) 0 (0) 168 (40) 457 (45)
    Bipolar disorder 3 (3) 26 (26) 0 (0) 0 (0) 29 (7) 166 (16) <0.001
    Major depression 6 (6) 0 (0) 82 (73) 0 (0) 88 (21) 299 (30) <0.001
    Substance misuse 0 (0) 0 (0) 1 (1) 101 (100) 102 (25) 250 (25)
    Anxiety 34 (3)
    Other diagnosis 0 (0) 0 (0) 30 (26) 0 (0) 30 (7) 40 (4)
More than two hospitalisations, n (%) 84 (82) 50 (50) 38 (34) 2 (2) 174 (42) <0.001 624 (62) <0.001
Duration of psychiatric history, years: mean (s.d.) 17.0 (9.26) 16.7 (10.78) 14.6 (11.86) 11.5 (8.63) 15.0 (10.45) <0.001 21 (11.6) <0.001
Number of out-patient visits in the past month, mean (s.d.) 6.3 (7.85) 1.3 (1.73) 1.1 (1.66) 2.2 (1.34) 2.7 (4.58) <0.001 4.4 (4.8) <0.001
CAGE Positive for drug or alcohol, n (%) 18 (18) 20 17 (15) 76 (75) 131 (31) <0.001 214 (21) <0.001
ITAQ total score, mean (s.d.) 15.5 (5.61) 17.7 (3.70) 17.6 (3.38) 16.5 (2.98) 16.9 (4.11) <0.001 18.4 (3.8) <0.001
BPRS, mean (s.d.) 34.2 (7.50) 32.9 (9.74) 35.8 (8.29) 29.1 (7.04) 33.0 (8.45) <0.001 32.4 (8.4) <0.05
GAF, mean (s.d.) 56.0 (11.69) 57.1 (14.54) 55.2 (14.53) 59.6 (11.13) 56.9 (13.18) 47.8 (9.4) <0.001

CMHT, community mental health team; ITAQ, Insight and Treatment Attitudes Questionnaire; BPRS, Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale; GAF, Global Assessment of Functioning.

a All percentages do not always add up, because of missing data.

b Comparison of baseline characteristic across four samples; Kruskall–Wallis test.

c Comparison of baseline characteristic between English and US samples; two-proportion Z-test.

d For US sample this category is ‘White or other’.

e For US sample this category is ‘Black’.

Table 2 Types of leverage received by patients in community mental healthcare across the four samples

Assertive outreach group (n = 102) CMHT psychosis group (n = 101) CMHT non- psychosis group (n = 113) Substance misuse group (n = 101) Total sample (n = 417) US sample (Monahan et al)4 (n = 1011)
Total, n n (%) Total, n n (%) Total, n n (%) Total, n n (%) P a Total, n n (%) Total, n n (%) P b
Experienced housing leverage (among all participants) 102 19 (19) 101 22 (22) 113 13 (12) 101 44 (44) <0.001 417 98 (24) 1011 325 (32) <0.01
Experienced financial leverage
    Among all participants 102 3 (3) 101 2 (2) 113 0 (0) 101 5 (5) 417 10 (2) 1011 121 (12) <0.001
    Among participants with a financial guardian or someone unofficially helping them with money 48 3 (6) 46 2 (4) 42 0 (0) 29 5 (17) 165 10 (6) 519 121 (32) <0.001
Experienced leverage through criminal justice system
    Among all participants 102 13 (13) 101 7 (7) 113 5 (4) 101 37 (37) 417 62 (15) 1011 230 (23) <0.001
    Among participants who had ever been arrested or on parole 63 13 (21) 38 7 (18) 39 5 (13) 92 37 (40) 232 62 (26) 512 230 (45) <0.001
Experienced childcare leverage
    Among all participants 102 6 (6) 101 6 (6) 113 6 (5) 101 16 (16) 417 34 (8)
    Among participants who reported having children under 18 20 6 (30) 24 6 (25) 37 6 (16) 49 16 (33) 130 34 (26)
Experienced any type of leverage ever 102 34 (33) 101 30 (30) 113 17 (15) 101 64 (63) <0.001 417 145 (35) 1011 519 (51) <0.001
Experienced more than one leverage 102 7 (7) 101 5 (5) 113 6 (5) 101 33 (33) <0.001 417 51 (12)

CMHT, community mental health team.

a Comparison of experienced leverage across four samples; Kruskall—Wallis test.

b Comparison of experienced leverage in English and US samples; two-proportion Z-test.

multivariate analysis for independent accommodation, substance misuse diagnosis and a history of imprisonment.

Discussion

England–USA differences

This is the first study of leverage in England and demonstrates that it is common in public mental health services for severely ill patients. A third reported feeling coerced to adhere to treatment at some time in their lives. These levels are substantially lower than those from the USA (35% compared with 51%). This difference is more pronounced when childcare and access leverage is removed (overall leverage 31% of English participants, 9% with more than one form) as this was not reported in the US reference paper, although it has been reported subsequently from the same sample. Reference Busch and Redlich13 Formal coercion in the community was available in the USA but not in the English context at the time of the study. It was experienced by 12–20% of the US sample Reference Monahan, Redlich, Swanson, Robbins, Appelbaum and Petrila4,Reference Swartz, Swanson, Kim and Petrila14 but, as it is formal coercion, is not included in our comparison with the US study.

Table 3 Univariate associations of leverages and baseline characteristicsa

Housing leverage OR (95% CI) Financial leverage OR (95% CI) Leverage through criminal justice, OR (95% CI) Childcare leverage OR (95% CI)
Ethnicity, White 0.8 (0.3–1.8) 0.1 ** (0.03–0.5) 0.3 (0.1–1.2)
Marital status, single v. others 1.8 * (1.1–3.1) 5.7 (0.3–114.8) 0.8 (0.2–2.5) 1.6 (0.7–3.5)
If children under 18 years, they live: with a separated partner 2.6 * (1.04–6.4) 0.2 (0.02–2.0) 1.9 (0.8–4.6)
Accommodation, independent v. other 0.3 *** (0.2–0.6) 1.2 (0.2–8.7) 1.4 (0.5–4.2) 0.9 (0.3–2.3)
If independent, live with: alone v. other 1.2 (0.7–2.1) 3.1 (0.1–125.3) 0.1 * (0.03–0.7) 1.3 (0.5–3.4)
Diagnosis (ICD-10) (compared with those with schizophrenia)
    Bipolar disorder 0.4 (0.1–1.4) 4.9 × 10–13 (0–∞) 1.1 (0.1–13.8) 1.9 (0.4–10.3)
    Major depression 0.5 (0.2–1.1) 4.3 × 10–13 (0–∞) 2.1 × 1012 (0–∞) 0.6 (0.2–1.9)
    Substance misuse 2.7 *** (1.5–4.6) 9.0 (0.8–100.2) 2.3 (0.8–6.6) 1.2 (0.5–3.3)
    Other diagnosis 0.4 (0.1–1.4) 4.3 × 10–13 (0–∞) 2.1 × 1012 (0–∞) 2.4 × 10–13 (0–∞)
Age at first contact with psychiatric services 0.5 * (0.3–0.9) 0.6 (0.1–3.5) 0.8 (0.3–2.2) 0.5 (0.2–1.1)
Number of out-patient visits in the past month 1.1 (0.7–2.0) 1.2 (0.2–10.0) 0.2 * (0.04–0.9) 1.3 (0.6–2.9)
CAGE: positive for drug or alcohol 1.80 * (1.0–3.2) 2.50 (0.46–13.65) 1.03 (0.27–3.91) 1.52 (0.69–3.37)

a Table 3 contains information only for variables that were significant for any type of leverage.

* P < 0.05

** P < 0.01

*** P < 0.001.

Table 4 Multivariate and univariate associations with receiving any type of leverage and more than one leveragea

Univariate Multivariate
Any leverage OR (95% CI) More than one type of leverage, OR (95% CI) Any leverage OR (95% CI) More than one type of leverage, OR (95% CI)
Children under 18 years old, yes 1.4 (0.9–2.2) 1.9 * (1.0–3.5) - 1.5 (0.7–2.8)
Accommodation, independent v. other 0.4 ** (0.2–0.7) 0.4 ** (0.2–0.8) 0.53 (0.3–1.0) 0.5 * (0.2–0.9)
Diagnosis (ICD-10) (compared with those with schizophrenia)
    Bipolar disorder 0.5 (0.2–1.2) 1.0 (0.2–4.5) 0.82 (0.3–2.5) 1.4 (0.3–6.8)
    Major depression 0.4 ** (0.2–0.7) 1.0 (0.3–2.6) 0.6 (0.3–1.3) 1.2 (0.4–3.5)
    Substance misuse 3.0 *** (1.8–5.1) 6.2 *** (3.0–12.8) 5.7 *** (2.2–15.1) 3.0 * (1.3–7.2)
    Other diagnosis 0.5 (0.2–1.4) 0.4 (0.1–3.6) 0.7 (0.2–2.3) 0.5 (0.1–4.3)
Age at first contact with psychiatric services 0.5 ** (0.3–0.8) 0.5 (0.3–1.0) 0.6 (0.4–1.0)
More than two hospitalisations 3.3 *** (1.8–6.0) 2.6 (0.9–6.9) 2.8 ** (1.4–5.6)
Hospitalisations ever 1.8 * (1.1–3.0) 1.5 (0.6–3.5) 1.1 (0.6–2.3)
Duration of psychiatric history, years 1.9 ** (1.2–2.9) 1.9 (0.98–3.5) 1.3 (0.8–2.2)
Criminal convictions ever, yes/no 2.5 *** (1.6–4.1) 3.2 ** (1.5–6.8) 1.4 (0.7–2.6) 1.4 (0.6–3.4)
Imprisonment ever, yes/no 2.7 *** (1.6–4.6) 3.4 *** (1.7–6.7) 1.8 (0.9–3.7) 2.4 * (1.1–5.2)
ITAQ total score 2.0 ** (1.2–3.1) 0.9 (0.5–1.8) 2.0 * (1.1–3.4)
CAGE: positive for drugs or alcohol 1.6 (0.96–2.7) 2.6 * (1.2–5.4) 1.6 (0.8–3.4)

ITAQ, Insight and Treatment Attitudes Questionnaire.

a Table 4 contains information only for variables that were significant in univariate analysis.

* P < 0.05

** P < 0.01

*** P < 0.001.

Clinical group differences

Leverage varied considerably across the clinical groups in the manner anticipated. The younger, substance misuse group reported the highest level of leverage; higher than the US group (63% compared with 51%). Levels fell according to the general severity of the groups through the psychosis groups to the non-psychosis CMHT group.

Types of leverage

The highest rate of leverage was related to accommodation. A quarter of the sample reported this (32% in the USA). It was substantially higher in the substance misuse group (44%), presumably reflecting the practice of many landlords (both private and public sector) of demanding attendance at drug programmes as a condition of tenancy. This is much less common in mental illness services, particularly in private accommodation. The 20% rate found in participants with psychosis is restricted to hostel and supervised accommodation. We found evidence of leverage by private landlords in the mental illness samples.

The USA has a highly developed system of ‘drug courts’ and, more recently, ‘mental health courts’ Reference Redlich, Steadman, Monahan, Robbins and Petrila15 exclusively trying and sentencing individuals with behavioural and mental health problems. England utilises a range of ‘court diversion’ schemes where individuals are identified pre-trial and diverted into the healthcare system. In the USA the coercion is explicit in the sentencing procedure –‘choose treatment or punishment’. The less formal English approach may mean that potential coercion is not registered.

The most striking difference between the two national samples is in the use of money as leverage. Over half of the US sample (n = 519) had a financial guardian or someone unofficially managing their money. Of these, 121 (32%) participants reported that receiving it had depended on their accepting treatment. In the English sample, 165 (40%) reported that they had someone managing their money but only 10 (6%) experienced it as leverage. Money management remains a controversial issue in England and few teams use it or feel comfortable with it. Half (n = 5) of those reporting it were in the substance misuse group and presumably this was family pressure as the clinical teams did not use it. Some AOTs in England are now experimenting with its use. Reference Burns and Firn16

Baseline characteristics associated with leverage

We failed to find the strong association between leverage and patient characteristics as in the USA, probably because of their larger sample and greater clinical variation. Our only significant associations were with a substance misuse diagnosis, more than two hospitalisations and a lower score on the insight and treatment attitudes measure (ITAQ).

Limitations

Our study has several limitations. It reports ‘experience’ of leverage without third-party corroboration. Some of the leverages reported would probably have been disputed by the clinicians. Leverage may also have been applied but not recognised as such. Szmukler & Appelbaum have outlined a gradation of informal coercion and many staff may strive to recast leverage as persuasion. Reference Szmukler and Appelbaum17 We were also obliged to use differing sampling strategies for the different teams but it is unclear what effects this would exert. Our refusal rate is also more than twice that in the US sample (31% compared with 2–15%). The reasons for this are unclear and it cannot be discounted that participants perceiving higher levels of coercion might be more likely to refuse, thereby reducing the reported English levels. Higher levels of leverage were demonstrated to be associated with committed acts of violence (18–21% in the preceding 6 months) in the US sample. Reference Swanson, Van Dorn, Monahan and Swartz18 In the absence of comparable violence data in the English sample we cannot estimate if it might account for the reported leverage differences. We are unable to draw any conclusions as to the efficacy of leverage in this cross-sectional study.

Implications

Despite these limitations we believe our results convey a broadly accurate picture of the experience of leverage in these patient groups. In the mental illness groups leverage rates are associated with increasing severity of illness. The high levels in substance misusers reflect a tougher negotiating approach to individuals generally perceived to have more control over their behaviour. English staff assume that severely ill individuals are ‘entitled’ to a wide range of social benefits (housing, financial support, etc). In the USA, benefits are rarely viewed as automatic entitlements, but are targeted where most likely to achieve effect. The participants in the US study were, however, generally in receipt of these benefits and it is possible that higher reported perceived coercion may reflect the more widespread sense of entitlement in the English sample rather than any higher actual leverage in the US sample.

Overall, the pattern is more similar than different between the USA and England. A substantial proportion of individuals in public mental healthcare do not feel entirely free in their interactions with services. Although some pressure comes from family and friends, many experience healthcare staff as coercing them. Informal coercion is as widespread, if not more so, than formal coercion. There is a pressing need for research to understand it both to inform policy and to train staff who are clearly using it routinely.

Appendix

Questions used to identify experienced leverage

Finance

Did anyone (financial manager/guardian) ever make giving you your money, or giving you spending money, depend on whether you did what he or she wanted in terms of getting mental health, alcohol or drug treatment (or taking medication)?

Housing

Have you ever lived somewhere where you were required to stay in mental health or substance use treatment (or required to continue taking your medication) to keep living there (including family home)?

Have you ever been told that obtaining new accommodation is dependent on you taking treatment?

Criminal justice

Has anyone in the legal system ever told you or your lawyer that the charges will be dropped or reduced if you get treatment in the community for your mental health, alcohol or drug problems?

Has a mental health professional, or anyone in the legal system, or their report, suggested that you take treatment for a mental health problem as a condition of not going to prison?

Child custody

Have you ever been told that your children might be taken into care if you did not participate in mental health, alcohol or drug treatment (or taking your medication)?

Have you ever been told that your access to see your children would be reduced if you did not participate in mental health, alcohol or drug treatment (or taking your medication)?

Footnotes

See editorial, pp. 90–91, this issue.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Kjellin, L, Hoyer, G, Engberg, M, Kaltiala-Heino, R, Sigurjonsdottir, M. Difference in perceived coercion at admission to psychiatric hospitals in the Nordic countries. Psychiatr Epidemiol 2006; 41: 241–7.Google Scholar
2 Dawson, J. Community Treatment Orders: International Comparisons. Otago University, 2005.Google Scholar
3 Hoge, SK, Lidz, CW, Eisenberg, M, Gardner, W, Monahan, J, Mulvey, E, et al. Perceptions of coercion in the admission of voluntary and involuntary psychiatric patients. Int J Law Psychiatry 1997; 20: 167–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Monahan, J, Redlich, AD, Swanson, J, Robbins, PC, Appelbaum, PS, Petrila, J, et al. Use of leverage to improve adherence to psychiatric treatment in the community. Psychiatr Serv 2005; 56: 3744.Google Scholar
5 Clark Robbins, P, Petrila, J, LeMelle, S, Monahan, J. The use of housing as leverage to increase adherence to psychiatric treatment in the community. Admin Policy Ment Health 2006; 33: 226–36.Google Scholar
6 Redlich, AD, Steadman, HJ, Clark Robbins, P, Swanson, JW. Use of the criminal justice system to leverage mental health treatment: effects on treatment adherence and satisfaction. J Am Acad Psychiatry Law 2006; 34: 292–9.Google ScholarPubMed
7 Endicott, J, Spitzer, RL, Fleiss, JL, Cohen, J. The Global Assessment Scale. A procedure for measuring overall severity of psychiatric disturbance. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1976; 33: 766–71.Google Scholar
8 Overall, JE, Gorham, DL. The Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale. Psychol Rep 1962; 10: 799812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
9 McEvoy, JP, Freter, SMS, Everett, G, Geller, JL, Appelbaum, P, Apperson, LJRL. Insight and the clinical outcome of schizophrenic patients. J Nerv Ment Dis 1989; 177: 4851.Google Scholar
10 Erwing, JA. Detecting alcoholism. The CAGE questionnaire. JAMA 1984; 252: 1905–7.Google Scholar
11 Hox, J. Applied Multilevel Analysis. TT-Publikaties, 1995.Google Scholar
12 World Health Organization. The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. WHO, 1992.Google Scholar
13 Busch, A, Redlich, A. Patients' perception of possible child custody or visitation loss if not adherent to psychiatric treatment. Psychiatr Serv 2007; 58: 9991002.Google Scholar
14 Swartz, MS, Swanson, JW, Kim, M, Petrila, J. Use of outpatient commitment or related civil court treatment orders in five U.S. communities. Psychiatr Serv 2006; 57: 343–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Redlich, AD, Steadman, HJ, Monahan, J, Robbins, PC, Petrila, J. Patterns of practice in mental health courts: a national survey. Law Hum Behav 2006; 30: 347–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
16 Burns, T, Firn, M. Assertive Outreach in Mental Health: A Manual for Practitioners. Oxford University Press, 2002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
17 Szmukler, G, Appelbaum, P. Treatment pressures, leverage, coercion and compulsion in mental health care. J Ment Health 2008; 17: 233–44.Google Scholar
18 Swanson, JW, Van Dorn, RA, Monahan, J, Swartz, MS. Violence and leveraged community treatment for persons with mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2006; 163: 1404–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Sociodemographic characteristics across the four samplesa

Figure 1

Table 2 Types of leverage received by patients in community mental healthcare across the four samples

Figure 2

Table 3 Univariate associations of leverages and baseline characteristicsa

Figure 3

Table 4 Multivariate and univariate associations with receiving any type of leverage and more than one leveragea

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.