Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T23:30:36.628Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Psychosis reactivity to cannabis use in daily life: an experience sampling study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Cécile Henquet*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, South Limburg Mental Health Research and Teaching Network, Maastricht, and Mondriaan Zorggroep, Division Addiction Care, South Limburg, The Netherlands
Jim van Os
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, South Limburg Mental Health Research and Teaching Network, Maastricht, The Netherlands, and Division of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Kings College, London, UK
Rebecca Kuepper
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, South Limburg Mental Health Research and Teaching Network, Maastricht, The Netherlands
Philippe Delespaul
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, South Limburg Mental Health Research and Teaching Network, Maastricht, and Mondriaan Zorggroep, South Limburg, The Netherlands
Maurice Smits
Affiliation:
Mondriaan Zorggroep, South Limburg, The Netherlands
Joost À Campo
Affiliation:
Mondriaan Zorggroep, South Limburg, The Netherlands
Inez Myin-Germeys
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, EURON, Maastricht University Medical Centre, South Limburg Mental Health Research and Teaching Network, Maastricht, The Netherlands, and School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester, UK
*
Dr Cécile Henquet, Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Maastricht University Medical Centre, PO BOX 616 (Loc. Vijverdal), 6200 MD Maastricht, The Netherlands. Email: cecile.henquet@sp.unimaas.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Little is known about the experiential dynamics of the interaction between cannabis and vulnerability to psychosis.

Aims

To examine the effects of cannabis on psychotic symptoms and mood in patients with psychosis and healthy controls.

Method

Patients with a psychotic disorder (η = 42) and healthy controls (η = 38) were followed in their daily lives using a structured time-sampling technique.

Results

Daily life cannabis use predicted subsequent increases in positive affect and in patients, but not in controls, decreases in negative affect. In patients, but not in controls, cannabis use predicted increased levels of hallucinatory experiences. Mood-enhancing properties of cannabis were acute, whereas psychosis-inducing effects were sub-acute. There was no direct evidence for self-medication effects in daily life.

Conclusions

Patients with psychosis are more sensitive to both the psychosis-inducing and mood-enhancing effects of cannabis. The temporal dissociation between acute rewarding effects and sub-acute toxic influences may be instrumental in explaining the vicious circle of deleterious use in these patients.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2010 

Cannabis use is associated with an increased risk of psychotic symptoms in individuals from the general population. Reference Murray, Morrison, Henquet and Di Forti1 In patients with an established psychotic disorder, cannabis has a negative impact on illness course, as evidenced by more and earlier relapses, more frequent hospitalisations, and poorer psychosocial functioning. Reference Grech, Van Os, Jones, Lewis and Murray2Reference Linszen, Dingemans and Lenior4 Other epidemiological work, however, has suggested that cannabis may reduce negative and affective symptoms in patients with schizophrenia. Reference Compton, Furman and Kaslow5 Studies using self-report questionnaires to investigate motives that maintain cannabis use among individuals with psychotic disorders indicate that the principal motives for use in this group are enhancement of positive affect, social acceptance and coping with negative affect. Reference Spencer, Castle and Michie6 Additional evidence that patients may use cannabis to self-medicate distress was reported in a population-based study where vulnerability for psychosis (measured by means of a questionnaire) predicted future cannabis use in those who had never used cannabis before the onset of psychotic symptoms. Reference Ferdinand, Sondeijker, van der Ende, Selten, Huizink and Verhulst7 Two earlier population-based studies, however, had found no evidence for such reverse causality. Reference Henquet, Krabbendam, Spauwen, Kaplan, Lieb and Wittchen8,Reference Stefanis, Delespaul, Henquet, Bakoula, Stefanis and Van Os9 In order to design successful interventions, epidemiological designs may not be sufficient to provide full insight into the complicated dynamics of cannabis use and its varied effects in patients with psychosis. The current study was therefore designed to further examine, in the context of these complicated dynamics, the association between cannabis use and psychosis using a momentary assessment method. The aims of the current study were to investigate whether:

  1. (a) frequency of cannabis use in daily life fluctuates as a function of mood and psychotic symptom level (i.e. self-medication effects);

  2. (b) cannabis use is associated with subsequent changes in mood and psychotic symptom level;

  3. (c) patients with a psychotic disorder differ from healthy controls in their sensitivity to the psychosis-inducing effects of cannabis; and

  4. (d) temporal dynamics of cannabis-induced symptoms are apparent (i.e. short- v. long-lasting effects of cannabis can be discerned).

Method

Participants

The study sample consisted of 48 patients with a clinical diagnosis of a psychotic disorder and 47 healthy controls. The patients and controls were all frequent cannabis users (current use of at least three times per week). Patients were recruited through in-patient and out-patient mental health service facilities in South Limburg, The Netherlands, and controls were recruited from local ‘coffee shops’ (cafés where cannabis is sold and consumed legally). Participants were provided with a complete description of the study and written informed consent was obtained. The study was carried out in accordance with the World Medical Association's (WMA's) Declaration of Helsinki on Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects, as adopted by the 52nd WMA General Assembly, Edinburgh, October 2000. The study was approved by the standing medical ethics committee of Maastricht University Medical Centre. Interview data were used to complete the Operational Criteria Checklist for Psychotic Illness (OPCRIT), Reference McGuffin, Farmer and Harvey10 yielding diagnoses according to Research Diagnostic Criteria (RDC) Reference Spitzer, Endicott and Robins11 through the OPCRIT computer program for Windows. In addition, current symptomatology was assessed with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler12 and additional information with regard to current psychiatric illness, past and current substance use, and other demographic information was collected. Exclusion criteria were: respiratory, cardiovascular or neurological disease and alcohol use in excess of 5 units per day. Pregnant women were also excluded. A personal or family history of psychosis or use of antipsychotic medication was a further exclusion criterion for the control group.

Experience sampling method

The experience sampling method (ESM) is a pseudo-random time sampling self-assessment technique. Previous applications of ESM have demonstrated feasibility, validity and reliability in patients with schizophrenia. Reference Myin-Germeys, van Os, Schwartz, Stone and Delespaul13,Reference Myin-Germeys, Krabbendam, Jolles, Delespaul and van Os14 Participants received a digital wristwatch, and a paper-and-pen ESM booklet. Twelve times a day on six consecutive days, the watch emitted a beep at random moment in each 85-minute time block between 07.30 h and 00.30 h. After each beep, participants were asked to fill in a self-assessment form, collecting reports of affect, thoughts, severity of symptoms and activity rated on 7-point Likert scales at the moment of the beep. Participants were instructed to complete the form immediately after the beep to minimise memory distortion and to record the time at which they completed their report. During a briefing session, the ESM procedure was explained and a practice booklet was completed and discussed with the participant to confirm that the scale format was accurately understood. During the ESM week, participants were contacted by phone in order to ensure that they complied with the instructions. Participants were requested not to use illicit drugs other than cannabis during the 6 consecutive study days; however, no sanctions were involved. None of the participants was a regular user of drugs other than cannabis. During the telephone contacts, participants were asked about their use of other drugs and one person admitted to having used cocaine on the third day of the ESM week. Reports following this cocaine use were excluded from the analyses. At each beep when forms were completed, participants were asked to report the exact time. Reports completed more than 5 mins before and 15 mins after the beep were excluded from the analyses. This was done because previous research has shown that remote answers are less reliable and less valid than reports at the exact moment of the beep. Reference Delespaul and de Vries15 Participants with less than 24 valid reports were excluded from the analyses, as previous work has shown that measures of individuals with less than 30% of completed reports are less reliable. Reference Delespaul and de Vries15

Measures

Measures regarding cannabis use, mood, and psychotic symptoms were derived from the ESM reports as described below.

Assessment of cannabis use

Cannabis use, reported after each beep, referred to the period between the previous beep and the current beep (cannabis use, a binary variable). Similarly, cannabis useprevious referred to cannabis use during the period between the previous beep and the beep before that. Alcohol use, reported after each beep, referred to the period between the previous beep and the current beep (alcohol use, a binary variable).

Assessment of mood

Mood states were assessed with 11 mood adjectives rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very) reported after each beep, referring to the mood state at that moment (e.g. ‘at this moment I feel anxious’). In previous ESM studies Reference Myin-Germeys, van Os, Schwartz, Stone and Delespaul13,Reference Myin-Germeys, Krabbendam, Jolles, Delespaul and van Os14 with similar populations (patients with psychosis and healthy controls), a positive and a negative mood scale were identified with factor analysis on the raw within-participant scores of the mood items. For the current analyses, therefore, the mean of the adjectives ‘cheerful’, ‘relaxed’, ‘happy’, ‘satisfied’, ‘enthusiastic’, ‘overall good’ formed the positive affect scale (a continuous variable, Cronbach's α = 0.89), and the mean of ‘insecure’, ‘lonely’, ‘anxious’, ‘blue’, ‘guilty’ formed the negative affect scale (a continuous variable, Cronbach's α = 0.80). Positive affectprevious and negative affectprevious referred to mood states reported at the previous beep.

Assessment of psychosis

Positive psychotic symptoms were assessed with seven items rated on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 7 = very) reported after each beep, referring to the psychotic experiences at that moment. In order to allow self-reporting of psychotic experiences, the items assessing delusional ideation include aspects that can be associated directly with concrete positive psychotic experiences, rather than interpretations of these experiences (e.g. ‘at the moment my thoughts are being controlled by others’). Guided by previous studies Reference Myin-Germeys, Marcelis, Krabbendam, Delespaul and van Os16,Reference Van Winkel, Henquet, Rosa, Papiol, Fananas and De Hert17 the mean of ‘preoccupied thoughts’, ‘racing thoughts’, ‘difficulty expressing thoughts’, ‘thoughts controlled by others’ and ‘suspicious’ formed the delusions scale (a continuous variable, Cronbach's α = 0.72). Hallucinatory experiences were assessed directly (e.g. ‘at the moment I'm hearing voices’) as it has been shown in previous studies that patients can distinguish between hearing real voices and verbal hallucinations. Reference Delespaul, deVries and van Os18 The mean of ‘seeing things’ and ‘hearing things’ formed the hallucinations scale (a continuous variable, Cronbach's α = 0.70). Delusionsprevious and hallucinationsprevious referred to psychotic symptom severity reported at the previous beep.

Statistical analyses

As ESM data have a hierarchical structure with multiple reports (level 1) nested within participants (level 2), multilevel random regression analyses were conducted to account for the variability associated with the two different levels. The odds ratios (for dichotomous variables) and betas (for continuous variables) of these regression analyses are the associations between the independent and dependent variables in the multilevel model. The STATA (version 10 for Windows) multilevel regression XTGEE routine was used for dichotomous variables, and the XTREG routine for continuous variables.

Overall symptom levels

Differences between patients and controls in overall symptom level were investigated using group (0 = controls; 1 = patients) as independent and negative affect, positive affect, delusions and hallucinations as dependent variables. To investigate whether patients and controls differed in overall symptom level, multilevel regression analyses were conducted using group as the independent variable, and using negative affect, positive affect, delusions and hallucinations as dependent variables in consecutive models.

Self-medication effects

Self-medication effects were examined using negative affectprevious, positive affectprevious, delusionsprevious and hallucinations previous as independent variables, and cannabis use as the dependent variable (Fig. 1, analysis A).

Fig. 1 Experience sampling analysis

A, analysis to investigate self-medication effects with negative affectprevious, positive affectprevious, delusionsprevious and hallucinationsprevious as independent variables and cannabis use as the dependent variable; B, analysis to investigate main effects of cannabis use with cannabis use as the independent variable and negative affect, positive affect, delusions and hallucinations as dependent variables; C, analyses (post-hoc in the patient group exclusively) to differentiate between sub-acute (C) and acute (B) effects of cannabis use on mood and positive symptoms.

Cannabis effects in patients v. controls

Main effects of cannabis use on subsequent symptom levels were investigated with cannabis use as independent, and negative affect, positive affect, delusions and hallucinations as dependent variables (Fig. 1, analysis B). In order to test the hypothesis that patients and controls differed in their sensitivity to cannabis effects, multilevel regression analyses were conducted with cannabis use and group as well as their interaction term as independent variables and with negative affect, positive affect, delusions and hallucinations as dependent variables. Regression model: ESM symptom level = β0+ β1(cannabis use) + β2(group) + β3(ESM cannabis use×group). This cannabis×group interaction was then fitted to allow estimation of cannabis effect sizes for both groups separately by calculating the appropriate linear combinations with the LINCOM command in STATA.

Temporal dynamics of cannabis effects

Based on the results of the previous analyses, post-hoc analyses were conducted to further investigate the duration of cannabis effects in the patient group only. To investigate these temporal dynamics, cannabis use (reported at the current beep) and cannabis useprevious (reported at the previous beep) were entered simultaneously in the same model predicting negative affect, positive affect and hallucinations (Fig. 1, analysis C).

All analyses were a priori adjusted for age, gender, alcohol use and overall level of cannabis use during the ESM week. The analyses investigating self-medication effects were additionally adjusted for cannabis use reported at the previous beep, given the fact that cannabis use was strongly associated with cannabis useprevious (OR = 3.66, 95% CI 3.05–4.40, P<0.001). In order to account for possible self-medication effects (i.e. symptom level reported at the previous beep causing an increase in symptoms and cannabis use reported at the current beep) the analyses investigating main effects of cannabis use on symptom levels were additionally adjusted for negative affectprevious, positive affectprevious, delusionsprevious and hallucinationsprevious. Main effects and interaction were assessed by Wald test.

Results

Participants

Of the 95 participants included in the study, 9 controls and 6 patients had fewer than 24 valid reports and were excluded from the analyses. Drop-out was not associated with group (OR = 0.94, 95% CI 0.32–2.73, P = 0.91) or cannabis use during the ESM week (β = 1.01, 95% CI 0.88–1.16, P = 0.90). The final study sample consisted of 80 participants (42 patients and 38 controls; Tables 1 and 2). The OPCRIT diagnoses according to RDC were: schizophrenia (n = 10), schizoaffective disorder (n = 28), and unspecified functional psychosis (n = 4). Groups differed significantly in age, but not in gender (Table 1), or in lifetime or current frequency of cannabis use or lifetime use of other drugs (Table 3).

Table 1 Sociodemographics of the study sample

Controls (n = 38) Patients (n = 42)
Age, years: mean (s.d.) (range) 26.9 (7.5) (18-47)a 36.1 (9.3) (19-58)
Gender (M/F) 31/7b 31/11
Education, n
    Elementary school 3 8
    Secondary school 18 28
    Higher education 17 3
Living situation, n
    Alone 19 22
    With partner/own family 7 9
    With parents/other family 12 5
    Protected housing 6
Work situation, n
    Working (e.g. school, household) 27 4
    Unemployed 7 8
    Incapable of work 2 26
    Protected work 1

Table 2 Clinical variables across the study sample

Controls (n = 38) Patients (n = 42)
Total PANSS score,a mean (s.d.) 32.3 (2.3) 47.5 (11.6)
PANSS positive score, mean (s.d.) 7.4 (0.8) 12.1 (4.7)
PANSS negative score, mean (s.d.) 7.5 (1.1) 10.1 (3.4)
Age first psychotic episode, years: mean (s.d.) 22.5 (6.9)
Usual symptom severity (past 2 years), n
    Severe 3
    Mild to moderate 19
    Recovered 4
Current use of medication, n
    No antipsychotic medication 38 7
    Typical antipsychotics 12
    Atypical antipsychotics 23

Table 3 Substance use in the total sample

Controls (n = 38) Patients (n = 42) âa 95% CI P
Cannabis use
    Age first cannabis use, years: mean (s.d.) 16.9 (4.4) 18.0 (6.3) -1.22 -3.75 to 0.33 0.35
    Use more than once daily (during most heavy use), n 30 37 -0.24 -0.90 to 0.41 0.46
    Use more than once daily (during past 12 months), n 29 37 0.24 -0.31 to 1.29 0.39
Cocaine and/or stimulants use, n
    Lifetime use 22 27 0.04 -0.20 to 0.28 0.75
    Use during past 12 months 12 14 0.13 -0.10 to 0.37 0.26

Overall symptom levels

Overall, patients reported significantly lower levels of positive affect (β = –0.56, 95% CI –1.03 to –0.09, P = 0.019) and higher levels of negative affect than controls (β = 0.41, 95% CI 0.07 to 0.75, P = 0.020). Patients and controls differed significantly in the intensity of hallucinations they reported (β = 0.34, 95% CI 0.09 to 0.60, P = 0.008), but not in levels of delusions (β = 0.26, 95% CI –0.17 to 0.70, P = 0.24). Frequency of cannabis use during the ESM week was significantly higher in patients than in controls (OR = 1.76, 95% CI 1.12 to 2.77, P = 0.015).

Self-medication effects

Neither positive affectprevious nor negative affectprevious predicted cannabis use reported at the following beep (OR = 1.08, 95% CI 0.99–1.16, P = 0.09 and OR = 0.97, 95% CI 0.88–1.08, P = 0.64 respectively). Similarly, no association was found between delusionsprevious and subsequent cannabis use (OR = 0.99, 95% CI 0.890–1.06, P = 0.97) or hallucinationsprevious and cannabis use (OR = 0.96, 95% CI 0.83–1.11, P = 0.59).

Cannabis effects on mood in patients v. controls

Cannabis use was associated with subsequent increases in positive affect (β = 0.21, 95% CI 0.14 to 0.29, P<0.001). Overall, cannabis use had no effect on negative affect (β = –0.04, 95% CI –0.09 to 0.01, P = 0.12). The cannabis use×group interaction, however, was significant for negative affect. Thus, patients were more sensitive to the mood-enhancing effects of cannabis than controls (i.e. large and significant decreases in negative affect were observed after cannabis use in patients, but not in controls; cannabis× group interaction: χ2 = 6.43, d.f. = 1, P = 0.011; Table 4). No such interaction effect was observed for changes in positive affect after cannabis use (χ2 = 0.98, d.f. = 1, P = 0.32).

Table 4 Effects of cannabis use on subsequent symptom levels, patients v. controls

Cannabis -, mean (s.d.) Cannabis +, mean (s.d.) Cannabis effect sizea Group × cannabisb
Positive affect
    Controls 4.99 (1.14) 5.12 (1.17) β = 0.18, 95% CI 0.07 to 0.28; P = 0.01 χ2 = 0.98, d.f. = 1, P = 0.32
    Patients 4.30 (1.26) 4.46 (1.32) β = 0.24, 95% CI 0.15 to 0.35, P < 0.001
Negative affect
    Controls 1.35 (0.65) 1.29 (0.70) β = 0.03, 95% CI -0.05 to 0.10; P = 0.47 χ2 = 6.43, d.f. = 1, P = 0.011
    Patients 1.96 (1.16) 1.78 (0.96) β = -0.10, 95% CI -0.17 to -0.03, P = 0.0043
Delusions
    Controls 1.87 (0.82) 1.87 (0.89) β = 0.02, 95% CI -0.06 to 0.10; P = 0.70 χ2 = 1.11, d.f. = 1, P = 0.28
    Patients 2.47 (1.25) 2.45 (1.26) β = -0.05, 95% CI -0.12 to 0.03, P = 0.25
Hallucinations
    Controls 1.00 (0.07) 1.00 (0.13) β = 0.01, 95% CI -0.04 to 0.06; P = 0.75 χ2 = 3.66, d.f. = 1, P = 0.056
    Patients 1.38 (0.88) 1.40 (0.95) β = 0.08, 95% CI 0.03 to 0.13, P = 0.002
Auditory hallucinations
    Controls 1.00 (0.08) 1.01 (0.25) β = 0.01, 95% CI -0.06 to 0.08; P = 0.72 χ2 = 3.36, d.f. = 1, P = 0.067
    Patients 1.40 (1.03) 1.50 (1.21) β = 0.11, 95% CI 0.04 to 0.17, P = 0.003

Cannabis effects on psychosis in patients v. controls

Cannabis use was significantly associated with subsequent increases of hallucinations (β = 0.05, 95% CI 0.01–0.08, P = 0.015). No main effect of cannabis use was found for subsequent delusion intensity (β = –0.02, 95% CI –0.07 to 0.04, P = 0.58). Patients and controls differed in their sensitivity to the hallucinogenic effects of cannabis (cannabis×group interaction χ2 = 3.66, d.f. = 1, P = 0.056; Table 4). Patients reported significant increases in hallucinations (β = 0.08, 95% CI 0.03–0.13, P = 0.002), whereas the association between cannabis use and hallucinations was small and non-significant in controls (β = 0.01, 95% CI –0.04 to 0.06, P = 0.75). Further post-hoc analyses for the separate hallucination items showed that the cannabis effects in patients were particularly associated with auditory hallucinations (‘hearing voices’, β = 0.11, 95% CI 0.04–0.17, P = 0.003; Table 4) but that they were less clearly associated with visual hallucinations. No interaction effects were found for delusions (χ2 = 1.17, d.f. = 1, P = 0.28; Table 4).

Temporal dynamics of cannabis effects

A follow-up post-hoc analysis was conducted, assessing the duration of cannabis effects on mood and hallucinations in the patient group only, by entering both cannabis use and cannabis useprevious simultaneously in the same model. This suggested that increases in positive affect were observed in the short term (β = 0.31, 95% CI 0.19–0.43, P<0.001 for cannabis use) rather than the long term (β = 0.07, 95% CI –0.056 to 0.19, P = 0.28 for cannabis useprevious). For hallucinations, however, when both cannabis use and cannabis useprevious were entered simultaneously in the same model, increases in hallucinatory experiences were observed only in the long term (β = 0.09, 95% CI 0.01–0.17, P = 0.030 for cannabis useprevious) and not in the short term (β = 0.06, 95% CI –0.01 to 0.14, P = 0.11 for cannabis use). This suggests that the association between cannabis and hallucinatory experiences was most prominent after a longer period of time compared with its shorter-term mood-enhancing effects.

Discussion

Cannabis use in daily life was associated with subsequent increases in hallucinatory experiences, in particular auditory hallucinations. Patients with a psychotic disorder were more sensitive to the hallucinogenic effects of cannabis than healthy controls. Overall, cannabis enhanced mood, with patients being more sensitive to the positive effects of cannabis on negative affect (i.e. stronger decreases in negative affect after cannabis use). In addition, the data suggest that the positive effects of cannabis on mood are acute, whereas its association with psychotic experiences is sub-acute. Neither negative affect nor hallucinations nor delusions predicted cannabis use, arguing against self-medication effects in daily life. In addition, the effect on subsequent symptom level remained both large and significant after adjustment of symptom level at the previous assessment (i.e. symptoms preceding the use of cannabis).

The current momentary assessment data confirm epidemiological and experimental findings, Reference Van Os, Bak, Hanssen, Bijl, de Graaf and Verdoux19,Reference D'Souza, Abi-Saab, Madonick, Forselius-Bielen, Doersch and Braley20 showing that patients with a psychotic disorder are more sensitive to the psychosis-inducing effects of cannabis than healthy controls. Furthermore, the current findings extend this to the real world of everyday life. The effect sizes were small but the cumulative effect may be considerable, as exposure to cannabis and subsequent fluctuations in symptoms were observed several times a day over several days. The method of multiple assessments in daily life furthermore has the advantage of avoiding retrospective assessment of symptom states. These are most likely to be distorted by cannabis in the patient group, as increased sensitivity to cannabis operates not only at the symptom level, but also at the level of memory function. Reference D'Souza, Abi-Saab, Madonick, Forselius-Bielen, Doersch and Braley20 A crucial point in the interpretation of the results is the validity of the psychosis measures, especially because the results seem to indicate that hallucinations may be a more sensitive phenotype than delusions to study the acute effects of cannabis in daily life. Previous research has shown that patients can distinguish between hearing real voices and verbal hallucinations in self-report questionnaires, Reference Romme, Honig, Noorthoorn and Escher21 as well as between items such as preoccupation, suspicion and feeling as if controlled by others. Reference Delespaul and de Vries15 In addition, the psychosis items used in this study are related to the same construct of psychosis, which is shown by a Cronbach's alpha of 0.71. In a previous study, it was shown that these ESM hallucinations and delusions scores were significantly and specifically correlated with PANSS positive symptom scores (r = 0.45, P<0.001), but not with PANSS negative symptom scores (r = 0.26, P = 0.12). Reference Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and van Os22 Previous studies have also shown that ESM psychosis scores have discriminate validity, as they distinguish patients from first-degree relatives of patients with psychosis and healthy controls. Reference Myin-Germeys, Delespaul and van Os22 The fact that in the current study patients did not differ in overall delusion levels from the control participants, may be explained by the long-term psychosis-inducing effects of chronic cannabis use which have been consistently reported in the general population as well. Reference Henquet, Krabbendam, Spauwen, Kaplan, Lieb and Wittchen8,Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Poulton, Murray, Caspi and Moffitt23 Comparison with a previous ESM study, indeed, showed that in the current controls using cannabis overall delusion levels were higher than in other control participants not using cannabis. Reference Thewissen, Bentall, Lecomte, van Os and Myin-Germeys24

Increased addiction potential in patients

In agreement with two longitudinal studies on this topic, Reference Henquet, Krabbendam, Spauwen, Kaplan, Lieb and Wittchen8,Reference Stefanis, Delespaul, Henquet, Bakoula, Stefanis and Van Os9 the present data did not support direct mechanisms of self-medication, since cannabis use was not predicted by previous changes in symptom level or mood. However, cannabis did improve mood, particularly in patients. The combination of differential sensitivity in patients to the acute rewarding effects and the sub-acute negative influences of cannabis on psychotic symptoms (despite the fact that the majority of patients were using antipsychotic medication) may be helpful in explaining the model of cannabis use in patients with psychosis, as proposed by Spencer et al. Reference Spencer, Castle and Michie6 According to this model, use of cannabis is driven by expectations that individuals may have about the (acute) effects of cannabis. The sub-acute negative psychotic effects may then be experienced as evidence that more use is necessary to bring about the anticipated rewarding effects. Hallucinatory experiences are strongly associated with negative affect, Reference Delespaul, deVries and van Os18 fuelling further use in order to experience acute improvement in mood. Patients may be more sensitive to these mechanisms as they (a) have overall higher levels of negative affect and (b) are more sensitive to the mood- and psychosis-enhancing effects of cannabis. The motivation to enhance affect may consequently direct the individual to future use and cannabis dependence, despite the long-term negative impact cannabis may have on functional outcome. Reference Grech, Van Os, Jones, Lewis and Murray2,Reference Linszen, Dingemans and Lenior4 The finding that cannabis use was not predicted by changes in mood at the previous beep, could be explained by the fact that in the context of ongoing high levels of negative affect, patients may delay use until they find an appropriate time to smoke (with mood fluctuations occurring further back in the chain of events).

Biological plausibility

A reciprocal interaction between the endocannabinoid and dopamine system may explain the psychotogenic effects of cannabis in individuals with increased liability to psychosis such as the patient group. Reference Giuffrida, Leweke, Gerth, Schreiber, Koethe and Faulhaber25 In the central nervous system, THC (Δ-9-tetrahydrocannabinol, the psychoactive component of cannabis) binds to cannabinoid 1 (CB1) receptors, the primary receptor site for endocannabinoids. Endocannabinoids act as retrograde signals at central nervous system synapses, by activating presynaptic CB1 receptors. Reference Wilson and Nicoll26,Reference Elphick and Egertova27 Activation of CB1 receptors inhibits presynaptic neurotransmitter release. The endocannabinoid system thus plays an important role in the modulation of other neurotransmitters (e.g. gamma-aminobutyric acid and glutamate) and may thereby indirectly influence dopamine firing as well. Reference Chevaleyre, Takahashi and Castillo28 Exogenous cannabinoids such as THC, however, disrupt these subtle, fine-tuning effects of endocannabinoids. Reference Leweke and Koethe29 Animal research has shown that THC evokes burst-firing in the ventral tegmental area and thereby increases dopamine concentrations in striatal brain regions. Reference Cheer, Wassum, Heien, Phillips and Wightman30,Reference Riegel and Lupica31 The cannabis effects as presented in this study might thus be attributable to the effects of THC on dopaminergic neurotransmission. Striatal dopamine is thought to play a crucial role in attributing salience to stimuli in the environment. As a consequence, a hyperdopaminergic state may facilitate psychotic experiences by enabling false attribution of significance to ambiguous stimuli. Reference Kapur, Mizrahi and Li32 Auditory hallucinations may then be most prominent after exposure to THC as they occur in the moment, whereas delusions may be secondary interpretations of aberrant perceptions. Reference Bentall, Kinderman and Kaney33 Recently, the acute effects of THC on striatal dopamine were investigated for the first time in human healthy volunteers, using experimental positron emission tomography paradigms. Bossong et al found THC-induced striatal dopamine release, Reference Bossong, van Berckel, Boellaard, Zuurman, Schuit and Windhorst34 whereas Stokes and colleagues did not find such effects on dopamine transmission. Reference Stokes, Mehta, Curran, Breen and Grasby35 These divergent findings may be due to the fact that individuals differ in their sensitivity to THC, with individuals expressing high psychosis liability, such as patients with schizophrenia, being more vulnerable than healthy controls. Reference Murray, Morrison, Henquet and Di Forti1,Reference Henquet, Di Forti, Morrison, Kuepper and Murray36 Epidemiological work has now identified several factors that co-participate with THC in causing psychosis, Reference Arseneault, Cannon, Witton and Murray37 as pre-existing psychotic symptoms, Reference Henquet, Krabbendam, Spauwen, Kaplan, Lieb and Wittchen8 exposure to childhood trauma Reference Houston, Murphy, Adamson, Stringer and Shevlin38,Reference Harley, Kelleher, Clarke, Lynch, Arseneault and Connor39 and a functional polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Reference Caspi, Moffitt, Cannon, McClay, Murray and Harrington40,Reference Henquet, Rosa, Krabbendam, Papiol, Fananas and Drukker41 have been shown to moderate the effects of cannabis on psychosis outcome. The results of the current study further support the idea that gene–environment interactions underlie the cannabis–psychosis association by showing that individuals at increased risk for psychosis (such as patients) are more sensitive to both the psychosis-inducing and mood-enhancing effects of THC.

Limitations

Several limitations need to be taken into account. First, reports on cannabis use and symptoms were based on self-report and were not confirmed by, for example, urine analysis. However, as consumption of cannabis is legal in The Netherlands and 32–51% of the assessments were cannabis moments, underreporting of drug use is unlikely. Experience sampling method adherence research Reference Jacobs, Nicolson, Derom, Delespaul, van Os and Myin-Germeys42 has shown that individuals generally adhere very well with ESM procedures as carried out in the current investigation. Second, the validity of the reports may be challenged by negative effects of cannabis on cognition. The number of drop-outs due to invalid reports, however, was similar to that reported in a recent ESM adherence study. Reference Jacobs, Nicolson, Derom, Delespaul, van Os and Myin-Germeys42 Third, patients were on average 10 years older than controls, thus duration of cannabis use may have been higher in patients than in controls. Adjustment for age did not change the results significantly. However, it may be useful to match on age and duration of cannabis use in future ESM studies. Fourth, it was found recently that patients with a first episode of psychosis show a preference for higher potency cannabis compared with healthy controls. Reference Di Forti, Morgan, Dazzan, Pariante, Mondelli and Marques43 In the current study, however, type and potency of cannabis was not controlled for in the analyses. It is unlikely, however, that this explains the stronger effects of cannabis on psychosis in the patient group. This is because the percentage of THC of the cannabis available in The Netherlands is correlated with its price and on average patients paid lower prices than controls for the cannabis they used during this study (β = –1.40, 95% CI –2.85 to 0.05, P = 0.058). Nevertheless, in future ESM studies it would be interesting to include information on type and potency of cannabis (e.g. by means of hair samples, Reference Morgan and Curran44 especially given the differential effects of THC and cannabidiol on mood and psychotic symptoms. Reference Bhattacharyya, Morrison, Fusar-Poli, Martin-Santos, Borgwardt and Winton-Brown45 Fifth, participants were regular cannabis users, which raises the question of whether the results would generalise to less frequent use of cannabis. D'Souza et al Reference D'Souza, Ranganathan, Braley, Gueorguieva, Zimolo and Cooper46 found frequent users to be blunted to the acute psychotomimetic effects of Δ-9-THC. Epidemiological work however, has also shown that the long-term effects of cannabis may increase the risk for psychosis in a dose–response fashion, Reference Henquet, Krabbendam, Spauwen, Kaplan, Lieb and Wittchen8 a finding that may be suggestive of a sensitisation process. More research is needed to investigate the moderating effect of duration of previous exposure on the acute psychosis-inducing effects of Δ-9-THC.

Clinical implications

In patients, the clinical goal may be to intervene as early as possible to limit progression of the illness to more severe states associated with comorbidity of psychotic illness and cannabis misuse. Cannabis use is frequently a concern in treatment, yet it is often insufficiently discussed between patient and mental health provider. Since the consumption of cannabis and use-related activities play an important role in the everyday social lives of patients, it is of major importance to get better insight into the mechanisms and patterns of use. Some patients who were participating in this study reported that completion of the ESM booklets and the feedback they received afterwards on their patterns of use and related symptoms may have already changed their conceptions about cannabis use to some extent. This suggests that the data presented here may be of great clinical use, being applicable at the individual level as well. A combination of motivational interviewing and cognitive–behavioural therapy (in which not only the positive but also the affective symptoms are addressed) has been proposed to be most effective for patients with psychosis and comorbid substance misuse. Reference Barrowclough, Haddock, Tarrier, Lewis, Moring and O'Brien47 The experience sampling data described here validate this idea by emphasising the need to recognise individual differences in sensitivity to cannabis and addiction potential resulting from differences in (genetic) liability to psychosis.

Funding

C.H. was supported by the Dutch Medical Research Council (VENI grant). M.S. was supported by the Dutch Medical Research Council (OOG grant). I.M.-G. was supported by the Dutch Medical Research Council (VIDI grant) and by a 2006 NARSAD Young Investigator Award.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Murray, RM, Morrison, PD, Henquet, C, Di Forti, M. Cannabis, the mind and society: the hash realities. Nat Rev Neurosci 2007; 8: 885–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
2 Grech, A, Van Os, J, Jones, PB, Lewis, SW, Murray, RM. Cannabis use and outcome of recent onset psychosis. Eur Psychiatry 2005; 20: 349–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
3 Caspari, D. Cannabis and schizophrenia: results of a follow-up study. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1999; 249: 45–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
4 Linszen, DH, Dingemans, PM, Lenior, ME. Cannabis abuse and the course of recent-onset schizophrenic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994; 51: 273–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
5 Compton, MT, Furman, AC, Kaslow, NJ. Lower negative symptom scores among cannabis-dependent patients with schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: preliminary evidence from an African American first-episode sample. Schizophr Res 2004; 71: 61–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
6 Spencer, C, Castle, D, Michie, PT. Motivations that maintain substance use among individuals with psychotic disorders. Schizophr Bull 2002; 28: 233–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
7 Ferdinand, RF, Sondeijker, F, van der Ende, J, Selten, JP, Huizink, A, Verhulst, FC. Cannabis use predicts future psychotic symptoms, and vice versa. Addiction 2005; 100: 612–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
8 Henquet, C, Krabbendam, L, Spauwen, J, Kaplan, C, Lieb, R, Wittchen, HU, et al. Prospective cohort study of cannabis use, predisposition for psychosis, and psychotic symptoms in young people. BMJ 2005; 330: 11–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
9 Stefanis, NC, Delespaul, P, Henquet, C, Bakoula, C, Stefanis, CN, Van Os, J. Early adolescent cannabis exposure and positive and negative dimensions of psychosis. Addiction 2004; 99: 1333–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
10 McGuffin, P, Farmer, A, Harvey, I. A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness: development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1991; 48: 764–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
11 Spitzer, RL, Endicott, J, Robins, E. Research diagnostic criteria: rationale and reliability. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978; 35: 773–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
12 Kay, SR, Fiszbein, A, Opler, LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13: 261–76.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
13 Myin-Germeys, I, van Os, J, Schwartz, JE, Stone, AA, Delespaul, PA. Emotional reactivity to daily life stress in psychosis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001; 58: s113744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14 Myin-Germeys, I, Krabbendam, L, Jolles, J, Delespaul, PA, van Os, J. Are cognitive impairments associated with sensitivity to stress in schizophrenia? An experience sampling study. Am J Psychiatry 2002; 159: 443–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
15 Delespaul, PAEG. Empirical studies in ESM reliability and validity. In Assessing Schizophrenia in Daily Life: The Experience Sampling Method. (ed. de Vries, MW): 107. Maastricht University Press, 1995.Google Scholar
16 Myin-Germeys, I, Marcelis, M, Krabbendam, L, Delespaul, P, van Os, J. Subtle fluctuations in psychotic phenomena as functional states of abnormal dopamine reactivity in individuals at risk. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58: 105–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
17 Van Winkel, R, Henquet, C, Rosa, A, Papiol, S, Fananas, L, De Hert, M, et al. Evidence that the COMT(Val158Met) polymorphism moderates sensitivity to stress in psychosis: an experience-sampling study. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2008; 147: 10–7.Google Scholar
18 Delespaul, P, deVries, M, van Os, J. Determinants of occurrence and recovery from hallucinations in daily life. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2002; 37: 97104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
19 Van Os, J, Bak, M, Hanssen, M, Bijl, RV, de Graaf, R, Verdoux, H. Cannabis use and psychosis: a longitudinal population-based study. Am J Epidemiol 2002; 156: 319–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
20 D'Souza, DC, Abi-Saab, WM, Madonick, S, Forselius-Bielen, K, Doersch, A, Braley, G, et al. Delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol effects in schizophrenia: implications for cognition, psychosis, and addiction. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57: 594608.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
21 Romme, MA, Honig, A, Noorthoorn, EO, Escher, AD. Coping with hearing voices: an emancipatory approach. Br J Psychiatry 1992; 161: 99103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
22 Myin-Germeys, I, Delespaul, P, van Os, J. Behavioural sensitization to daily life stress in psychosis. Psychol Med 2005; 35: 733–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
23 Arseneault, L, Cannon, M, Poulton, R, Murray, R, Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE. Cannabis use in adolescence and risk for adult psychosis: longitudinal prospective study. BMJ 2002; 325: 1212–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
24 Thewissen, V, Bentall, RP, Lecomte, T, van Os, J, Myin-Germeys, I. Fluctuations in self-esteem and paranoia in the context of daily life. J Abnorm Psychol 2008; 117: 143–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
25 Giuffrida, A, Leweke, FM, Gerth, CW, Schreiber, D, Koethe, D, Faulhaber, J, et al. Cerebrospinal anandamide levels are elevated in acute schizophrenia and are inversely correlated with psychotic symptoms. Neuropsychopharmacology 2004; 29: 2108–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
26 Wilson, RI, Nicoll, RA. Endogenous cannabinoids mediate retrograde signalling at hippocampal synapses. Nature 2001; 410: 588–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
27 Elphick, MR, Egertova, M. The neurobiology and evolution of cannabinoid signalling. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2001; 356: 381408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
28 Chevaleyre, V, Takahashi, KA, Castillo, PE. Endocannabinoid-mediated synaptic plasticity in the CNS. Ann Rev Neurosci 2006; 29: 3776.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
29 Leweke, FM, Koethe, D. Cannabis and psychiatric disorders: it is not only addiction. Addict Biol 2008; 13: 264–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
30 Cheer, JF, Wassum, KM, Heien, ML, Phillips, PE, Wightman, RM. Cannabinoids enhance subsecond dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens of awake rats. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 4393–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
31 Riegel, AC, Lupica, CR. Independent presynaptic and postsynaptic mechanisms regulate endocannabinoid signaling at multiple synapses in the ventral tegmental area. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 11070–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
32 Kapur, S, Mizrahi, R, Li, M. From dopamine to salience to psychosis: linking biology, pharmacology and phenomenology of psychosis. Schizophr Res 2005; 79: 5968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
33 Bentall, RP, Kinderman, P, Kaney, S. The self, attributional processes and abnormal beliefs: towards a model of persecutory delusions. Behav Res Ther 1994; 32: 331–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
34 Bossong, MG, van Berckel, BN, Boellaard, R, Zuurman, L, Schuit, RC, Windhorst, AD, et al. Delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol induces dopamine release in the human striatum. Neuropsychopharmacology 2009; 34: 759–66.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
35 Stokes, PR, Mehta, MA, Curran, HV, Breen, G, Grasby, PM. Can recreational doses of THC produce significant dopamine release in the human striatum? NeuroImage 2009; 48: 186–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
36 Henquet, C, Di Forti, M, Morrison, P, Kuepper, R, Murray, RM. Gene-environment interplay between cannabis and psychosis. Schizophr Bull 2008; 34: 1111–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
37 Arseneault, L, Cannon, M, Witton, J, Murray, RM. Causal association between cannabis and psychosis: examination of the evidence. Br J Psychiatry 2004; 184: 110–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
38 Houston, JE, Murphy, J, Adamson, G, Stringer, M, Shevlin, M. Childhood sexual abuse, early cannabis use, and psychosis: testing an interaction model based on the National Comorbidity Survey. Schizophr Bull 2008; 34: 580–5.Google ScholarPubMed
39 Harley, M, Kelleher, I, Clarke, M, Lynch, F, Arseneault, L, Connor, D, et al. Cannabis use and childhood trauma interact additively to increase the risk of psychotic symptoms in adolescence. Psychol Med 2009; 9: 18.Google Scholar
40 Caspi, A, Moffitt, TE, Cannon, M, McClay, J, Murray, R, Harrington, H, et al. Moderation of the effect of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene: longitudinal evidence of a gene×environment interaction. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 57: 1117–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
41 Henquet, C, Rosa, A, Krabbendam, L, Papiol, S, Fananas, L, Drukker, M, et al. An experimental study of catechol-O-methyltransferase Val(158)Met moderation of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol-induced effects on psychosis and cognition. Neuropsychopharmacology 2006; 31: 2748–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
42 Jacobs, N, Nicolson, NA, Derom, C, Delespaul, P, van Os, J, Myin-Germeys, I. Electronic monitoring of salivary cortisol sampling compliance in daily life. Life Sci 2005; 76: 2431–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
43 Di Forti, M, Morgan, C, Dazzan, P, Pariante, C, Mondelli, V, Marques, TR, et al. High-potency cannabis and the risk of psychosis. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 195: 488–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
44 Morgan, CJA, Curran, HV. Effects of cannabidiol on schizophrenia-like symptoms in people who use cannabis. Br J Psychiatry 2008; 192: 306–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
45 Bhattacharyya, S, Morrison, PD, Fusar-Poli, P, Martin-Santos, R, Borgwardt, S, Winton-Brown, T, et al. Opposite effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol and cannabidiol on human brain function and psychopathology. Neuropsychopharmacology 2010; 35: 764–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
46 D'Souza, DC, Ranganathan, M, Braley, G, Gueorguieva, R, Zimolo, Z, Cooper, T, et al. Blunted psychotomimetic and amnestic effects of delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in frequent users of cannabis. Neuropsychopharmacology 2008; 33: 2505–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
47 Barrowclough, C, Haddock, G, Tarrier, N, Lewis, SW, Moring, J, O'Brien, R, et al. Randomized controlled trial of motivational interviewing, cognitive behavior therapy, and family intervention for patients with comorbid schizophrenia and substance use disorders. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158: 1706–13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Experience sampling analysisA, analysis to investigate self-medication effects with negative affectprevious, positive affectprevious, delusionsprevious and hallucinationsprevious as independent variables and cannabis use as the dependent variable; B, analysis to investigate main effects of cannabis use with cannabis use as the independent variable and negative affect, positive affect, delusions and hallucinations as dependent variables; C, analyses (post-hoc in the patient group exclusively) to differentiate between sub-acute (C) and acute (B) effects of cannabis use on mood and positive symptoms.

Figure 1

Table 1 Sociodemographics of the study sample

Figure 2

Table 2 Clinical variables across the study sample

Figure 3

Table 3 Substance use in the total sample

Figure 4

Table 4 Effects of cannabis use on subsequent symptom levels, patients v. controls

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.