Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rdxmf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T23:24:22.692Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Treatment engagement and violence risk in mental disorders

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Eric B. Elbogen*
Affiliation:
Duke University Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina
Richard A. Van Dorn
Affiliation:
Duke University Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina
Jeffrey W. Swanson
Affiliation:
Duke University Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina
Marvin S. Swartz
Affiliation:
Duke University Medical Center, Durham, North Carolina
John Monahan
Affiliation:
University of Virginia, Charlottesville, Virginia, USA
*
Dr Eric Elbogen, Duke University Medical Center, DUMC 3071, Durham, NC 27710, USA. Tel: +1 919 682 8394; email: eric.elbogen@duke.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Research has uncovered many characteristics related to violence committed by people with mental illness. However, relatively few studies have focused on understanding the connection between violence and dynamic, malleable variables such as a patient's level of treatment engagement.

Aims

To explore the link between community violence and patients' beliefs about psychiatric treatment benefit.

Method

A sample of 1011 adults receiving out-patient treatment for a psychiatric disorder in the public mental health systems of five US states were interviewed.

Results

Bivariate analyses revealed community violence was inversely related to treatment adherence, perceived treatment need and perceived treatment effectiveness. Multivariate analyses showed these three variables were associated with reduced odds of violent and other aggressive acts.

Conclusions

The results suggest clinical consideration of patients' perceptions of treatment benefit can help enhance violence risk assessment in psychiatric practice settings.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2006 

Violence risk assessment has become increasingly important for clinicians treating adults with mental disorders such as schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and depression (Reference Douglas, Cox and WebsterDouglas et al, 1999; Reference Monahan and SteadmanMonahan & Steadman, 1994). To optimise violence prediction, research has uncovered variables empirically related to community violence (Reference Douglas and WebsterDouglas & Webster, 1999; Reference Monahan, Steadman and RobbinsMonahan et al, 2000; Reference Steadman, Silver and MonahanSteadman et al, 2000; Harris et al, Reference Harris, Rice and Cormier2002, Reference Harris, Rice and Camilleri2004; Nichols et al, 2004). Although ‘static’, unchanging characteristics such as gender, history of violence, past child abuse and psychopathy are important to consider when predicting violence risks, these factors are not amenable to change and therefore are seen as less applicable for reducing violence risk (Reference Steadman, Monahan, Robbins and HodginsSteadman et al, 1993; Reference Douglas, Cox and WebsterDouglas et al, 1999; Reference Skeem and MulveySkeem & Mulvey, 2001). Instead, ‘dynamic’ factors could point to methods of preventing community violence, since such variables are malleable (Reference Steadman, Monahan, Robbins and HodginsSteadman et al, 1993; Reference HeilbrunHeilbrun, 1997; Reference Strand, Belfrage and FranssonStrand et al, 1999; Reference Monahan, Steadman and RobbinsMonahan et al, 2000; Reference Douglas and SkeemDouglas & Skeem, 2005). Although static factors are statistically reliable, the dynamic factors are those that are changing and therefore pertinent to violence risk management. One dynamic factor potentially linked to violence in mental disorders is a patient's level of treatment engagement. Research confirms that treatment adherence reduces violence risk (Swartz et al, Reference Swartz, Swanson and Hiday1998a ,Reference Swartz, Swanson and Hiday b ; Swanson et al, Reference Swanson, Swartz and Elbogen2004a ,Reference Swanson, Swartz and Elbogen b ). Another facet of treatment engagement, however, has received less attention: namely, patients’ perceptions of treatment benefit. Because clinicians consider exactly this type of information when assessing violence risk (Elbogen et al, Reference Elbogen, Mercado and Scalora2002, Reference Elbogen, Huss and Tomkins2005), further empirical investigation elucidating the relationship between perceived treatment benefit and violent behaviour would seem especially relevant to psychiatric service providers. The aim of our study therefore was to explore the link between perceived treatment benefit and violent behaviour among patients with mental disorders.

METHOD

The study method is described in detail elsewhere (Reference Monahan, Redlich and SwansonMonahan et al, 2005). In brief, approximately 200 out-patients from publicly funded mental health treatment programmes were sampled from each of five sites; Chicago, Illinois; Durham, North Carolina; San Francisco, California; Tampa, Florida; and Worcester, Massachusetts (total n=1011). Sample inclusion criteria were age 18–65 years, speaker of English or Spanish, had first mental health treatment episode at least 6 months ago, and had at least one out-patient treatment encounter with a publicly supported mental health service provider within the past 6 months. Persons treated only for substance misuse and not for any other psychiatric disorder were excluded. Otherwise, the inclusion criteria did not specify particular mental health diagnoses or level of acuity.

At the Worcester, Tampa and San Francisco sites, potential participants were recruited sequentially in the waiting rooms of out-patient clinics of the community mental health centres. In Durham a list of potentially eligible individuals was created from management information system data, and patients were randomly selected to be approached for participation in the study. The Chicago site used both sampling methods, enrolling about half the sample using the waiting-room approach and the other half using the eligibility-list approach. Participants were enrolled after receiving a complete description of the study and providing written informed consent. All sites received approval from their respective institutional review boards. Refusal rates varied from 2% to 13% across sites. A single structured interview, lasting about 90 min, was administered in person by a trained lay interviewer. Participants were paid US$25 for the interview.

Sample characteristics

Consistent with the core paper from this study (Reference Monahan, Redlich and SwansonMonahan et al, 2005), we report the cross-site range of means and proportions for these characteristics, i.e. the highest and lowest values across the five sites. The mean age of participants ranged from 41.3 to 46.7 years. Between 24.6% and 41.1% of respondents reported having less than a high school education and between 12.5% and 24.5% of respondents were married or cohabiting. The proportion from Black and minority ethnic groups ranged from 28.5% to 64.0%, and the proportion of male participants ranged from 32.4% to 64.5%.

Regarding clinical characteristics, between 41.5% and 49.5% of respondents had a chart diagnosis of schizophrenia or another psychotic disorder, between 14.4% and 17.6% had a diagnosis of bipolar disorder and between 27.5% and 30.7% had major depression. Rates of substance abuse comorbidity ranged from 13.9% to 35.5% between sites, while mean scores on the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale and the Global Assessment of Functioning (see below) ranged from 31 to 33 and 18 to 19 respectively across the sites. Between 30.2% and 38.2% of respondents indicated that they had not adhered to treatment during the past 6 months. Personality disorder diagnoses ranged from 13% to 26% across sites. Between 47.6% and 63.3% of respondents reported four or more lifetime hospitalisations. Finally, between 25.5% and 47.6% of respondents reported recognising the need for mental health treatment, and between 43.4% and 54.4% of respondents reported positive benefits from recent mental health treatment.

Measures

Violence and other aggressive acts

We used the MacArthur Community Violence Interview (Reference Monahan, Steadman and RobbinsMonahan et al, 2000; Reference Steadman, Silver and MonahanSteadman et al, 2000; Reference MonahanMonahan, 2002) to measure violent and aggressive behaviour at three levels of severity:

  1. (a) ‘serious violence’, corresponding to any assault using a lethal weapon or resulting in injury, and threat with a lethal weapon in hand or any sexual assault;

  2. (b) ‘other aggressive acts’, corresponding to simple assault without injury or weapon use;

  3. (c) ‘any physically assaultive behaviour’, denoting a violence composite capturing serious violence and other aggressive acts.

This operationalisation of community violence corresponds to the concept of violence employed in the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study and to other studies of violence among people with mental illness (Reference Swanson, Swartz and Van DornSwanson et al, 2006).

Perceived treatment effectiveness

Commentators have noted two important and distinct dimensions of perceived treatment benefit (Reference PerkinsPerkins, 2002). The first is perceived treatment effectiveness, which was measured using the Consumer Satisfaction Questionnaire (Reference GanjuGanju, 1999) assessed with four items that were summed and dichotomised above the median; those responding in the negative to two or more of these four questions served as the reference group and were coded as 0. Items from this questionnaire included, ‘As a direct result of the services I received, (a) I deal more effectively with daily problems, (b) I am better able to control my life, (c) I am getting along better with my family, and (d) my symptoms are not bothering me as much’ (Reference GanjuGanju, 1999). Teague et al (Reference Teague, Ganju and Hornik1997) describe the reliable use of this scale to measure patients’ views of treatment effectiveness.

Perceived treatment need

A second facet of perceived treatment benefit is a patient's perceptions of treatment need, which in this study was measured using questions from the National Institute of Mental Health Epidemiologic Catchment Area (ECA) study section on perceived barriers to care (Reference Blazer, George and LandermauBlazer et al, 1985). Participants were asked about reasons for not attending mental health treatment care via three items that were summed and dichotomised above the median; those responding in the affirmative to any of these three questions served as the reference group and were coded as 0. The questions were, ‘You think that going for help probably wouldn't do any good’; ‘You think the [mental health] problem might get better by itself’ and ‘You want to solve the [mental health] problem on your own’. Research on violence and arrests in mental disorders confirm good psychometric properties on employing the ECA study section on perceived barriers to care in order to measure patients’ beliefs about the need for psychiatric treatment (Reference Elbogen, Mustillo and Van DornElbogen et al, 2006).

Demographic characteristics

Demographic variables included: age (reference group 44 years or younger), education (reference group high school or beyond), married or cohabiting (reference group single), ethnic status (reference group White) and gender (reference group female).

Clinical factors

Psychiatric diagnosis was based on chart diagnoses at the mental health centres. This analysis compares psychotic disorder with affective disorders as well as the presence or absence of an Axis II personality disorder. The anchored version of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS; Reference Woerner, Mannuzza and KaneWoerner et al, 1988) was used to assess current psychiatric symptoms and the Global Assessment of Functioning scale (GAF; Reference Endicott, Spitzer and FleissEndicott et al, 1976; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) was used to score overall functioning, with low scores indicating more severe functional impairment. Treatment adherence was measured by the question, ‘In the past 6 months, were there times when you thought you should go to a doctor or clinic for mental health or alcohol or drug problems, but did not go?’ (0 nonadherent, 1 adherent). Age at onset of the disorder and the number of lifetime hospitalisations were included in the model as well. All of these factors were dichotomised above the median to capture non-linear associations.

Substance misuse

Substance misuse was assessed with questions adapted from the CAGE questionnaire (Reference Allen, Eckard and WallenAllen et al, 1988). This consists of four questions asking whether people felt they needed to cut down on their drinking, were annoyed by people complaining about their drinking, felt guilty about drinking, and if they need an eye-opener in the morning. These same four questions were also asked in relation to drug use. For these analyses we combined alcohol and drug misuse into a single dichotomous variable, coded 1 for one or more substance misuse symptoms and 0 for no symptoms.

Statistical analysis

We used logistic regression to examine the associations between participants’ demographic and clinical characteristics and the likelihood of engaging in any physically assaultive behaviour, in addition to other aggressive acts and violence in the past 6 months. For the purpose of multivariable modelling, pooling the data across sites offered the advantage of greater statistical power, but also posed two problems that required adjustment in the analyses. First, we had to account for site effects and site-by-covariate interactions associated with violence. To examine and control for these site effects, we used Zelen's test of the homogeneity of odds ratios (Reference ZelenZelen, 1971; StatXact, 2003: pp. 511–517). The Zelen statistic allowed us to test the null hypothesis that the relative risk for the multiple measures of violence did not vary across the five sites, but represented a sampling distribution from a common population. If Zelen's test showed the sites’ odds ratios for a given variable were homogeneous, we then pooled the data for that variable and calculated a common odds ratio across sites. The second problem was that pooling the data could have distorted statistical inferences, insofar as the observations within each site were not independent. Without an adjustment for the clustered nature of the data, the standard errors around the pooled estimates would have been understated, leading to overly liberal tests of statistical significance. Accordingly, we used the same specialised statistical software (StatXact, 2003) to adjust significance tests and confidence intervals around the common (pooled) odds ratios.

For multivariable analysis we used a companion statistical package designed to conduct multivariable logistic regression with stratified data (LogXact, 2002: pp. 83–103). These techniques provided the appropriate correction of variance estimates, taking into account within-site correlation of observations. Specifically, the software uses the Cochran–Armitage method, as adapted by Rao & Scott (Reference Rao and Scott1992), to adjust the ‘effective sample size’ for design effects that occur with a clustered sample (LogXact, 2002: pp. 755, 774).

Finally, we dichotomised independent variables at the median to meet statistical assumptions of normal distribution and to allow a more informative classification of respondents in terms of the presence or absence of relevant characteristics of perceived treatment benefit that might be associated with violence. Along these lines, we appeal to the argument developed by Farrington & Loeber: ‘Dichotomized variables do not contain inherently less information than scales; it all depends on the relative number of variables of each type and on the accuracy of measurement’ (Reference Farrington and LoeberFarrington & Loeber, 2000: p. 107). These authors advocate dichotomising non-linear explanatory variables because this allows a risk- and protective-factor approach to the analysis, interpretation and presentation of data on crime and violence, which is consistent with the goals of our study.

RESULTS

Table 1 displays the prevalence of the various levels of violence by sample characteristics. Across the sites (pooled n=1011) the proportion of respondents engaging in other aggressive acts during the past 6 months ranged from 12.6% to 18.2% with an overall proportion of 14.1%, whereas the proportion of respondents engaging in serious violence ranged from 3.4% to 8.5% with an overall proportion of 5.5%. Finally, a composite of ‘any violence’ (i.e. other aggressive acts or serious violence) ranged from 18.3% to 21.0% with an overall proportion of 19.7%.

Table 1 Prevalence of violent and aggressive behaviour over preceding 6 months by sample characteristics

n Serious violence n (%) Other aggressive acts n (%) Violence composite n (%)
Total 1011 56 (5.54) 143(14.14) 199(19.68)
Demographic characteristics
    Age
        Below median (<44 years) 485 38 (7.84) 82(16.91) 120(24.74)
        Median or above (44 years or older) 525 18 (3.43) 60(11.43) 78(14.86)
    Education
        Less than high school 313 20 (6.39) 41(13.10) 61(19.49)
        High school or beyond 697 36 (5.16) 102(14.63) 138(19.80)
    Marital status
        Single 828 43 (5.19) 107(12.92) 150(18.12)
        Married, cohabiting 182 13 (7.14) 36(19.78) 49(26.92)
    Ethnicity
        Black and minority 446 26 (5.83) 70(15.70) 96(21.52)
        White 565 30 (5.31) 73(12.92) 103(18.23)
    Gender
        Male 509 33 (6.48) 57(11.20) 90(17.68)
        Female 502 23 (4.58) 86(17.13) 109(21.71)
Clinical characteristics
    Treatment adherence
        Yes 655 23 (3.51) 61 (9.31) 84(12.82)
        No 355 33 (9.30) 81(22.82) 114(32.11)
    Diagnosis
        Psychotic 455 16 (3.52) 43 (9.45) 59(12.97)
        Non-psychotic 556 40 (7.19) 100(17.99) 140(25.18)
    Substance use
        Abstinent 797 28 (3.51) 103(12.92) 131(16.44)
        Abuse/dependece 214 28(13.08) 40(18.69) 68(31.78)
    BPRS score
        Below median (<31) 471 17 (3.61) 51(10.83) 68(14.44)
        Median or above (≥31) 539 39 (7.24) 92(17.07) 131(24.30)
    GAF score
        Below median (<48) 486 30 (6.17) 80(16.46) 110(22.63)
        Median or above (≥48) 524 26 (4.96) 63(12.02) 89(16.98)
    Onset of mental illness
        <16 years of age 346 27 (7.80) 61(17.63) 88(25.43)
        ≥16 years of age 664 29 (4.36) 73(11.58) 102(15.36)
    Personality disorder
        No 807 43 (5.32) 99(12.25) 139(17.22)
        Yes 203 13 (6.40) 39(19.21) 51(25.12)
    Number of lifetime hospitalisations
        Below median (<4) 460 20 (4.35) 63(13.70) 83(18.04)
        Median or above (≥4) 545 36 (6.61) 78(14.31) 114(20.92)
Perceived treatment benefit
    Perceived treatment effectiveness
        No (below median) 521 39 (7.49) 92(17.66) 131(25.14)
        Yes (above median) 490 17 (3.47) 51(10.41) 68(13.88)
    Perceived treatment need
        No (below median) 467 38 (8.12) 83(18.59) 121(25.91)
        Yes (above median) 543 18 (3.31) 51 (9.39) 69(12.71)

We first examined bivariate associations between the three levels of violence and a range of salient demographic and clinical variables. Next, multivariable associations were tested using logistic regression procedures. The multivariable models were conducted in three stages: first, the domain of demographic characteristics was assessed; then the clinical characteristics were combined with the demographic characteristics; finally, the factors assessing perceived need for and benefits from recent treatment were added to the model along with the other two domains. All models also controlled for site and the clustering of observations within site.

Table 2 displays the results for the composite measure of any physically assaultive behaviour. In the demographic domain there was a significant and negative bivariate relationship between age and any physically assaultive behaviour (OR=0.52, P<0.001), whereas marital status (OR = 1.69, P<0.01) was positively related to any physically assaultive behaviour. In the clinical domain, significant and negative bivariate associations were present for treatment adherence (OR=0.31, P<0.001), psychotic diagnosis (OR=0.44, P<0.001) and GAF score (OR=0.67, P<0.05), whereas substance misuse (OR = 2.42, P<0.001), personality disorder (OR=1.59, P<0.01) and BPRS score (OR=1.90, P<0.001) were positively associated with any physically assaultive behaviour. Finally, both perceptions of the effectiveness of treatment (OR=0.48, P<0.001) and the need for treatment (OR=0.33, P<0.001) were negatively associated with any physically assaultive behaviour.

Table 2 Cross-site multivariable models for violence composite (any physically assaultive act)

Independent variables Bivariates Stage 12 Stage 23 Stage 34
OR1 (95% CI) OR1 (95% CI) OR1 (95% CI) OR1 (95% CI)
Demographic variables
    Age >44 years 0.52 (0.38-0.73)*** 0.54 (0.39-0.74)*** 0.57 (0.40-0.81)** 0.58 (0.41-0.84)**
    Less than high school education 0.99 (0.69-1.40) NS 0.97 (0.69-1.39) NS 1.00 (0.69-1.46) NS 0.97 (0.66-1.42) NS
    Married 1.69 (1.14-2.50)** 1.56 (1.06-2.31)* 1.29 (0.85-1.97) NS 1.31 (0.86-2.02) NS
    Black and minority ethnic group 1.24 (0.89-1.74) NS 1.26 (0.90-1.76) NS 1.38 (0.96-1.98) NS 1.42 (0.99-2.05) NS
    Male 0.76 (0.54-1.05) NS 0.74 (0.53-1.04) NS 0.70 (0.49-1.02) NS 0.71 (0.49-1.02) NS
Clinical characteristics
    Treatment adherence 0.31 (0.22-0.43)*** 0.43 (0.30-0.62)*** 0.51 (0.35-0.73)***
    Psychotic disorder 0.44 (0.31-0.63)*** 0.47 (0.32-0.70)*** 0.47 (0.32-0.70)***
    Substance misuse 2.42 (1.67-3.48)*** 2.04 (1.37-3.02)*** 1.97 (1.31-2.93)***
    BPRS score ≥31 1.90 (1.36-2.67)*** 1.22 (0.85-1.77) NS 1.09 (0.75-1.60) NS
    GAF score ≥48 0.67 (0.47-0.95)* 0.68 (0.45-1.00) NS 0.71 (0.48-1.07) NS
    Onset of disorder < 16 years 1.85 (0.53-1.04)*** 1.32 (0.92-1.90) NS 1.31 (0.91-1.90) NS
    Personality disorder 1.59 (1.10-2.29)** 1.34 (0.89-2.02) NS 1.25 (0.83-1.91) NS
    Hospitalised ≥4 times 1.20 (0.86-1.67) NS 1.40 (0.97-2.01) NS 1.38 (0.96-1.98) NS
Perceived treatment benefit
    Perceived treatment effectiveness 0.48 (0.34-0.67)*** 0.69 (0.48-1.00)*
    Perceived treatment need 0.33 (0.22-0.50)*** 0.59 (0.41-0.85)**

In the final multivariable model (stage 3), age was negatively associated with any physically assaultive behaviour (OR=0.58, P<0.01). Clinically, treatment adherence (OR=0.51, P<0.01) and having a psychotic diagnosis (OR=0.47, P<0.001) were negatively associated with the outcome, whereas substance misuse (OR = 1.97, P<0.001) was positively associated with the same. With respect to perceived treatment benefit, violence was negatively associated with both perceived treatment effectiveness (OR=0.69, P<0.05) and perceived treatment need (OR=0.59, P<0.01).

To assess the constancy of these findings across different levels of violence severity, we also modelled both serious violence and other aggressive acts. Bivariate associations were virtually identical to those described above for any physically assaultive behaviour. In the multivariable model assessing serious violence, age (OR=0.39, P<0.05), having a psychotic diagnosis (OR=0.44, P<0.05) and perceiving the need for treatment (OR=0.44, P<0.05) were negatively associated with violence. In the multivariable model assessing other aggressive acts, age (OR=0.62, P<0.05), treatment adherence (OR=0.53, P<0.01), having a psychotic diagnosis (OR=0.43, P<0.001) and substance misuse (OR=1.91, P<0.01) were significantly associated. Additionally, there was a significant and negative association between other aggressive acts and perceived treatment need (OR=0.45, P<0.01).

Figure 1 illustrates the odds of any physically assaultive behaviour as a function of level of treatment engagement, as measured by perceived treatment need, perceived treatment benefit and treatment adherence. With respect to violence risk, these findings show that in the absence of these three factors the predicted probability of any physically assaultive behaviour was 0.39. However, the presence or endorsement of these factors was associated with a greatly decreased probability of any physically assaultive behaviour (0.08). It should be noted that probabilities were calculated controlling for all other variables in the model; thus, even individuals in the A+B+C group may in fact possess characteristics increasing odds of violence (e.g. substance misuse or young age).

Fig. 1 Predicted probability of violence composite as a function of level of treatment engagement. A, perceived treatment effectiveness (above median); B, perceived treatment need (above median); C, treatment adherence reported in past 6 months.

Interestingly, most participants’ data were clustered in the ‘none’ and ‘all’ groups, suggesting connection between adherence behaviour and perceived treatment benefit. Correspondingly, there were fewest participants when treatment adherence was present and perceived treatment need or effectiveness was absent (and vice versa). Spearman correlations confirmed treatment adherence was significantly associated with perceived treatment need (r=0.27, P<0.0001) and with perceived treatment effectiveness (r=0.18, P<0.0001).

We additionally examined whether treatment engagement was related to psychiatric diagnosis. We found that people with affective disorders were more likely to report treatment non-adherence than people with psychotic disorders (41% v. 28%; χ2=19.02, d.f.=1, P<0.0001).

However, However, there was no relationship between treatment adherence and personality disorder. Interestingly, although perceived treatment need was not related to either Axis I or Axis II disorders, perceived treatment effectiveness was significantly related to both: specifically, people with a psychotic disorder (52%) were somewhat more likely to perceive their treatment as effective than people with an affective disorder (45%; χ2=4.9, d.f.=1, P=0.02), and people with a personality disorder (39%) were less likely to perceive treatment as effective than people without a personality disorder (51%; χ2=7.46, d.f.=1, P= 0.0063). Thus, although ‘perceived treatment need’ and ‘perceived treatment effectiveness’ are conceptually related, they appear to tap into two distinct facets of treatment engagement.

DISCUSSION

The data revealed significant bivariate associations between different levels of violent behaviour and both the dimensions of perceived treatment benefit measured: perceived treatment effectiveness and perceived treatment need. Other findings were consistent with past research on the relationship between violence and age (Reference Monahan and SteadmanMonahan & Steadman, 1994), cohabitation (Reference Estroff, Swanson and LachicotteEstroff et al, 1998), personality disorder (Reference Moran, Walsh and TyrerMoran et al, 2003; Reference Walsh, Moran and ScottWalsh et al, 2003), substance misuse (Reference Steadman, Mulvey and MonahanSteadman et al, 1998), psychiatric symptoms (Swanson et al, Reference Swanson, Holzer and Ganju1990, Reference Swanson, Swartz and Van Dorn2006) and poor functioning (Reference Swanson, Swartz and EstroffSwanson et al, 1998). Multivariate analyses controlling for these and other covariates demonstrated that perceived treatment need was related to significantly reduced odds of all three levels of violence severity analysed.

Clinical implications

Given the cross-sectional nature of this study, there are several ways to interpret the connection between perceived treatment need and violent behaviour among people with mental disorder. First, one could conjecture that people who do not perceive they need treatment are less likely to attend treatment and take their medications. These individuals may instead ‘self-medicate’ by misusing alcohol or illicit drugs to stave off psychiatric symptoms. Such lack of engagement in services may therefore lead to relapse and increase the chances of violent behaviour. This interpretation is consistent with sociocognitive theories of behaviour change, such as the health belief model (Reference Norman, Abraham and ConnerNorman et al, 2000), self-determination theory (Reference Ryan and DeciRyan & Deci, 2000) and the transtheoretical model (Reference Prochaska and DiClementeProchaska & DiClemente, 1983), which posit that perceptions about treatment benefit predict treatment adherence. To the extent this causal pathway exists, interventions that address perceived treatment need, such as motivation interviewing (e.g. Reference Ruesch and CorriganRuesch & Corrigan, 2002), may be warranted as means of managing – and potentially reducing – violence risk among people with mental disorders.

Another possibility is that violent behaviour might lead to a patient feeling less confident about the benefits of the treatment he or she may have been receiving. Specifically, if a person has been violent and then arrested or involuntarily detained in hospital, the often circuitous process of accessing services (even if the patient tries to do so) after incarceration or hospitalisation may colour people's assessment of the value of these services or their effectiveness, compared with people who have not recently been violent.

This would be accentuated by more difficult access associated with public health insurance-related barriers and low income, certainly characteristic of a sample of patients in the public mental health system in the USA. Thus, violent behaviour may affect a patient's attitudes about the benefits and needs for treatment, rather than the other way around.

However, there is a final interpretation of the data: the statistically significant association between perceived treatment need and violence may indicate that both of the aforementioned causal pathways are present, and are perhaps reinforcing one another. To illustrate, one could imagine a patient with a mental disorder who is violent and arrested and then has difficulty reconnecting to services in the community. Thus, this patient might very well become sceptical about the benefits of treatment, which in turn could lead to poor adherence to prescribed medications, substance misuse to self-medicate and increased psychiatric symptoms – each of which elevate the risks of violence. The findings in this study may thus indicate a cycle in which patients’ perceptions of treatment benefit and violence influence one another reciprocally.

Limitations

Although this study is a first step into exploring the link between perceived treatment benefit and community violence among people with mental disorders, it does have limitations that need to be considered. The overall effect of mental disorder per se cannot be examined using these data, since treatment for mental disorder was a requirement for study participation, and no comparison group without treated mental illness was included. Despite use of sample weighting and robust variance estimation techniques to improve generalisability, it is difficult to define with precision the population with treated major mental disorders to which our results should generalise. In particular, the study surveyed patients connected with mental health services in the USA, who may be different from patients with psychiatric disorders in other countries.

Additionally, it should be noted that the study examines patients’ perceptions of treatment need, as opposed to whether patients’ need for treatment was actually met. If a patient has a need for treatment and the treatment is not provided, or is provided but is inappropriate, then one might anticipate that ‘unmet need’ would be positively associated with violence. Correspondingly, whereas 99% of patients in the sample were actively receiving pharmacological treatment, we did not measure the amount of psychosocial treatment obtained. Thus, future research needs to examine the interconnections between the quality and type of treatment provided, patients’ perceptions of treatment benefit, and violent behaviour.

Finally, our study relied only on self-report to obtain sensitive personal information about committing violent acts. Recent studies using composite indices of violence with multiple informants and record reviews have found higher rates of violence in psychiatric populations than those in our study (Reference Steadman, Mulvey and MonahanSteadman et al, 1998; Reference Swanson, Borum and SwartzSwanson et al, 1999). Further, it is possible that because our sample involved many patients over 40 years old (i.e. past the peak age of violent behaviour), violence rates might have been further influenced. This implies that our findings are probably conservative estimates of the true prevalence of violent behaviour in people with mental disorders.

Future research

The findings provide empirical support for the assertion that perceived treatment need is associated with reduced levels of violence among patients with mental disorders. Future research is needed to replicate findings, using longitudinal data measuring violence from multiple sources. Systematic examination of dynamic, malleable variables such as perceived treatment benefit is needed in scientific literature (as well as in clinical practice) because information on these variables can point to potential risk management strategies. At the very least, the results from this survey of over a thousand patients with mental disorders appear to support the clinical intuition that treatment engagement is important to consider in the context of violence risk assessment. Indeed, the findings also suggest that clinical consideration of patients’ perceived need for treatment can help enhance violence risk assessment in psychiatric practice settings.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the John D. and Catherine T. MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Mandated Community Treatment

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None. Funding detailed in Acknowledgements.

References

Allen, J. P., Eckard, M. J. & Wallen, J. (1988) Screening for alcoholism: techniques and issues. Public Health Report, 103, 586592.Google ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Blazer, D. G., George, L. K. & Landermau, R. (1985) Psychiatric disorders: a rural/urban comparison. Archives of General Psychiatry, 42, 651656.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Douglas, K. S. & Skeem, J. L. (2005) Violence risk assessment: getting specific about being dynamic. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 11, 347383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, K. S. & Webster, C. D. (1999) The HCR-20 violence risk assessment scheme: concurrent validity in a sample of incarcerated offenders. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 26, 319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, K. S., Cox, D. N. & Webster, C. D. (1999) Violence risk assessment: science and practice. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 4, 149184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbogen, E. B., Mercado, C. C., Scalora, M. J., et al (2002) Perceived relevance of factors for violence risk assessment: a survey of clinicians. International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 1, 3747.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbogen, E. B., Huss, M., Tomkins, A. J., et al (2005) Clinical decision-making about psychopathy and violence risk assessment in public sector mental health settings. Psychological Services, 2, 133141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbogen, E. B., Mustillo, S., Van Dorn, R., et al (2006) The impact of perceived need for treatment on risk of arrest and violence in severe mental illness. Criminal Justice and Behavior in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Endicott, J., Spitzer, R., Fleiss, J., et al (1976) The global assessment scale: a procedure for measuring overall severity of psychiatric disturbances. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 766771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Estroff, S. E., Swanson, J. W., Lachicotte, W., et al (1998) Risk reconsidered: targets of violence in the social networks of people with serious psychiatric disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, S95S101.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrington, D. & Loeber, R. (2000) Some benefits of dichotomization in psychiatric and criminological research. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 10, 100122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ganju, V. (1999) The MHSIP Consumer Survey Austin, TX: Texas Department of MHMR.Google Scholar
Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E. & Cormier, C. A. (2002) Prospective replication of the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide in predicting violent recidivism among forensic patients. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 377394.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, G. T., Rice, M. E. & Camilleri, J. A. (2004) Applying a forensic actuarial assessment (the Violence Risk Appraisal Guide) to non forensic patients. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 19, 10631074.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heilbrun, K. (1997) Prediction versus management models relevant to risk assessment: the importance of legal decision-making context. Law and Human Behavior, 21, 347359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LogXact (2002) Software for Exact Logistic Regression. Cambridge, MA: Cytel Software Corporation.Google Scholar
Monahan, J. (2002) The Mac Arthur studies of violence risk. Criminal Behaviour and Mental Health, 12, S67S72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monahan, J. & Steadman, H. J. (eds) (1994) Violence and Mental Disorder: Developments in Risk Assessment: Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Monahan, J., Steadman, H. J., Robbins, P. C., et al (2000) Developing a clinically useful actuarial tool for assessing violence risk. British Journal of Psychiatry, 176, 312319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monahan, J., Redlich, A. D., Swanson, J., et al (2005) Use of lever age to improve adherence to psychiatric treatment in the community. Psychiatric Services, 56, 3744.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moran, P., Walsh, E., Tyrer, P., et al (2003) Impact of comorbid personality disorder on violence in psychosis: report from the UK700 trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 129134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nicholls, T. L., Ogloff, J. R. P. & Douglas, K. S. (2004) Assessing risk for violence among male and female civil psychiatric patients: the HCR–20, PCL: SV, and VSC. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 22, 127158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norman, P., Abraham, C. & Conner, M. (eds) (2000) Understanding and Changing Health Behaviour: From Health Beliefs to Self-regulation. Amsterdam: Harwood.Google Scholar
Perkins, D. O. (2002) Predictors of noncompliance in patients with schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 63, 11211128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prochaska, J. O. & DiClemente, C. C. (1983) Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: toward an integrative model of change. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 51, 390395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rao, J. & Scott, A. (1992) A simple method for the analysis of clustered binary data. Biometrics, 48, 577585.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruesch, N. & Corrigan, P. W. (2002) Motivational interviewing to improve insight and treatment adherence in schizophrenia. Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 26, 2332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, R. M. & Deci, E. L. (2000) Self-determination theory and the facilitation of intrinsic motivation, social development, and well-being. American Psychologist, 55, 6878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skeem, J. L. & Mulvey, E. P. (2001) Psychopathy and community violence among civil psychiatric patients: results from the Mac Arthur Violence Risk Assessment Study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 358374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
StatXact (2003) Statistical Software for Exact Nonparametric Inference. Cambridge, MA: Cytel Software Corporation.Google Scholar
Steadman, H. J., Monahan, J., Robbins, P. C., et al (1993) From dangerousness to risk assessment: implications for appropriate research strategies. In Mental Disorder and Crime (ed. Hodgins, S.), pp 3962. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Steadman, H. J., Mulvey, E. P., Monahan, J., et al (1998) Violence by people discharged from acute psychiatric inpatient facilities and by others in the same neighborhoods. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 393401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steadman, H. J., Silver, E., Monahan, J., et al (2000) A classification tree approach to the development of actuarial violence risk assessment tools. Law and Human Behavior, 24, 83100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strand, S., Belfrage, H., Fransson, G., et al (1999) Clinical and risk management factors in risk prediction of mentally disordered off enders – more important than historical data? A retrospective study of 40 mentally disordered offenders assessed with the HCR–20 violence risk assessment scheme. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 4, 6776.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, J. W., Holzer, C. E., Ganju, V. K., et al (1990) Violence and psychiatric disorder in the community: evidence from the Epidemiological Catchment Area surveys. Hospital and Community Psychiatry, 41, 761770.Google ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., Estroff, S. E., et al (1998) Psychiatric impairment, social contact, and violent behavior: evidence from a study of outpatient-committed persons with severe mental disorder. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, S86S94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. W., Borum, R., Swartz, M. S., et al (1999) Violent behavior preceding hospitalization among persons with severe mental illness. Law and Human Behavior, 23, 185204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S. & Elbogen, E. B. (2004a) Effectiveness of atypical antipsychotic medications in reducing violent behavior among persons with schizophrenia in community-based treatment. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 30, 320.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., Elbogen, E. B., et al (2004b) Reducing violence risk in persons with schizophrenia: olanzapine vs. risperidone. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 65, 16661673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swanson, J. W., Swartz, M. S., Van Dorn, R. A., et al (2006) A national study of violent behavior in persons with schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 490499.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swartz, M. S., Swanson, J. W., Hiday, V. A., et al (1998a) Violence and severe mental illness: the effects of substance abuse and non adherence to medication. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155, 226231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swartz, M. S., Swanson, J. W., Hiday, V. A., et al (1998b) Taking the wrong drugs: the role of substance abuse and medication noncompliance in violence among severely mentally ill individuals. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 33, S75S80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teague, G. B., Ganju, V., Hornik, J. A., et al (1997) The MHSIP Mental Health Report Card: consumer-oriented approach to monitoring the quality of mental health plans. Evaluation Review, 21, 330341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walsh, E., Moran, P., Scott, C., et al (2003) Prevalence of violent victimisation in severe mental illness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 183, 233238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woerner, M. G., Mannuzza, S. & Kane, J. M. (1988) Anchoring the BPRS: an aid to improved reliability. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 24, 112117.Google ScholarPubMed
Zelen, M. (1971) The analysis of several 2×2 contingency tables. Biometrika, 58, 129137.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Prevalence of violent and aggressive behaviour over preceding 6 months by sample characteristics

Figure 1

Table 2 Cross-site multivariable models for violence composite (any physically assaultive act)

Figure 2

Fig. 1 Predicted probability of violence composite as a function of level of treatment engagement. A, perceived treatment effectiveness (above median); B, perceived treatment need (above median); C, treatment adherence reported in past 6 months.

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.