Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-lnqnp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T16:09:48.288Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A Short Scale for Measuring Political Secularism

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 March 2022

Kai Arzheimer*
Affiliation:
JGU University of Mainz
*
Address correspondence and reprint requests to: Kai Arzheimer, Department of Political Science, JGU University of Mainz, Mainz, Germany. E-mail: arzheimer@politik.uni-mainz.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

As religiousness is declining across democracies, scientific interest in secular orientations and their political implications is growing. One specific and particularly important aspect of secular attitudes is political secularism. Political secularism is not merely the absence of religiousness, but rather a world view which holds that religious beliefs should play no role in politics. While there are hundreds of survey instruments that measure the strength and content of religiousness, there is no comparable measure that taps into political secularism. In this research note, I briefly review the concept of political secularism and present a cluster of items which target it. Utilizing data from four large population representative samples taken from the eastern and western states of Germany, I use confirmatory factor analysis to show that these items form a short but internally consistent scale. This scale also displays convergent and discriminant validity. It may be readily used in future surveys.

Type
Note
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Religion and Politics Section of the American Political Science Association

Introduction

Social scientists have long been aware that religion is a powerful political force and that by implication, significant changes to the religious makeup of a society will have far-reaching political repercussions. The growing number of people who identify as Muslims is one such change that has become a hot button issue in Europe. However, Muslims currently make up just 5% of Europe's population, and even under a high immigration/high fertility scenario, their share will rise to less than 15% over the next three decades (Pew Research Center 2017). At least numerically, secularization is a much more important development in Europe.

Secularization is a multi-faceted term which denotes both social and intrapersonal processes (Sommerville Reference Sommerville1998) that diminish the role of religion at the societal, group, and individual levels (Stolz and Tanner Reference Stolz and Tanner2019, 2). At the micro-level, applied research usefully distinguishes between a decline in membership and practice on the one hand and a decline in individual beliefs on the other. The former may outpace the latter, creating “unchurched” segments of the population that still believe but no longer belong (Davie Reference Davie1990). However, in their global survey of religious life, Norris and Inglehart (Reference Norris and Inglehart2011) demonstrate that in some parts of the world and most notably in Western Europe, both membership and belief have been on the retreat for decades. These trends continue to the present day, making Western Europe “one of the world's most secular regions” where “many do not describe themselves as Christian” (Pew Research Center 2018, 6).

For many of these citizens (in Europe and elsewhere), being secular is not just the absence of religion: they embrace secularism as a non-religious world-view. In the broadest possible sense, secularism is an idea that “seeks to restrict religious elements” (Stolz and Tanner Reference Stolz and Tanner2019). While such an outlook has multiple dimensions, one particularly interesting one is political secularism, a preference to limit religion's role in politics (Beard et al. Reference Beard, RB, GS, B and RD2013).

One prominent account (Voas Reference Voas2009) of religious change in Europe that is well supported by empirical data holds that long-running societal secularization brings about individual secularism, via a mechanism of changing socialization patterns and generational replacement. At the same time, individual (political) secularism may lead to demand for policy changes (e.g., full legal recognition of same-sex partnerships) that in turn drive further societal secularization. Somewhat paradoxically, secularization in Europe is also contributing to a re-politicization of religion: it makes societies more culturally diverse and results in polarization between religious organizations and citizens on the one hand and their political secular opponents on the other (Pickel Reference Pickel2017).

To gauge the importance of these issues at the micro-level, a survey measure for political secularism is required. Yet, although there is a plethora of scales for measuring religiousness, secularism as such is rarely studied, and not a single psychometrically evaluated instrument for measuring politically secular attitudes exists.

In this research note, I briefly review the concept of political secularism and present a cluster of items which target it. Using data from four large population representative samples, I show that these items form a short, internally consistent scale, and that this scale also displays convergent and discriminant validity.

What is Political Secularism, and How can it be Measured?

Rates of religious membership and participation have been a concern for social scientists since the inception of sociology in the 19th century. Attempts to complement them by instruments that capture more private religious practice as well as the structure of religious beliefs and attitudes hark back to at least the 1950s. Allport and Ross's distinction between intrinsic and extrinsic orientations (Allport and Ross Reference Allport and Ross1967) is perhaps the most famous example of this early research. Less than two decades later, Gorsuch (Reference Gorsuch1984) reviewed the many instruments already existing at the time and strongly argued against their further proliferation. Yet after another quarter-century, Cutting and Walsh (Reference Cutting and Walsh2008) counted nearly 180 scales for measuring religiousness, and their number is still growing.

Almost all of these scales implicitly follow Sommerville's classic notion that secular attitudes represent a “shift of attention from ultimate (religious) concerns to proximate concerns” (Sommerville Reference Sommerville1998, 250), i.e., a lapse from or a neglect of religion. While such “passive secularism” is an important phenomenon in itself, “active secularism,” i.e., “the affirmation of secular identity and beliefs” (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, GC, JC and NG2018, 553) is arguably a more interesting cluster of attitudes. While passively secular citizens are by definition indifferent toward religion but may still accept its traditional role in society and politics, actively secular citizens are positively opposed to religious arguments and authority, which may spur them into political activity and lead them to adopt more extreme positions (see, e.g., Layman and Weaver Reference Layman and Weaver2016; Brockway Reference Brockway2018; Layman et al. Reference Layman, Campbell, JC and NG2021).

Just like religion, secularism has several “dimensions” (Campbell et al. Reference Campbell, GC, JC and NG2018) or “facets” (Beard et al. Reference Beard, RB, GS, B and RD2013). These include philosophical convictions, social values, secular identities, and negative emotions toward religious authority and doctrine, all of which can have political consequences.

In a narrower sense, political secularism refers to one's attitudes on “the proper role of religious beliefs in political life” (Beard et al. Reference Beard, RB, GS, B and RD2013, 758). More succinctly, political secularism holds that religious beliefs should not be used to justify curbing individual freedoms, and should play little, if any role in political debates.

As a political and legal idea, political secularism is well-researched (Bhargava Reference Bhargava, Dryzek, Honig and Phillips2008; Cliteur and Ellian Reference Cliteur and Ellian2020). However, there is not one validated scale for political secularism at the micro-level. This is problematic, because politically secular views are believed to become more and more widespread in Europe and even in the United States, and because they represent a particularly disruptive type of active secularism. This is because they are conceptually linked to a host of polarizing “morality policies,” which pit religious views and actors against notions of individual freedom (Mourão Permoser Reference Mourão Permoser2019). If one wants to assess the alleged spread of politically secular views as well as their impact on politics, it is therefore desirable to have an instrument that is both reliable and valid and preferably comprises multiple indicators.

However, even single-item questions or short batteries that tap into politically secular views are rare in the extant literature. Beard et al. (Reference Beard, RB, GS, B and RD2013), who prominently make the case for researching political secularism at the micro-level, propose only two items that fit their own definition: “Do you worry the government is getting too involved in the issue of morality?” and “Should churches and other houses of worship keep out of political matters” (Beard et al. Reference Beard, RB, GS, B and RD2013, 760).Footnote 1

Brockway (Reference Brockway2018), in his useful study of “committed seculars,” relies on a single item that refers to one important but very specific aspect of political secularism, namely the separation of church and state. Similarly, Castle (Reference Castle2015) measures opinions on “religious establishment” using an index comprising four items that refer to government institutions and officials engaging in and supporting religious activities.

Finally, Ribberink et al. (Reference Ribberink, Achterberg and Houtman2018) measure “anti-religiosity” by combining four items that were included in the 1998 and 2008 waves of the International Social Survey Programme (ISSP). Two of these items refer to respondents' views of believers and their creeds as intolerant and belligerent. Such opinions may be secular, but they are not politically secular in the aforementioned sense. The two other items tap into attitudes on the role of religious leaders in politics. These items are a better fit for the concept of political secularism but again address a single, rather narrow and specific aspect of it.

Conversely, a valid and reliable scale for measuring political secularism should consist of several items that cover the breadth of the relationship between religion and politics. While developing such a scale from scratch would have its own obvious benefits, re-using existing items from large-scale surveys has at least three important advantages. First, it is already clear that these items “work” in surveys of the general public. Second, replicating single items or group of items opens up the possibility of meaningful comparisons between new and existing surveys. Third, if a whole set of items is replicated that form a scale, its reliability and validity can be assessed across different settings.

The questionnaire of an older survey (“Church and Religion in an enlarged Europe” (CREE)), which ran in 2006 in various European countries and targeted attitudes on “religion, society, and life in general” (Pollack et al. Reference Pollack, Pickel and Müller2007, 20), provides a useful starting point. Its large pool of questions contains five items that closely fit Beard et al.'s (Reference Beard, RB, GS, B and RD2013) definition of political secularism.

One of these items (Symbols, see below) was taken from the earlier “Religious and Moral Pluralism” survey. Another one (Debates) came from the ISSP survey series and is one of the two that were also used by Ribberink et al. (Reference Ribberink, Achterberg and Houtman2018). The remaining three were written by members of the CREE team.Footnote 2

These five (and also some of the other) items were then subsequently included in the 2012 iteration of Germany's biannual General Social Survey (ALLBUS) in somewhat modified form:

  1. (1) “The EU treaties should contain a reference to the Christian god” (God EU) (reversed)

  2. (2) “Religious symbols such as crucifixes should be banned from state schools” (Symbols)

  3. (3) “Education in state schools should be free of religious elements” (Education)

  4. (4) “Scientific research on humans should not be limited by religious norms and values” (Science)

  5. (5) “Religious norms and values have no place in public debates on political issues” (Debates)

When the “Problems of Representation in the Domain of Biopolitics” (PRDB) project set out in 2016 to measure political secularism and its effects at the micro-level, it adopted these five items for its nationally representative survey. For comparability with the ALLBUS and because the rewritten items are even better suited for measuring political secularism, the PRDB project also used the new wording.Footnote 3

The PRDB project chose these items because collectively, they capture different aspects of political secularism as defined by Beard et al. (Reference Beard, RB, GS, B and RD2013). The first question was highly controversial in European Union politics in the 2000s and is still useful as a supranational variation of the classic question about the separation of church and state. The second and third items relate to the role of religion in state schools, which is another contentious issue for secularists in many societies. The fourth item alludes to the role of religious authority in one specific domain of regulatory politics where appeals to religious doctrine are frequent. More generally, it also taps into secular support for science and rationality. Finally, the fifth item spells out the core tenet of political secularism: that religion should play no role in politics.

Data

The analyses presented in the remainder of this research note are based on data from the 2012 ALLBUS round (GESIS—Leibniz-Institut für Sozialwissenschaften 2019) and the 2016 PRDB survey (Arzheimer Reference Arzheimer2016). Both datasets are available in their entirety from Germany's national data archive, where they can be located by following their respective DOIs.

Because of the persistent political, social, and economic east-west differences within Germany, both studies drew separate and disproportionately large samples of respondents from Germany's eastern states. For the present research question, this is particularly apt because West German politics was dominated for decades by a Christian Democratic party, whereas Eastern Germany was a self-avowed irreligious state. As a consequence of this research design, the analyses are effectively based on four large (n = 740–2,538), independent samples.

While the ALLBUS usually uses an odd (five, seven, or eleven) number of categories for measuring agreement, they retained the CREE's original four-point format. Conversely, the PRDB survey employed five categories with a neutral mid-point for almost all items and adjusted the items' response options accordingly. Otherwise, the wording of the items is identical across the samples.

Reliability and Dimensionality

Methods

Any presumptive scale must be internally consistent, i.e., its items should tap into a single common dimension. Internal consistency is one important type of reliability, which in turn is a necessary but insufficient condition for validity. Traditionally, internal consistency is assessed by calculating Cronbach's alpha (Cronbach Reference Cronbach1951), which can be interpreted as the mean of all possible split-half reliabilities across a given set of items.

Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) provides a complementary perspective that is more nuanced and can also help mitigate some of the well-known problemsFootnote 4 linked to the exclusive reliance on alpha (Viladrich et al. Reference Viladrich, Angulo-Brunet and Doval2017). However, the common practice of establishing unidimensionality mechanically by assessing the fit of a single-factor model may also be misleading, because established fit indices as well as newer measures may be too sensitive to trivial violations under some circumstances while failing to identify clearly multidimensional structures in others (Reise et al. Reference Reise2013). Therefore, a detailed inspection of the measurement model that takes into account not just various fit indices and newer variations of alpha but also the factor loadings and possibly alternative specifications of the measurement model is required.

One final complexity arises from the ordinal nature of the survey responses. While items with as few as five categories are sometimes treated as if they were continuous (Viladrich et al. Reference Viladrich, Angulo-Brunet and Doval2017), the WLSMV estimator, which was specifically developed for large samples of ordinal measurements, was employed for both surveys. Therefore, the factor loadings are probit coefficients, and variants of alpha that account for the ordinal nature of the data (Green and Yang Reference Green and Yang2009) were calculated. Threshold parameters are omitted from the main tables but documented in the Appendix.

Findings

Table 1 shows the estimates for a preliminary measurement model for political secularism. All five items display substantial factor loadings in the correct direction. (Ordinal) alpha varies between 0.7 and 0.83, a range that is usually seen as acceptable to good. Because the measurement model assumes that the items are congeneric whereas alpha presupposes tau-equivalent items, (ordinal) omega is a more appropriate measure of internal consistency (Viladrich et al. Reference Viladrich, Angulo-Brunet and Doval2017). The respective row in Table 1 shows that values for omega are indeed slightly higher, but the differences are minuscule.

Table 1. Measurement of political secularism by five items (factor loadings only)

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05. Threshold parameters are omitted from the table for better legibility. Full set of parameters are given in the Appendix.

The evidence from the fit indices, however, is more mixed. The CFI and the SRMR show an acceptable fit in all four samples. Conversely, the TLI and the RMSEA indicate that the model fit is unsatisfactory for all respondents but those interviewed in the east in 2012.

A closer look at the bivariate correlations reveals that there is a particularly close relationship between the two items referring to the role of religion in state schools, which can be explained by their very similar content. This in turn suggests that allowing their error variances to correlate could improve the model fit substantially.

From a substantive point of view, however, it is preferable to remove the Symbols item from the scale. First, having two items that refer to public education gives disproportionate weight to this specific aspect of political secularism. Second, while both items are similar, “religious elements” is more general and already encompasses “religious symbols.” Third, the meaning of the former item is also clearer, because almost all federal states provide some form of religious education in state schools, whereas crucifixes in schools (and other public buildings) are closely linked to the Bavarian Christian Democrats (CSU) and their representation of Bavarian identity. Moreover, because “religious symbols” also include the hijab, they item may also tap into anti-Muslim sentiment. And finally, short(er) scales have clear advantages: they often provide a more realistic assessment of reliability, increase the motivation of and decrease the burden on respondents, and are cheaper to administer (Gogol et al. Reference Gogol, M, T, R, S, U, A and F2014).

Table 2 shows the estimates for the four-item measurement model. The factor loadings remain high, and their direction is still correct. Omega and alpha are somewhat lower, which is expected because they partly depend on the number of items, but still acceptable. Finally, the CFI, TLI, and SRMR indicate near perfect fit in all four samples. The RMSEA indicates a very good fit for all samples but the eastern one in 2016.

Table 2. Measurement of political secularism by four items (factor loadings only)

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05. Threshold parameters are omitted from the table for better legibility. Full set of parameters are given in the Appendix.

Taken together, these findings suggest that the four items form a consistent scale that works reasonably well across slightly different formats (four versus five scale points) and cultural settings (Germany's staunchly secular eastern states versus the more religiously diverse states in the west). The next section will assess the question whether the scale is also valid.

Validity

Many different types of validity have been proposed in the literature. However, they all relate to the same underlying question: to what degree does an instrument tap into what it is supposed to measure? Thus, they should be seen as “tests” that provide (sometimes conflicting) evidence on the question of validity, which “can never be answered with absolute certainty” (Bollen Reference Bollen1989, 185).

This uncertainty is fundamental: in an ideal world, validity could be quantified simply by the correlation of a scale with some universally accepted external criterion, but in reality, there is usually no such gold standard. Like reliability, validity is therefore best investigated making use of CFA, which opens up many sophisticated possibilities (see, e.g., O'Leary-Kelly and Vokurka Reference O'Leary-Kelly and Vokurka1998).

In this section, I follow a comparatively simple approach (see, e.g., Hayes et al. Reference Hayes, Glynn and Shanahan2005, 309): I demonstrate that scores on the secularism scale are highly correlated with measurements that they should correlate with (convergent validity), yet remain distinguishable from them (discriminant validity). This provides a more rounded view of the scale's validity than the relationship with a single criterion and is a first step toward a full nomological validation, which would test a whole network of theoretical propositions and auxiliary assumptions (see, e.g., Hagger et al. Reference Hagger, Gucciardi and Chatzisarantis2017). There are two constructs available across the four samples that can be used for validation, one rather specific, the other more general.

Attitudes on a specific policy, namely legal access to abortion, form the first of these constructs. The regulation of abortion is widely seen as one of the most prominent “morality issues” (Mooney and Schuldt Reference Mooney and Schuldt2008; Studlar et al. Reference Studlar, Cagossi and Duval2013) worldwide. Efforts to limit legal access to abortions are almost universally spearheaded by religious organizations, who explicitly claim that they act on of their beliefs. This suggests that political secularism should be positively associated with more permissive views on abortion. However, because this is a single specific policy, because many other factors may shape one's views on abortion, and because the items used in the surveys make no reference to religious doctrine, this correlation should be far from perfect.

All surveys include four identical or very similar items on the regulation of abortion (see the Appendix for details). A preliminary analysis shows that all items display substantial loadings on the common factor. Ordinal alpha is high and ordinal omega is acceptable across all four samples so that the construct may be used for validation.

The second, more general construct is a composite measure of religiousness that encompasses individual and collective religious practice on the one hand (prayer and attendance) and self-conception as more or less religious on the other. All three items display substantial loadings on the common factor. Alpha is high, and omega is acceptable across all four samples (see the Appendix for details).

Following Beard et al. (Reference Beard, RB, GS, B and RD2013), I have argued above that political secularism should not be understood as the absence of religiousness but rather as an attitude on the proper role of religion in political life. For the validity of the political secularism scale, its correlation with religiousness is therefore crucial. Overall, a strong negative relationship is to be expected: non-religious citizens should be opposed to religious interference in the public sphere, whereas believers should support institutional arrangements that make outcomes which align with their beliefs more likely. Having said that, many religious citizens (particularly in some protestant traditions) may still favor a clear separation of church and state. Conversely, lukewarm believers and even non-believers may see a societal value in religion. Political secularism and religiousness should hence remain distinguishable.

To investigate the validity of the secularism scale, a joint model of secularism and attitudes on abortion was estimated (see the tables in Appendix for the full results). Table 3 shows that the two-factor model, which treat secularism and abortion attitudes as separate constructs, fit the data very well. In line with expectations, the correlation between the factors is clearly positive (more secular respondents do indeed have more permissive views on legal access to abortion) but far from perfect, with values between 0.35 and 0.53.

Table 3. Secularism and abortion attitudes

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

Conversely, assuming that all items load on a single common factor results in a dramatic decline of model fit to values that would be unacceptable by common standards. This provides some initial evidence for the convergent and discriminant validity of the secularism scale.

In the next step, a joint model for secularism and religiousness was estimated (see the tables in the Appendix for the full results). Table 4 shows that a two-factor solution again achieves an acceptable fit across all four samples.Footnote 5 In line with expectations, the estimated correlation between secularism and religiousness is negative and quite substantial, with values ranging from −0.56 to −0.70.

Table 4. Secularism and religiousness

***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

To ascertain that political secularism and religiousness are indeed closely related but not identical, again all items were forced to load on a single common factor. Once more this results in a dramatic increase in χ 2 and a decline in all measures of model fit well below the common cut-off criteria.

Taken together, these findings already provide strong evidence for the convergent and discriminant validity of the political secularism scale. Ideally, they should be complemented by a full nomological validation in future research.

Conclusion

Recent research highlights the importance of political secularism as an individual attitude that affects political behavior. While secularism as a political and philosophical concept has existed for centuries and while there is a plethora of measures for individual religiousness, there is no established instrument for measuring political secularism at the micro-level.

In this research note, I introduce a cluster of items which tap into individual political secularism. Applying CFA, I show that these items form an internally consistent scale. Importantly, good internal consistency can be achieved with just four items. Such short scales are becoming increasingly popular, because they reduce both surveys costs and the burden on respondents.

The secularism scale also displays a moderate to strong positive correlation with permissive views on abortion and a strongly negative correlation with religiousness, yet it remains clearly distinguishable from both. This suggests that the scale is not just a reliable but also a valid instrument for measuring political secularism.

However, there are some limitations. First, the analyses presented in this note rely on surveys that were carried out in Germany only, albeit both in the territory of the former Federal Republic of Germany and the former German Democratic Republic. In future research, they should be complemented by comparative survey data to ascertain that measurements are invariant (Davidov Reference Davidov2009) across cultural, political, and linguistic borders. Ideally, they would be included in large-scale projects such as the European Social Survey.

Second, and relatedly, the item referencing the European Union (EU) treaties should obviously be confined to the EUs 27 member states. In an even broader context, it would have to be adapted, e.g., by instead referring to the national constitution.Footnote 6

Third, while internal consistency is a central aspect of the scale's reliability, it is unrelated to test–retest reliability (McCrae et al. Reference McCrae, JE, S and A2011). Political secularism itself should be a relatively stable attitude. If the scale is reliable, measurements of secularism should be relatively stable, too, at least in the short and medium term. Ideally, future surveys should therefore administer the items repeatedly so that we may learn more about the scale's properties.

But ultimately, the usefulness of the scale can only be gauged in applied research: including the scale in rich and substantively interesting models of political attitudes and behavior will yield the true test of nomological validation.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S1755048322000104.

Data

Replication data and scripts for this article are available from the author's dataverse: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/XEKNYW

Kai Arzheimer is professor of German politics and political sociology at the University of Mainz (Germany). He has published widely in political behavior and electoral politics.

Footnotes

*

I gratefully acknowledge financial support from Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft/German Research Foundation for the collection of the PRDB data (grant number AR 369/5).

1. A third item (“Do religious beliefs most influence your thinking about government and public affairs?”) is more a matter of self-perception than a statement about the desired role of religion in public life.

2. Personal communication with Olaf Müller, one of the former CREE project members.

3. The Debates item originally more specifically referred to “religious leaders” trying to influence public debate. Because the new wording is more general, it better captures political secularism. Other modifications are very minor.

4. Alpha can be misleading if the items are not tau-equivalent or if the structure of the underlying measurement model is not simple. In its original formulation, it is also not adequate if measurements are ordinal. Finally, striving for a particularly high value of alpha during scale development may lead to the inclusion of redundant indicators, because alpha is also a function of the number of items.

5. For the West German sample from 2012, the TLI is below the usual cut-off value of 0.95, but the other indices still suggest a reasonable fit.

6. The CREE project used this variation of the item in Russia.

References

Allport, GW and Ross, JM (1967) “Personal Religious Orientation and Prejudice.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 5, 432443.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arzheimer, K (2016) “Problems of Representation in the Domain of Biopolitics (PRDB)—Einstellungen Zur Präimplantationsdiagnostik.” DOI: https://doi.org/10.7802/2028CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beard, TR, RB, Ekelund, GS, Ford, B, Gaskins and RD, Tollison, (2013) “Secularism, Religion, and Political Choice in the United States.” Politics and Religion 6, 753777.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhargava, R (2008) “Political Secularism, eds. Dryzek, JS, Honig, B and Phillips, A (eds), The Oxford Handbook of Political Theory. Oxford: Oxford University Press, online publication. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199548439.003.0035.Google Scholar
Bollen, KA (1989) “Structural Equations with Latent Variables.” New York: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brockway, M (2018) “Home on Sunday, Home on Tuesday? Secular Political Participation in the United States.” Politics and Religion 11, 334363.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bumpass, LL (1997) “The Measurement of Public Opinion on Abortion: The Effects of Survey Design.” Family Planning Perspectives 29, 177.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, DE, GC, Layman, JC, Green and NG, Sumaktoyo (2018) “Putting Politics First: The Impact of Politics on American Religious and Secular Orientations.” American Journal of Political Science 62, 551565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castle, JJ (2015) “The Electoral Impact of Public Opinion on Religious Establishment.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 54, 814832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cliteur, P and Ellian, A (2020) “The Five Models for State and Religion: Atheism, Theocracy, State Church, Multiculturalism, and Secularism.” ICL Journal 14, 103132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cronbach, LJ (1951) “Coefficient Alpha and the Internal Structure of Tests.” Psychometrika 16, 297334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutting, M and Walsh, M (2008) “Religiosity Scales: What Are We Measuring in Whom?” Archive for the Psychology of Religion/Archiv Für Religionpsychologie 30. Review of scales, 137154.Google Scholar
Davidov, E (2009) “Measurement Equivalence of Nationalism and Constructive Patriotism in the ISSP: 34 Countries in a Comparative Perspective.” Political Analysis 17, 6482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davie, G (1990) “Believing without Belonging: Is this the Future of Religion in Britain?” Social Compass 37, 455469.Google Scholar
GESIS—Leibniz-Institut für Sozialwissenschaften (2019). “German General Social Survey—ALLBUS 2012/German General Social Survey—ALLBUS 2012.” DOI: https://doi.org/10.4232/1.13418CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gogol, K, M, Brunner, T, Goetz, R, Martin, S, Ugen, U, Keller, A, Fischbach and F, Preckel (2014) “‘My Questionnaire Is Too Long!’ The Assessments of Motivational-Affective Constructs with Three-Item and Single-Item Measures.” Contemporary Educational Psychology 39, 188205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorsuch, RL (1984) “Measurement: The Boon and Bane of Investigating Religion.” American Psychologist 39, 228236.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, SB and Yang, Y (2009) “Reliability of Summed Item Scores Using Structural Equation Modeling: An Alternative to Coefficient Alpha.” Psychometrika 74, 155167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagger, MS, Gucciardi, DF and Chatzisarantis, NLD (2017) “On Nomological Validity and Auxiliary Assumptions: The Importance of Simultaneously Testing Effects in Social Cognitive Theories Applied to Health Behavior and Some Guidelines.” Frontiers in Psychology 8, 1933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, AF, Glynn, CJ and Shanahan, J (2005) “Willingness to Self-Censor: A Construct and Measurement Tool for Public Opinion Research.” International Journal of Public Opinion Research 17, 298323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layman, GC, Campbell, DE, JC, Green and NG, Sumaktoyo (2021) “Secularism and American Political Behavior.” Public Opinion Quarterly 85, 79100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Layman, GC and Weaver, CL (2016) “Religion and Secularism among American Party Activists.” Politics and Religion 9, 271295.Google Scholar
McCrae, RR, JE, Kurtz, S, Yamagata and A, Terracciano (2011) “Internal Consistency, Retest Reliability, and Their Implications for Personality Scale Validity.” Personality and Social Psychology Review 15, 2850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mooney, CZ and Schuldt, RG (2008) “Does Morality Policy Exist? Testing a Basic Assumption.” Policy Studies Journal 36, 199218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mourão Permoser, J (2019) “What are Morality Policies? The Politics of Values in a Post-Secular World.” Political Studies Review 17, 310325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norris, P and Inglehart, R (2011) Sacred and Secular. Religion and Politics Worldwide, 2nd Edn. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Leary-Kelly, SW and Vokurka, RJ (1998) The Empirical Assessment of Construct Validity. Journal of Operations Management 16, 387405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew Research Center (2017) “Europe's Growing Muslim Population.” Pew Research Center. Available at https://www.pewforum.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/7/2017/11/FULL-REPORT-FOR-WEB-POSTING.pdf.Google Scholar
Pew Research Center (2018) “Being Christian in Western Europe.” Pew Research Center. Available at https://www.pewforum.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/7/2018/05/Being-Christian-in-Western-Europe-FOR-WEB1.pdf.Google Scholar
Pickel, G (2017) “Secularization—An Empirically Consolidated Narrative in the Face of an Increasing Influence of Religion on Politics.” Política & Sociedade 16, 259294.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pollack, D, Pickel, G and Müller, O (2007). Church and Religion in an Enlarged Europe (C&R 2006). Cumulated Codebook. Frankfurt/Oder: European University Viadrina/Chair for Comparative Sociology of Culture. Available at https://www.uni-muenster.de/imperia/md/content/soziologie/personen/pollack/church_religion_codebuch.pdf.Google Scholar
Reise, SP, et al. (2013) “Multidimensionality and Structural Coefficient Bias in Structural Equation Modeling.” Educational and Psychological Measurement 73, 526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ribberink, E, Achterberg, P and Houtman, D (2018) “Religious Polarization: Contesting Religion in Secularized Western European Countries.” Journal of Contemporary Religion 33, 209227.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommerville, CJ (1998) “Secular Society/Religious Population: Our Tacit Rules for Using the Term ‘Secularization’.” Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 37, 249253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stolz, J and Tanner, P (2019) “Secularization, Secularity, and Secularism in the New Millennium: Macro-Theories and Research.” In: Oxford Research Encyclopedia of Politics. Oxford University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780190228637.013.792CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studlar, DT, Cagossi, A and Duval, RD (2013) “Is Morality Policy Different? Institutional Explanations for Post-War Western Europe.” Journal of European Public Policy 20, 353371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Viladrich, C, Angulo-Brunet, A and Doval, E (2017) “A Journey Around Alpha and Omega to Estimate Internal Consistency Reliability.” Anales de Psicologïa 33, 755782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voas, D (2009) “The Rise and Fall of Fuzzy Fidelity in Europe.” European Sociological Review 25, 155168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Measurement of political secularism by five items (factor loadings only)

Figure 1

Table 2. Measurement of political secularism by four items (factor loadings only)

Figure 2

Table 3. Secularism and abortion attitudes

Figure 3

Table 4. Secularism and religiousness

Supplementary material: Link

Arzheimer Dataset

Link
Supplementary material: File

Arzheimer supplementary material

Arzheimer supplementary material 1
Download Arzheimer supplementary material(File)
File 46.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Arzheimer supplementary material

Arzheimer supplementary material 2
Download Arzheimer supplementary material(File)
File 46.1 KB