Racial animus is the primary focus of research on white racial attitudes in the United States. While expressions of negative affect towards racial minorities have changed over time, understandings of race have likewise evolved to recognize what it means to be white. So even as survey measures of racial animosity remain a powerful tool for capturing white racial attitudes, scholarly (and increasingly public) understandings of race have evolved, to some extent, to recognize that whiteness confers privilege (McIntosh Reference McIntosh1988). These advantages, which are systemic, include greater social status, material welfare, and freedom from ill-treatment (DuBois Reference Du Bois1935; Pager and Shepard Reference Pager and Shepherd2008; Rothstein Reference Rothstein2017; Trounstine Reference Trounstine2016).
Yet these advantages are hidden or not easily observed by whites because they constitute everyday “normal” life. White privilege denial is a subtle and frequently overlooked type of racial thinking, one that quietly, but powerfully, rejects the idea that there are unearned advantages of being white. White privilege is a distinctly intergroup concept, capturing the positive self-regard whites derive from their high status in the racial hierarchy. Given the centrality of race in American politics (Hutchings and Valentino Reference Hutchings and Valentino2004), we introduce a measure of white privilege denial, explore its relationship to other prominent racial constructs, and examine whether, and how, it is pertinent to whites’ thinking about the political world.
Using the Cooperative Congressional Election Study (CCES), we examine the role of white privilege denial during the 2016 presidential election and two years later, during the 2018 midterm election, replicate and extend the analyses. Although related to other racial constructs, we found white privilege to be a distinct attitude. Nearly half of whites exhibit at least some denial of white privilege and nearly a third deny all the items in the index. In exploring its political manifestations, we found white privilege denial to be associated with evaluations of the 2016 presidential candidates and presidential approval (Obama). In the 2018 midterm election, we revisited presidential approval (Trump) and extended the analyses to a wide-ranging examination of racial(ized) policy attitudes, further demonstrating the political relevance of white privilege denial to politics.
We conclude by suggesting that white privilege denial become a central attitude of inquiry in the study of racial attitudes and politics. Although racial animus is a powerful predictor of political attitudes, especially now that racially explicit appeals have become more acceptable (Valentino, Neuner, and Vandenbroek Reference Valentino, Neuner and Vandenbroek2018), it is limited in capturing how whites think about race. White privilege denial, a largely hidden but potent form of bias, focuses on how whites, as a privileged group, think about their advantaged position in the social order. Beyond measures of racial animus and white identity, white privilege denial provides new understandings of the powerful role that race plays in American politics.
Racial Attitudes, Old and New
White racial attitudes in the United States have changed dramatically over the last hundred years. The rationale for chattel slavery and the subsequent oppression of African Americans that followed emancipation drew on the doctrine of biological racism, the idea that Blacks are genetically inferior to whites. Many whites believed Blacks to be of lower intelligence and character and, consequently, deserving of subordinate status as an inherently “lesser” race. The decline of biological racism is widely attributed to a shift in elite discourse about race, a turn that repudiated and diminished (but did not extinguish) biological explanations (Banks Reference Banks2014; Hutchings and Valentino Reference Hutchings and Valentino2004; Jardina and Piston Reference Jardina and Piston2019; Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996).
After the decline of biological racism, white attitudes about race changed, taking on an egalitarian character with large majorities of whites endorsing the principle of equal treatment and integration of the races (Schuman et al. Reference Schuman, Steeh, Bobo and Krysan1997). In other words, white support for legal discrimination plummeted. Although support for the principle of equal treatment was (and is) widely embraced, white respondents consistently exhibited less enthusiasm for public policy solutions addressing racial inequalities (Schuman et al. Reference Schuman, Steeh, Bobo and Krysan1997). Much of the reason for this gap is that racial animosity did not end with the demise of biological racism but instead was manifested in a newer, “subtler” form of racism.
In place of biological racism emerged understandings of racial inequality rooted in societal and individual causes (Hutchings and Valentino Reference Hutchings and Valentino2004; Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Kinder and Sears Reference Kinder and Sears1981; Myrdal Reference Myrdal1944). Inequalities between Blacks and whites came to be understood either as a matter of white discrimination against Blacks (the racially progressive argument) or a lack of motivation shown by Blacks (the racially conservative argument). Most whites subscribed to the latter view (Schuman et al. Reference Schuman, Steeh, Bobo and Krysan1997), at least initially. The perception that Blacks are not willing to apply themselves, coupled with the idea that they take what they have not earned, are the twin pillars of symbolic racism (Sears and Henry Reference Sears and Henry2003) or racial resentment (Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Kinder and Sears Reference Kinder and Sears1981). Much like biological racism, these new understandings of race stem from antipathy toward Blacks. Yet rather than proclaiming the biological inferiority of Blacks, racial resentment is presumably more subtle or implicit because it calls out Blacks for violating traditional values such as hard work and not taking more than one deserves. As made clear by Kinder and Sanders (Reference Kinder and Sanders1996), the racial resentment scale avoids referencing inborn abilities and instead fixates on questions of character, or the lack thereof.
Since racial resentment focuses on Black disadvantage it is inherently limited in what it can say about how whites think about race. From the perspective of whites, Black disadvantage minimizes (or erases) white advantage, marking racial inequality as a “them” problem. In addition, as research on intergroup relations has aptly shown, ingroup bias and outgroup prejudice are distinct concepts and there are generally weak and inconsistent relationships between them (Brewer Reference Brewer1979, Reference Brewer1999). Rather than fueling animosity towards outgroups, positive ingroup sentiment is often accompanied by indifference towards outgroups. In short, outgroup hate is not simply the reciprocal of ingroup love. Jardina (Reference Jardina2019) advances these insights in her study of white identity (and group consciousness) and its political manifestations, showing that white identity (and group consciousness) is relevant to understanding support for politicians and public policy (also see Chudy, Piston, and Shipper Reference Chudy, Piston and Shipper2019; Jefferson and Takahashi Reference Jefferson and Takahshi2021; Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2019), especially when they are perceived to benefit the group interests of whites.
Although white identity turns the theoretical lens inward revealing the depth and boundaries of ingroup assimilation and group solidarity, it does not emphasize group differentiation, a key aspect of intergroup relations. Since a strong motivation for identifying with a group is to provide a positive sense of self, individuals seek to not only differentiate their group from others but to do so in a way that reflects positively on the ingroup. According to Tajfel and Turner, “the aim of differentiation is to maintain or achieve superiority over an out-group on some dimensions” (Reference Tajfel, Turner, Austin and Worchel1979, 41). Members of the high-status group seek to maintain the legitimacy of the social hierarchy by maintaining positive social comparisons. The positive distinctiveness of higher status groups, however, can be made insecure from “a conflict of values, i.e., it is conceived by some as based on unfair advantages, various other forms of injustice, exploitation, illegitimate use of force, etc.” (Tajfel Reference Tajfel1974, 79). Members of high-status groups therefore reject challenges to positive distinctiveness, especially challenges to the legitimacy of their higher status. In three words: white privilege denial.
As a challenge to the legitimacy of the racial status hierarchy, a primary source of positive distinctiveness, we look at the concept of white privilege, the largely invisible, unearned advantages associated with being white (McIntosh Reference McIntosh1988). Although he did not coin the term, the concept of white privilege borrows from DuBois’ (Reference Du Bois1935) insight about “the wage of whiteness,” the psychological and social benefits the white working class received from being aligned with white elites rather than the Black working class. More tangibly, DuBois discussed how poor whites benefited from belonging to white society by having access to better schools, higher paying jobs, and extra leniency in the criminal justice system. Access to these and other resources and benefits were, and continue to be, codified through public policy creating structural inequalities (e.g., Pager and Shepard Reference Pager and Shepherd2008; Rothstein Reference Rothstein2017; Trounstine Reference Trounstine2016).
Racial privilege is nearly invisible to white people (Golash-Boza Reference Golash-Boza2017; McIntosh Reference McIntosh1988; Phillips and Lowery Reference Phillips and Lowery2018). According to Golash-Boza (Reference Golash-Boza2017, 187), “If you are White, it can be difficult to notice that you are not being followed around the store; that people are smiling at you on the street instead of clutching their purses; that no one asks you if you speak English; that you are not asked for identification when paying with a credit card.” In other words, it is “normal,” everyday life for most white people. While many whites acknowledge racial inequality and the disadvantages of being a person of color (Branscombe, Schmitt, and Schiffhauer Reference Branscombe, Schmitt and Schiffhauer2007), some do not consider themselves to be part of a racial social order, much less residing at the top of it (Hartmann, Gerteis, and Croll Reference Hartmann, Gerteis and Croll2009).
The desire to maintain a positive self-concept cloaks privilege and in the aggregate, practiced by many whites, creates a “herd invisibility” that protects the position of whites in the racial hierarchy (Phillips and Lowery Reference Phillips and Lowery2018). Whites may be reluctant to acknowledge the advantages associated with whiteness (Conway et al. Reference Conway, Lipsey, Pogge and Ratliff2017), and when confronted with the idea that they have benefited from their race, deny the existence of white privilege (Knowles et al. Reference Knowles, Lowery, Chow and Unzueta2014) or that it applies to them (Phillips and Lowery Reference Phillips and Lowery2015; De Sante and Smith Reference DeSante and Smith2020a). In sum, white privilege denial is a subtle, but powerful, form of racial bias that maintains advantaged position of whites in the racial hierarchy and “promote[s] insensitivity and inaction with respect to racial inequality” (Knowles et al. Reference Knowles, Lowery, Chow and Unzueta2014). Whereas the dominant approach to studying white racial attitudes, racial resentment, is centered on the notion that Blacks have created their own disadvantages, white privilege denial is about dismissing the advantages of whiteness to maintain positive comparisons to other racial groups.
Not only have scholars uncovered how whites understand their position in the racial hierarchy through white privilege and related concepts such as color-blindness (Bonilla-Silva Reference Bonilla-Silva2006) but such understandings have intensified public discussions in recent years, especially in the aftermath of protests against racial injustice and police brutality arising from George Floyd’s death. With the advent of social media, phrases such as “check your privilege” have drawn attention to the advantages associated with being white and have come to animate public discussions of racism and racial inequality. Regardless of whether a white person has been exposed to the concept of white privilege, we are interested in whether, and how, denying its existence manifests itself in the political world.
We proceed in two steps. In the first, we introduce our measure of white privilege denial and compare it to other prominent measures of racial attitudes including racial resentment, white identity, and white group consciousness. In the second, we examine the political relevance of white privilege denial by looking at various political manifestations including evaluations of political figures and policy attitudes.
Measuring White Privilege Denial
Our data come from the 2016 and 2018 Cooperative Congressional Election Study (CCES), a nationally descriptive internet survey of the U.S. population (see Ansolabehere and Rivers Reference Ansolabehere and Rivers2013). The analyses are limited to white respondents, producing 770 cases in 2016 and 762 cases in 2018.Footnote 1 For our measure of white privilege denial, we borrow three items from Swim and Miller’s (Reference Swim and Miller1999) white privilege scale to capture the extent to which whites, at the group level, acknowledge systemic racism and the unearned advantages of Whiteness (refer to online appendix A).Footnote 2 The white privilege denial scale (WPD) is, to our knowledge, novel in the study of white racial attitudes and politics (but see Chudy, Piston, and Shipper Reference Chudy, Piston and Shipper2019 and DeSante and Smith Reference DeSante and Smith2020b for measures of white racial attitudes that include items on white privilege).
We also examine how WPD is related to other prominent measures of racial attitudes and, of course, include them as controls when we examine the relationship between WPD and political outcomes. For racial resentment (RR), we use Kinder and Sanders’ (Reference Kinder and Sanders1996) four-item canonical measure frequently used in studies of white racial attitudes. Respondents rated their level of agreement on a 5-point scale ranging from “Strongly disagree” to “Strongly agree” on the items for each scale, depicted in table 1, and are coded such that higher values represent greater denial of white privilege or racial resentment. Following Jardina (Reference Jardina2019), we also examine white identity and white consciousness, both of which are coded such that higher values represent stronger identity or higher group consciousness.
Figure 1 depicts how many white privilege items respondents denied in both years of our inquiry. In 2016, a slim majority of whites agreed with all the white privilege items (53%), although nearly half are in denial of one or more items. Among those who acknowledged any denial, 16% are a little in denial (one item was endorsed), 16% are mostly in denial (two items were endorsed), and a little over two-thirds (68%) exhibit strong denial (all three items were endorsed). Results are similar in 2018, in which 58% of whites deny zero items, 8% of whites deny one item, 6% of whites deny two items, and 28% of whites deny all three items. Of the deniers, 19% were a little in denial, 14% were mostly in denial, and 67% expressed the highest denial possible on the index.
Next, we examined the relationship between WPD and other racial attitudes. As mentioned, there are generally weak and inconsistent relationships between ingroup love and outgroup hate, so WPD may only be weakly related to RR. Yet WPD is a measure of social comparison, not a measure of identity or group consciousness, so for those whites desiring to reject challenges to their group’s high status we might expect a strong relationship between WPD and RR. Figure 2 compares the distribution of each racial attitude for both years of our analyses, where each of the respective scales are rescaled to range between 0 and 1 (higher values reflect greater denial or resentment). In 2016, WPD resembles a bimodal distribution with responses clustered at the extremes (M = .498, sd = .0125) whereas RR approximates a normal distribution (M = .544, sd = .0116) further suggesting they are distinct attitudes. In 2018, the bimodal distribution of WPD and the unimodal distribution of RR come into sharper focus. Nevertheless, the mean values for WPD and RR in 2018 are very similar to those in 2016. In 2018, WPD has a mean of .464 (sd = .014) and RR a mean of .525 (sd = .01). As is the case with RR, the distribution of WPD varies by ideology suggesting that both WPD and RR have joined the ideological or partisan divide (refer to online appendix B).
To directly address the question of whether WPD and RR are distinct, we conducted an exploratory factor analysis to examine whether each set of items is represented by a single factor or whether they load onto separate factors. Since WPD and RR are related constructs, we use promax rotation which assumes the measures are related. Thus, even though the constructs are corelated, we can determine whether WPD and RR can be meaningfully distinguished from one another (as well as from other related racial constructs).
The left side of table 2 displays the results of the factor analysis of the racial attitude items in 2016. WPD appears to be a meaningfully distinct construct from RR. The WPD items load strongly onto a single factor, especially the first two items that each load above .8. The third WPD item appears to be better represented on the first factor (.52) as well since it loads weakly onto the second factor (.29). Importantly, none of the RR items appear to load onto the same factor as WPD suggesting that racial resentment is distinct from white privilege denial. Although distinct, the two factors correlate at .7, suggesting they are solidly related. As a scale, the RR items also exhibit less coherence. The first and fourth RR items load weakly onto two separate factors below the conventional threshold of .4. However, the second and third RR items load strongly onto factor 2, slightly above .8.
Since white identity politics represent a meaningful shift in how some whites view themselves and the political world (see Jardina Reference Jardina2019), we also examined the relationship between WPD and measures of white identity (WI) and white consciousness (WC) (refer to online appendix C for full question wording). According to Jardina (Reference Jardina2019), whites who strongly identify with their race and have a strong sense of racial group solidarity are more likely to “embrace their privileged status” in ways that maintain racial inequality, and likewise harbor exclusionary attitudes. Thus, WPD should be related to measures of WI and WC but, as Jardina (Reference Jardina2019) found, they should be distinct. Although these constructs belong to the same family, WPD (like racial resentment) is an attitude, not an identity or form of group consciousness. In our initial analysis, we find that WPD and WI are weakly correlated (r = -.0053) but that WPD and WC are more strongly correlated (r = .5198). To be sure that WPD is distinct from WI and WC, we nevertheless included them in the factor analysis.
The right side of table 2 investigates whether WI and WC are distinct from WPD and RR (only available in the 2018 data). If white identity and consciousness are indistinguishable from racial attitudes, either WPD or RR, we would expect them to load onto the same factors. The results suggest this is not the case. Neither WI nor WC load onto the same factor as either WPD or RR at the conventional .4 standard, indicating they are distinct scales. For interested readers, we also considered social dominance orientation (SDO), a predisposition towards group-based hierarchy (Pratto et al. Reference Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth and Malle1994), as well as hostile sexism (HS), a belief that women are trying to control men (Cassese and Holman Reference Cassese and Holman2019; Schaffner Reference Schaffner2021), as both are related to racial attitudes and are increasingly important in understanding political preferences. As shown in online appendix C, the SDO and HS items do not load onto the same factor as WPD, and from the results in online appendix D, the effects for WPD do not substantively change when controlling for SDO or HS.
Given the modest number of cases in our analyses, we also replicated some of our results (when items were available) using a question similar to the first item in the WPD scale from the 2018 CCES and 2020 CES (formerly the CCES) common content. In online appendix E, we report the results from these analyses featuring approximately 35,000 whites to rule out that our results are due to a relatively small number of cases. In each of the analyses, we replicated the findings for WPD and RR. In sum, by using a single item similar to an item in the WPD scale, our basic findings regarding the importance of WPD are corroborated in a large-N analysis.
Having established that white privilege denial is distinct from other prominent measures and correlates of racial attitudes and identity, in the following sections we examine whether, and how, it informs a variety of important political attitudes including evaluations of presidential candidates, presidents, and public policy. In the following models, we include these other measures of racial attitudes, identity, and consciousness to ensure that the reported effects of WPD matter beyond the usual suspects.
The Manifestation of White Privilege Denial in American Politics
Race is a defining feature of the American party system (Bowler and Segura Reference Bowler and Segura2012; Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1989; Hutchings and Valentino Reference Hutchings and Valentino2004; Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Mendelberg Reference Mendelberg2001; Parker and Barreto Reference Parker and Barreto2013; Philpot Reference Philpot2007). The differences in the racial groups that make up party coalitions and the positions taken by parties on racial issues have forged party images or reputations reflective of these differences (Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1989; Heit and Nicholson Reference Heit and Nicholson2016; Philpot Reference Philpot2007) and these reputations shape evaluations of parties and politicians (Carmines and Stimson Reference Carmines and Stimson1989; Hutchings and Valentino Reference Hutchings and Valentino2004; Jardina Reference Jardina2019; Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Mendelberg Reference Mendelberg2001; Nicholson Reference Nicholson2005; Reny, Collingwood, and Valenzuela Reference Reny, Collingwood and Valenzuela2019; Sheagley, Chen, and Farhart Reference Sheagley, Chen and Farhart2017; Tesler Reference Tesler2016; Tesler and Sears Reference Tesler and Sears2010; Valentino, Hutchings, and White Reference Valentino, Hutchings and White2002; Valentino, Neuner, and Vandenbroek Reference Valentino, Neuner and Vandenbroek2018; Sides, Tesler and Vavreck Reference Sides, Tesler and Vavreck2019; Segura and Valenzuela Reference Segura and Valenzuela2010). Similarly, racial attitudes are a primary ingredient in explaining public support for a variety of policy proposals (Brader, Valentino, and Suhay Reference Brader, Valentino and Suhay2008; Gilens Reference Gilens1999; Jardina Reference Jardina2019; Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Pérez Reference Pérez2010; Reference Pérez2016; Tesler Reference Tesler2012).
White privilege denial enables whites to dismiss how they benefit from racism, maintain the legitimacy of the racial system, and ultimately eschew responsibility for addressing racial inequalities. As a way of maintaining positive racial intergroup comparisons (positive distinctiveness), those in denial of white privilege are likely to align themselves with political actors and embrace public policies that will maintain the racial status quo. Given the clear party differences on the question of race, white privilege denial is likely to manifest itself in support for (opposition towards) Republican (Democratic) politicians. In online appendix F, we report findings showing that white privilege denial has a positive and significant association with affective polarization towards Republicans at the detriment of Democrats.
Since racial attitudes also shape public opinion on a variety of policy issues (Gilens Reference Gilens1999; Jardina Reference Jardina2019; Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Tesler Reference Tesler2016), we advance this research by looking at a host of public policies. Although some of the issues we examine are explicitly racial by design (e.g., affirmative action), others have become racialized, the process whereby a race-neutral policy such as health care becomes tied to racial group images through the media and elite discourse (Gilens Reference Gilens1999; Tesler Reference Tesler2015). By looking at white privilege denial, we can examine whether notions of unearned white advantage, a threat to the positive distinctiveness of whites, is associated with racialized policy attitudes. For racial and racialized policies, we expect white privilege denial to be associated with greater support for racially conservative positions that promote, or at least do not undermine, the higher status of whites. For instance, we expect those higher in denial to be more likely to oppose affirmative action, immigration, and oppose NFL player protests against racial injustice.
In all our analyses, we control for a variety of alternative explanations. Foremost among our controls is racial resentment, the most widely used measure of white racial attitudes in political science. In addition to racial resentment, we also control for white identity and white consciousness (2018 only) since Jardina (Reference Jardina2019) found they were relevant to explaining policy attitudes that affect whites as a group. By including a thorough set of controls, we can be confident that the results we obtain for white privilege denial are not simply standing in for omitted variables. In other words, we can examine the effects of white privilege denial beyond existing explanations. Nevertheless, given the observational nature of our data we are limited to establishing associations between racial attitudes. Future research is necessary to examine causal relationships.
White Privilege Denial and Candidate Evaluation
Race has played an important role in American elections, and it was especially salient in the 2016 presidential race (Abramowitz and McCoy Reference Abramowitz and McCoy2019; Reny, Collingwood, and Valenzuela Reference Reny, Collingwood and Valenzuela2019; Sides, Tesler, and Vavreck Reference Sides, Tesler and Vavreck2019). Although racial animus is central to understanding vote choice in the 2016 election, our inquiry advances understanding of it (and perhaps candidate evaluations more generally) by examining the role of white privilege denial. Since the denial of white privilege permits whites to retain a positive self-concept by maintaining positive distinctiveness, a favorable comparison between their racial group and people of color, we expect whites in greater denial to rate Trump more favorably in relation to Clinton.
To capture candidate evaluations, we rely on feeling thermometers, measures of how warmly a person feels toward a candidate that ranges on a scale from zero to one hundred. We created the dependent variable by subtracting thermometer ratings for Clinton from Trump so that the scores range from -100 to 100 in which higher values reflect greater relative affect for Trump. In online appendix G, we present an alternative version of this analysis where the feeling thermometers for both candidates are each measured separately. In keeping with Trump’s racial campaign rhetoric and the long-standing racial differences between the parties, we expect higher levels of WPD to produce warmer feelings toward Trump relative to Clinton. Put differently, we expect that whites who acknowledge White privilege to have more positive feelings towards Clinton relative to Trump. Although we also expect RR to similarly shape candidate evaluation in this manner, it of course captures the effects of outgroup animus whereas WPD captures the desire for whites to maintain positive distinctiveness (and feel good about the ingroup). We also include controls for party identification, ideology, and demographic characteristics. All variables have been rescaled from zero to one to aid in the comparison of coefficients.
Table 3 displays the results of an OLS regression analysis with Huber-White standard errors looking at the effects of WPD and RR on relative evaluations of the 2016 presidential candidates. We find that WPD is a strong, significant predictor of favoring Trump over Clinton, controlling for RR. The effect of RR, however, appears to be somewhat larger than WPD so we looked at the thermometer ratings for each candidate separately (refer to online appendix G). The models in online appendix G suggest that WPD appears to be more strongly related to negative affect towards Clinton whereas RR appears to be more strongly associated with positive affect towards Trump. These results make sense considering what we know about the candidates’ campaigns. Whereas Trump emphasized racial animus, Clinton’s campaign stressed themes of racial inequality with references to “systemic racism” and racial bias in policing (see Sides, Tesler, and Vavreck Reference Sides, Tesler and Vavreck2019). In online appendix H, we also examine whether the effects of WPD and RR on candidate evaluations (and other outcome variables) are moderated by ideology or party identification, but we found inconsistent interaction effects.
* p < .05
Note: The candidate differential ranges from −100 to 100 where greater values indicate higher relative support for Trump over Clinton and was estimated using OLS.
White Privilege Denial and Presidential Approval
We conducted similar analyses of presidential approval for Obama in 2016 and Trump in 2018 to provide further insight into how WPD informs evaluations of political figures. Like the previous analysis, we expect whites higher in white privilege denial to be less approving of President Obama (a Democrat) and more approving of President Trump (a Republican). Again, we expect a similar pattern of results for racial resentment but for different reasons. Whereas respondents high in RR are more likely to support or oppose presidents because of outgroup animus, those high in WPD wish to align themselves with presidents who do not challenge the legitimacy of the racial hierarchy to preserve favorable intergroup comparisons. For ease of presentation, presidential approval is coded one for approval and zero for disapproval (refer to online appendix I for ordered probit results).
Table 4 presents the results of a probit analysis investigating the effects of WPD on presidential approval for Obama and Trump. Consistent with our expectations, we find that WPD is associated with approval of President Trump and disapproval of President Obama. When controlling for other racial constructs in 2018, the effect of WPD remains robust while the effect of RR is rendered statistically insignificant and neither WI nor WC are significant predictors.
* p < .05
Note: All models are binary (0 = Disapprove; 1 = Approve) and were estimated using probit.
Figure 3 depicts the changes in probability of presidential approval when moving from the minimum to the maximum levels of each racial attitude while holding all other variables constant at their mean values. We use the estimates for Trump approval (model 1) without WI and WC to maintain comparability with the Obama estimates. The probability of Obama approval decreases by .437 when moving from the lowest to the highest level of WPD and .444 when looking at the same shift across RR. As for Trump approval, the probability of approval increases by .491 when moving from minimum to maximum WPD whereas the increase is somewhat smaller, .354, for RR. Although WPD and RR have comparable effects on Obama approval, WPD appears to have a stronger association than RR on approval of President Trump. Although speculative, the larger effect of racial resentment in 2016 might reflect the greater amount of racial rhetoric that defined Trump’s 2016 campaign (see Sides, Tesler, and Vavreck Reference Sides, Tesler and Vavreck2019).
As mentioned, in analyses presented in online appendix F, we also examined whether WPD contributed to a measure of affective polarization that captured warmth towards the Republican Party relative to the Democratic Party. Using the same approach for candidate evaluations and presidential approval, we found that WPD is significantly associated with pro-Republican affective polarization in both 2016 and 2018.
White Privilege Denial and Policy Attitudes
We present three sets of analyses of policy attitudes. We begin with a broad sweep, looking at a variety of policy areas scholars have recognized as racial or racialized including affirmative action and social welfare, but also a more contemporary racialized issue, the Affordable Care Act. A second set of analyses focuses on immigration and refugee policies, the foremost racialized issues of the Trump presidency (at least at the time of our surveys). The final set of analyses focuses on symbolic racial issues including the birther conspiracy theory and NFL players “taking a knee” in protest of racial injustice.
White privilege denial should be associated with opposition to racially egalitarian programs and support for more exclusionary policies. In this respect, the broad reach of white privilege denial should be much like racial resentment. Motivated by the desire to maintain positive distinctiveness, white privilege denial involves group differentiation (which necessarily includes both ingroup and outgroup considerations) and thus supporting public policy that maintains the advantaged position of whites relative to people of color. Motivated by outgroup animus, racial resentment is about opposing policies that help racial and ethnic minorities. Since we examine racial and racialized policy, the scope of white privilege denial and racial resentment should be largely the same. In contrast, white identity and white consciousness will be more limited, only informing opinion on policies perceived to benefit whites since, according to Jardina (Reference Jardina2019), white identity and consciousness motivate support for policies that are perceived to protect whites but not policies that are perceived to help racial and ethnic minorities.
The question wording for all items is available in online appendix J. For all the dependent variables, policy attitudes are coded such that higher values reflect greater support for racially conservative opinions.
White Privilege Denial and Racial(ized) Policy Attitudes
We begin with racialized policy areas. Foremost among the policies we examine are affirmative action and social welfare policy. Despite critics (Pratto et al. Reference Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth and Malle1994; Sniderman and Tetlock Reference Sniderman and Tetlock1986), a wealth of studies show that racial animus increases opposition to affirmative action (Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Tarman and Sears Reference Tarman and Sears2005) and social welfare spending (Gilens Reference Gilens1999; Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Rabinowitz et al. Reference Rabinowitz, Sears, Sidanius and Krosnick2009). We also examine racial attitudes toward the Affordable Care Act (ACA) since Tesler (Reference Tesler2012) found that whites high in racial resentment are more likely to oppose the ACA, a policy strongly associated with President Obama. Whereas those who are high in racial resentment will oppose these programs because they hold negative stereotypes of Blacks and find them undeserving, those high in white privilege denial are likely to oppose these programs because they undermine the advantaged position of whites in employment, economic status, and health care coverage, a threat to the positive distinctiveness of whites.
Table 5 presents the results of the analyses of racialized policies. As expected, WPD is strongly associated with increased opposition to affirmative action, social welfare spending, and the Affordable Care Act. Among the other racial constructs, only RR helps explain attitudes on these racialized policies. Our results for WI and WC replicate Jardina’s (Reference Jardina2019) analysis since neither are significant predictors of white opposition to affirmative action or welfare spending. This is not unexpected, however, since she found that white identity and consciousness are relevant to understanding policy that is perceived to affect whites, not racial outgroups (Jardina Reference Jardina2019).
* p < .05
Note: Affirmative Action ranges from 1 (Strongly Favor) to 7 (Strongly Oppose) and was estimated using ordered probit. Welfare Spending ranges from 1 (Greatly increase) to 5 (Greatly decrease) and was estimated using ordered probit. Repeal ACA is binary (0 = Oppose, 1 = Support) and was estimated using probit.
Figure 4 depicts predicted probability changes for the strongest opposition to affirmative action, social welfare spending, and support for repealing the Affordable Care Act moving from the lowest to the highest levels of WPD and RR. The probability of choosing the strongest opposition to affirmative action increases by .45 when moving from the minimum to the maximum of WPD. The change in probability is smaller, an increase of .35, when making the same comparison across levels of RR. For social welfare spending, the probability of expressing the strongest opposition increases by .15 when moving from the lowest to the highest value of WPD. In contrast, moving from the lowest to the highest value of RR produces an increase of .08 in expressing the strongest opposition. Lastly, the probability of strongly supporting the repeal of the ACA increased by .20 when moving from the minimum to the maximum level of WPD and .44 when moving from the lowest to the highest levels of RR.
In sum, white privilege denial and racial resentment both matter for understanding attitudes towards racialized policy areas but the effect sizes varied. For affirmative action and social welfare attitudes, WPD may have a more pronounced effect than RR, suggesting that opposition to these programs is more strongly associated with how they threaten the positive distinctiveness of whites by undermining their advantaged position rather than anti-Black affect. Attitudes towards the ACA, however, appear to be more strongly associated with racial animus, supporting Tesler’s finding (Reference Tesler2012) that its racialization is primarily due to its strong association with President Obama.
White Privilege Denial and Immigration Policy Attitudes
The second set of analyses focuses on immigration and refugee policy, the defining racialized policy issues of the Trump presidency (at least prior to the death of George Floyd). Trump made immigration a key issue during his 2016 campaign as well as during his presidency when he advanced these issues through high-profile executive orders suspending visas from Muslim-majority countries, increasing border security, and restricting the number of refugees allowed into the United States. Although immigration attitudes are informed by a variety of factors (Hainmueller and Hopkins Reference Hainmueller and Hopkins2014), anti-Latino sentiment is central to explaining opposition to immigration in the United States (Abrajano and Hajnal Reference Abrajano and Hajnal2015; Brader, Valentino, and Suhay Reference Brader, Valentino and Suhay2008; Nicholson Reference Nicholson2005; Pérez Reference Pérez2010, Reference Pérez2016; Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2010; Valentino, Brader, and Jardina Reference Valentino, Brader and Jardina2013). Given that Latinos are the face of contemporary immigration in the United States (Valentino, Brader, and Jardina Reference Valentino, Brader and Jardina2013), scholars have largely side-stepped racial resentment given its focus on Blacks. Nevertheless, racial resentment likely taps into a broader prejudice, ethnocentrism (Kinder and Kam Reference Kinder and Kam2009), suggesting it is also likely to shape immigration policy attitudes. Yet, even so, any measure of outgroup animus is inherently limited in explaining immigration attitudes since immigration, and the threat it represents, has implications for the dominant position of whites. Our expectation is that those who seek positive distinctiveness for their group in the denial of white privilege will be more likely to oppose immigration in a variety of forms.
To evaluate the role of white privilege denial on opinion towards immigration and refugee policy we proceed with three sets of analyses. The first considers preferences on five immigration laws combined into a single index ranging from 0 to 1 (Chronbach’s α = .84). The index includes items about increasing border security and building a wall between the United States and Mexico border, eliminating the visa lottery and family-based migration, granting legal status to DACA children, fining police departments that do not report illegal immigrants, and sending to prison deportees that reenter the United States. The second analysis examines a single, broad question about whether “the number of immigrants from foreign countries who are permitted to the U.S. to live” should be increased or decreased. Finally, the third analysis examines an item asking respondents whether they supported President Trump’s executive order to prohibit “Syrian refugees from entering the country” and to “ban immigrants from Iran, Sudan, Yemen, Syria, and Libya.” Full question wording is available in online appendix J. Variables are rescaled from 0 to 1 and coded such that higher values reflect exclusionary attitudes.
Table 6 presents the results of the analyses. In each model, controlling for racial resentment, WPD has a consistently positive and statistically significant effect on support for anti-immigrant and anti-refugee policy. Despite focusing on Blacks, whites who are higher in RR also consistently hold exclusionary attitudes towards immigrants and refugees, a finding that suggests the measure captures a broader prejudice towards people of color. Lastly, table 6 shows that WI has a positive and statistically significant effect on immigration levels but not the immigration laws index nor the refugee order. The opposite pattern is found for WC. It is significantly associated with the immigration laws index and refugee executive order but not immigration levels.
* p < .05
Note: Immigration Laws Index ranges from 0 to 1, composed of 5 items, and was estimated using OLS. Opinion of Immigration Levels ranges from 1 (Increased a Lot) to 5 (Decreased a Lot) and was estimated using ordered probit. Refugee Order is binary (0 = oppose; 1 = support) and was estimated using probit.
Figure 5 presents the predicted probabilities of choosing the most racially conservative response for our analysis of opinion on immigration levels and support for President Trump’s executive order on restricting refugees. In looking at immigration levels, we examined the predicted probability of choosing “Decreased a lot.” The probability of this response increases by .22 when moving from the lowest to highest level of WPD and increases .29 when comparing the lowest to the highest levels of RR. Lastly, moving from the lowest to the highest value of WI increases the probability of “Decreased a lot” by .084. In the analysis of attitudes towards the refugee executive order, the change from the lowest to the highest value of WPD increased support by .48. whereas the same change in RR produced an increase of .60. Furthermore, we considered the shift from lowest to the highest value of WC, which produces a change in probability of “Decreased a lot” by .39. Taken together, both measures of racial attitudes (WPD and RR) and racial solidarity (WI and WC) play an important role informing opinion on these policies with a slightly larger effect for RR suggesting that outgroup animus might play a larger role in explaining immigration and refugee policy attitudes.
White Privilege Denial and Symbolic Political Attitudes
In the final set of analyses, we examine whether white privilege denial informs opinion on two symbolic political issues imbued with race; the birther conspiracy theory and NFL players taking a knee in protest of racial injustice. The birther rumor, the claim that President Obama was born outside the United States, is perhaps the most high-profile conspiracy theory of the Obama era. Although partisanship plays a central role in birtherism (Duran, Nicholson, and Dale Reference Duran, Nicholson and Dale2017; Jardina and Traugott Reference Jardina and Traugott2019; Miller, Saunders, and Farhart Reference Miller, Saunders and Farhart2016), it is also animated by racial animus (Jardina and Traugott Reference Jardina and Traugott2019; Tesler and Sears Reference Tesler and Sears2010). We expect those higher in WPD to be inclined to find a Black president illegitimate since it threatens the advantaged position of whites in the political system to have a Black president.
NFL players kneeling during the national anthem in protest of racial injustice is an explicitly racial matter. Colin Kaepernick, who led the movement, became the face of the controversial protests and the league’s most-disliked player (Rovell Reference Rovell2016). In looking at opposition to the protests, we expect those in denial of white privilege to define what is “appropriate” behavior through the lens of their own experience, discounting the lived experiences of Blacks. In addition, acknowledgement of systemic racial injustice challenges white advantage in the racial justice system, a threat to the positive distinctiveness of whites.
Table 7 presents the results of ordered probit analyses examining belief in the birther conspiracy theory and opposition to the NFL protests. We found WPD to be strongly associated with opposition to the NFL protests, as well as belief in the birther conspiracy. Racial resentment is also a significantly related to opposition to the protests, but it is statistically insignificant for explaining birtherism. This finding suggests whites are motivated to find a Black president illegitimate by way of maintaining positive distinctiveness than through racial animosity. We also find WC to be significantly related to these symbolic racial attitudes suggesting an important role for racial solidarity whereas WI matters for understanding birther attitudes but not the national anthem protests.
* p < .05
Note: Anthem ranges from 1 (Strongly approve) to 5 (Strongly disapprove) and Birther ranges from 1 (Definitely yes) to 4 (Definitely not). Both models estimated using ordered probit.
Figure 6 depicts predicted probabilities at the minimum and maximum levels of WPD and RR for the most extreme response categories, “Strongly Disapproving” of the NFL protests and responding “Definitely not” when asked if Obama was born in the United States. For birtherism, we find little change associated with racial attitudes, as the probability of believing Obama was “Definitely not” born in the United States increases by .05 when moving from the minimum to maximum levels of WPD and .03 comparing the lowest to the highest levels of RR. Similarly, the same move across WI increases the probability of this response by .03, and for WC we find a .09 increase in probability. In contrast, racial attitudes appeared to matter a great deal to the NFL protests. The probability of “Strongly Disapproving” increases by .53 when moving from the lowest to the highest levels of WPD and increases by .49 moving from the lowest to highest values of RR. WC also matters, increasing the probability of “Strongly Disapproving” by .23 moving from the lowest to the highest value. The larger effect sizes for attitudes towards the NFL protests suggest that it is more racialized than birtherism, a highly partisan conspiracy theory.
Discussion and Conclusion
The nature of racial attitudes among the American public is evolving. Where previous studies have focused on creating more palatable ways for whites to express racial animus toward minority groups, primarily Blacks, we examined white privilege as a means of focusing on white acceptance, or denial, of the unearned advantages that accompany whiteness. White privilege is a threat to the positive distinctiveness of whites since its acceptance means delegitimizing favorable intergroup comparisons with other racial groups. Denying white privilege preserves favorable comparisons to groups lower in the racial hierarchy.
Many whites fully acknowledge white privilege, but it is just as common to find whites who do not; nearly half express at least some denial and nearly one-third deny all the items on the WPD scale. Although related to other racial constructs, white privilege denial is distinct from other measures such as racial resentment (Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996), social dominance orientation (Pratto et al. Reference Pratto, Sidanius, Stallworth and Malle1994), and hostile sexism (Cassese and Holman Reference Cassese and Holman2019; Schaffner Reference Schaffner2021). As reported in Jardina (Reference Jardina2019), we also confirmed her findings that white privilege denial is distinct from measures of racial solidarity such as white identity and white group consciousness. Intergroup relations, especially involving race, are inherently complex phenomena, and each racial construct provides a different lens for examination. Despite the wealth of insightful instrumentation scholars have used for understanding race (Kinder Reference Kinder, Huddy, Sears and Levy2013), we believe that understanding is incomplete without accounting for white privilege denial.
We found ample evidence that white privilege denial matters. Examining the question of whether, and how, white privilege denial shaped support for candidates, presidents, and policy attitudes, in each of these dependent variables we found that white privilege denial mattered beyond measures of racial resentment, white identity, and white cconsciousness. White privilege denial gives shape to the party system by fostering support for Republican leaders (Donald Trump as candidate and president) and diminishing support for Democratic leaders (Hillary Clinton and Barack Obama). White privilege denial is also associated with a variety of racial and racialized policy attitudes. Those who deny white privilege are more likely to oppose affirmative action, social welfare spending, and liberal immigration policy. The denial of white privilege is also associated with opposition to NFL players “taking a knee” during the national anthem, and, to a lesser extent, belief in the birther conspiracy theory. The consistency of results for white privilege denial indicates that the political relevance of racial attitudes is not limited to negative evaluations of racial minorities but instead includes other, newer understandings of race, namely whether whites acknowledge their privileged, advantaged place in the racial hierarchy and how acknowledging this advantage diminishes favorable intergroup comparisons.
Racial resentment also played a prominent role in understanding the political views of white people. This was not unexpected. Any study of white racial attitudes in the United States requires an investigation of attitudes towards racial outgroups, especially Blacks. Not only is racial resentment the most prominent measure of white racial attitudes in the study of politics but its relevance has grown, having broadened during the Obama presidency (Tesler Reference Tesler2012, Reference Tesler2016) and deepened in the Trump era (Newman et al. Reference Newman, Merolla, Shah, Lemi, Collingwood and Ramakrishnan2021; Valentino, Neuner, and Vandenbroek Reference Valentino, Neuner and Vandenbroek2018). Yet in looking at the effect sizes for racial resentment and white privilege denial, they were often of similar magnitude suggesting that white privilege denial captures something large and meaningful that racial resentment does not. Given the centrality of racial resentment to the study of politics, the consistently strong and comparable effects we found for white privilege denial is no small feat.
White identity and white consciousness also played an important role in our study. Yet these constructs were less consistently associated with sentiment towards politicians and policy preferences. This pattern of results was not surprising given that white identity and white consciousness are relevant to explaining policies that concern the interests of the racial ingroup, not racial outgroups (Jardina Reference Jardina2019). Given how these measures, especially white consciousness, are conceptually and empirically related to white privilege denial, exploring further how they are associated would be a fruitful line of inquiry.
In one such exploration, we used the white identity and white consciousness items to better understand whites who acknowledge the existence of white privilege. We believe that many of these whites are likely to lament the fact that it exists (lamenters), but some may embrace—or at least appreciate—their privilege (appreciators). Since our measure of white privilege denial cannot distinguish between appreciators and lamenters, we investigated it using white identity and white consciousness (online appendix K). Following Jardina (Reference Jardina2019, 134), we had expected white identifiers (and those with white group consciousness) to be appreciators since it is “the feature of their group they desire to maintain and protect.” Conversely, we had expected whites who were low in racial solidarity to be lamenters since they do not think of themselves in racial group terms. In brief, the results in online appendix K suggest that lamenters are more common than appreciators (although it depends on measurement), but regardless of whether we looked at lamenters or appreciators, privilege-acknowledging whites do not meaningfully differ in their political views. Both lamenters and appreciators are likely to self-identify as liberals, disapprove of President Trump, and hold liberal positions on racial policy. Since both groups hold similar political views, it suggests that some white liberals have moved beyond the invisibility of whiteness recognizing how being white is important to their own identity and politics (see Schildkraut Reference Schildkraut2019).
White privilege denial is about rejecting the notion that whiteness confers unearned advantages but for many whites denial may not go far enough. Most whites, for instance, believe that anti-white bias is more common than anti-Black bias (Norton and Sommers Reference Norton and Sommers2011). In her examination of white consciousness, Jardina (Reference Jardina2019, 67) found a large majority of whites responded that it is at least “somewhat likely” that whites are discriminated against and that roughly one-fifth indicated that such discrimination is “very likely” or “extremely likely.” The consequences of this belief have only recently materialized in electoral politics. Sides, Tesler, and Vavreck (Reference Sides, Tesler and Vavreck2017) show that perceived discrimination against whites was unrelated to support for Republican candidates prior to 2016 but that it had a powerful effect on support for Trump in 2016, especially among whites mostly likely to believe their racial group faces discrimination. Future research should seek to understand the relationship between white privilege denial and perceptions of discrimination and how they inform political attitudes.
Another promising avenue for future research should examine the emotional foundations of white privilege denial. Previous work suggests that anger underpins racial resentment, whereas old-fashioned, biological racism is animated by disgust (Banks Reference Banks2014; Banks and Valentino Reference Banks and Valentino2012). Activating these emotions may trigger different racial beliefs and increase opposition toward policies that increase racial equality such as affirmative action. White privilege, its acceptance or denial, may also very well have distinct emotional foundations. For those accepting white privilege, shame or guilt (see Chudy, Piston, and Shipper Reference Chudy, Piston and Shipper2019) may be the associated emotion but for those who deny its existence, anger or indignation may be the emotional substrate.
In sum, although much work remains, our results have helped establish that whites’ desire for group differentiation (by way of privilege denial) is strongly associated with political attitudes beyond racial resentment, racial group identity, or racial group solidarity. Nevertheless, the observational nature of our data precludes us from claiming a causal effect. Studies of racial attitudes suggest that they are relatively stable at the individual (Henry and Sears Reference Henry and Sears2009; Kinder and Sanders Reference Kinder and Sanders1996; Tesler and Sears Reference Tesler and Sears2010) and aggregate levels (Smith, Kreitzer, and Suo Reference Smith, Kreitzer and Suo2020), but politics can shape racial attitudes (Engelhardt Reference Engelhardt2021) and future research should make use of panel data or experiments to demonstrate a causal relationship.
Given the centrality of race for understanding the American political system, white privilege denial helps us better understand the racial landscape and how it shapes whites’ thinking about politics. In asking whites whether they see themselves as benefactors of race in America, we are better able to understand the nature of their racial thoughts as well as how their racial understandings manifest themselves politically. In so doing, understanding white privilege denial further underscores the significance of race in explaining how white people think about the political world.
Supplementary Materials
To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/S1537592721004102.
Appendix A. White Privilege Items
Appendix B. Distribution of Racial Attitudes
Appendix C. Question Wording for WI, WC, SDO, Hostile Sexism, and Factor Analyses
Appendix D. Robustness Checks, Controlling for SDO and Sexism
Appendix E. Replication of Findings Using the Common Content in 2018 and 2020
Appendix F. Effects of the Racial Attitudes on Pro-Republican Affective Polarization
Appendix G. Candidate Evaluations Analysis—Alternative
Appendix H. Interaction Effects of the Racial Attitudes by Ideology and Party ID
Appendix I. Presidential Approval Ordered Logit Analysis
Appendix J. Question Wording for Policy Items
Appendix K. Exploring the Acknowledgement of White Privilege using WI and WC
Acknowledgement
Earlier versions of this paper were presented at the Annual Meeting of the Western Political Science Association, San Diego, CA, April 18–20, 2019, and of the American Political Science Association, Boston, MA, August 31–September 2, 2018. The authors would like to thank Tom Hansford, Kiku Huckle, Ashley Jardina, Cindy Kam, Jay Yeon Kim, Jenn Merolla, Alex Theodoridis, and Jessica Trounstine for helpful comments and suggestions.