Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T16:43:39.310Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Adolescent precursors of cannabis dependence: findings from the Victorian Adolescent Health Cohort Study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Carolyn Coffey
Affiliation:
Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Parkville, Victoria
John B. Carlin
Affiliation:
Murdoch Children's Research Institute and University of Melbourne, Australia
Michael Lynskey
Affiliation:
Washington University, St Louis, Missouri, USA
Ning Li
Affiliation:
Murdoch Children's Research Institute and University of Melbourne
George C. Patton
Affiliation:
Murdoch Children's Research Institute, Parkville, Victoria, Australia
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Dependence increases the likelihood of adverse consequences of cannabis use, but its aetiology is poorly understood.

Aims

To examine adolescent precursors of young-adult cannabis dependence.

Method

Putative risk factors were measured in a representative sample (n=2032) of secondary students in the State of Victoria, Australia, six times between 1992 and 1995. Cannabis dependence was assessed in 1998, at age 20–21 years.

Results

Of 1601 young adults, 115 met criteria for cannabis dependence. Male gender (OR=2.6, P < 0.01), regular cannabis use (weekly: OR=4.9; daily: OR=4.6, P=0.02), persistent antisocial behaviour (linear effect P=0.03) and persistent cigarette smoking (linear effect P=0.02) independently predicted cannabis dependence. Neither smoking severity (P=0.83) nor persistent psychiatric morbidity (linear effect P=0.26) independently predicted dependence. Regular cannabis use increased risk only in the absence of persistent problematic alcohol use.

Conclusions

Weekly cannabis use marks a threshold for increased risk of later dependence, with selection of cannabis in preference to alcohol possibly indicating an early addiction process.

Type
Papers
Copyright
Copyright © 2003 The Royal College of Psychiatrists 

Half to two-thirds of young adults in the UK, the USA, New Zealand and Australia have used cannabis recreationally (Reference Webb, Ashton and KellyWebb et al, 1996; Reference Fergusson and HorwoodFergusson & Horwood, 2000; Reference Coffey, Carlin and LynskeyCoffey et al, 2002; Reference Johnston, O'Malley and BachmanJohnston et al, 2002). Most have used it infrequently without health consequences, but a minority progress to harmful heavy use (Reference Fergusson and HorwoodFergusson & Horwood, 1997). Adverse consequences include accidental injury, educational and legal difficulties, mental health problems and respiratory effects beyond those attributable to tobacco use alone (Reference AmeriAmeri, 1999; Reference Hall and BaborHall & Babor, 2000; Reference Taylor, Poulton and MoffittTaylor et al, 2000; Reference JohnsJohns, 2001; Reference AshtonAshton, 2002). Cannabis dependence is increasingly recognised as a further consequence of heavy use, with a lifetime risk in ever-users of about 10% (Reference Anthony, Warner and KesslerAnthony et al, 1994). The development of dependence probably prolongs use and increases the potential for harm (Reference AshtonAshton, 2002). Increasing use of more effective methods of drug delivery and increasing drug potency may underlie the development of dependence, but other contributing factors remain little explored (Reference Hall and BaborHall & Babor, 2000). An understanding of the adolescent antecedents of dependence can inform the extent to which substance exposures increase risks for dependence as opposed to other factors such as intercurrent emotional or behavioural disorders (Reference Fergusson and HorwoodFergusson & Horwood, 2000).

METHOD

Procedure and sample

Between August 1992 and December 1998 we conducted a seven-wave cohort study of adolescent health in Victoria, Australia. The cohort was defined using a two-stage sampling procedure in which we selected two classes at random from each of 44 government, Catholic and independent schools (total number of students 60 905). School retention rates to year nine in the year of sampling were 98%. One class from each school entered the cohort in the latter part of the ninth school year (wave 1) and the second class 6 months later, early in the tenth year (wave 2). Participants were subsequently reviewed at a further four 6-month intervals during their teens (waves 3 to 6) with a final follow-up at the age of 20-21 years (wave 7), 3 years after the final school year (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1 Participation rates of 2032 secondary school students in the Victorian Adolescent Health Cohort Study.

Adolescent phase: waves 1 to 6

Altogether, 1947 adolescents (96% of the intended sample) participated at least once during waves 1 to 6, with a gender ratio (males 48.6%) similar to that in Victorian schools at the time of sampling (Australian Bureau of Statistics, 1993). Surveys were self-administered at school using laptop computers, thereby allowing the use of branched questions. Participants unavailable for follow-up at school completed the questionnaire by telephone.

Young-adult survey (wave 7, 1998)

The young-adult survey was carried out by telephone using computer-assisted interviews consistent with the adolescent phase. A total of 1601 young adults (82% of cohort participants; mean age 20.7 (s.d.=0.5) years, 46.0% male) were interviewed between April and December 1998. All analyses are based on this subset. Reasons for non-participation at wave 7 were: refusal (n=152); person traced but non-contactable (n=59); person not traced (lost) (n=133); and death (n=2). Of the 1601 participants interviewed, 71%, 27% and 3% respectively lived at home, with others or alone; 82% had completed the final school year; 85% had commenced post-school study, with 68% still studying at the time of the interview; 82% were in paid employment; 8% were neither studying nor employed.

Characteristics of non-completers at wave 7 were examined in a multivariate logistic regression model. Males were over-represented (odds ratio (OR)=1.9, 95% CI 1.5-2.4), as were those who had experienced parental divorce or separation (OR=1.8, 95% CI 1.4-2.5) and those reporting daily smoking at study inception (OR=2.1, 95% CI 1.5-2.9).

Outcome measure: DSM—IV cannabis dependence

A DSM—IV diagnosis of dependence required evidence that, within the previous 12 months, an individual continued cannabis use despite significant substance-related problems (American Psychiatric Association, 1994), supported by endorsement of three of the following seven criteria: tolerance to the effects of cannabis; withdrawal symptoms on ceasing or reducing use; cannabis used in larger amounts or for a longer period than intended; a persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to reduce or cease use; a disproportionate amount of time spent obtaining, using and recovering from use; social, recreational or occupational activities reduced or given up owing to cannabis use; and use continued despite knowledge of physical or psychological problems induced by cannabis (American Psychiatric Association, 1994).

To generate the DSM—IV criteria for a diagnosis of cannabis dependence, the Composite International Diagnostic Interview 2.1, 12-month version (CIDI; Reference Hall, Teesson and LynskeyHall et al, 1999), was administered. We assessed cannabis dependence only in participants reporting weekly cannabis use in the preceding 12 months, to minimise responder fatigue. We considered that a diagnosis of cannabis dependence was consistent only with regular cannabis use, given the DSM—IV description of substance dependence as occurring with a ‘pattern of repeated [substance] self-administration’ (American Psychiatric Association, 1994).

Population prevalence estimates for cannabis dependence and dependence symptoms in the cohort at wave 7 have been reported earlier (Reference Coffey, Carlin and LynskeyCoffey et al, 2002). We estimated that 7% of the cohort, equivalent to 13% of ever-users, met criteria for DSM—IV cannabis dependence within the preceding 12 months. The most prevalent symptoms were persistent desire or unsuccessful abstinence attempts (10%) and unintentional use (8%). Tolerance (2%) and social consequences of use (1%) were the least prevalent symptoms. Eleven wave 7 participants did not report on their cannabis use and were classified as non-users for all analyses.

Measures: waves 1 to 6

Demographic variables

Gender and country of birth were recorded at study entry. Parental partnership status was assessed throughout the study.

Cannabis use

Cannabis use during the previous 6 months was assessed using the following rating scale: never used; not used in the past 6 months; a few times; monthly; weekly; daily. Those reporting the use of cannabis at least a few times in the past 6 months were classified as ‘any users’.

Cigarette smoking

Participants reporting that they had smoked on 6 or 7 days in the previous week were categorised as daily smokers. Occasional smoking was defined as reporting smoking in the past month, but on fewer than 6 days in the past week.

Alcohol consumption

Participants reporting that they had drunk alcohol in the week before the survey completed a 1-week retrospective alcohol diary (specifying beverage and quantity), allowing derivation of two measures of problematic alcohol consumption: ‘frequent drinking’ on 3 or more days in the previous week, and ‘high-dose drinking’ with an average consumption of 5 units or more of ethanol per drinking day (1 unit is equivalent to one standard drink containing 9 g ethanol).

Antisocial behaviour

Ten items from the Moffitt & Silva (Reference Moffitt and Silva1988) self-report Early Delinquency Scale assessed antisocial behaviour relating to property damage, interpersonal conflict and theft in the previous 6 months. Antisocial behaviours were categorised according to whether more than one behaviour was endorsed ‘more than once’, in order to distinguish participants with more-global antisocial behaviours.

Psychiatric morbidity

A computerised form of the Clinical Interview Schedule (CIS) was used to quantify the severity of psychiatric morbidity (Reference Lewis, Pelosi and ArayaLewis et al, 1992). Scores greater than 11 were taken to indicate psychiatric morbidity, reflecting the level at which clinical intervention is appropriate.

Explanatory variables: waves 1 to 6

Responses on adolescent risk factors (waves 1 to 6) were summarised as follows:

  1. (a) The number of waves in which a condition was reported. It was necessary to collapse small categories, so we reclassified into four levels: none, one wave (indicating experimentation), two or three waves (indicating moderate exposure), and four to six waves (indicating persisting exposure and implying early onset, that is, the behaviour was necessarily reported at least by wave 3). This categorisation was applied to any cannabis use, any cigarette smoking, frequent alcohol use, high-dose alcohol use, antisocial behaviour and psychiatric morbidity.

  2. (b) The maximum level reported during the six waves of follow-up for cannabis use (none, occasional, weekly, daily) and cigarette smoking (none, less than daily, daily).

Missing waves of data collection: waves 1 to 6

Seventy-five per cent of the cohort completed five of the first six waves of data collection, but owing to the staged recruitment, 54% of observations were missing from the first wave (Fig. 1). Missing observations for waves 2, 3, 4, 5 and 6 were 11%, 13%, 16%, 19% and 21% respectively. Overall, 59% of participants missed at least one wave. Multiple imputation was used to handle this fact, enabling summary measures to be defined for each participant in each of five ‘completed’ data-sets. Imputation was performed using the multivariate mixed effects model of Schafer & Yucel (Reference Schafer and Yucel2002).

Data analysis

Logistic regression analyses were performed on the binary outcome of cannabis dependence. In multivariable models, exposure effects were estimated as linear trends in the log odds ratio across ordered categories of exposure on explanatory variables. Two-tailed P values are reported based on Wald tests.

All analyses were performed using Stata 7.0 for Windows (Reference StataStata, 2001). We used the method of Rubin (Reference Rubin1987) for creating valid inferences with the multiple imputation model, by combining over standard analyses performed on each of the imputed data-sets. Software for facilitating these analyses was written in Stata (details available from the authors upon request).

Ethical approval

Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the Royal Children's Hospital Ethics in Human Research Committee. Written parental consent was obtained at study inception and individuals gave informed verbal consent before commencing the wave 7 interview.

RESULTS

Young-adult cannabis dependence

Of 1601 young-adult participants, 936 (66% of males; 52% of females) interviewed in wave 7 reported ever using cannabis and 115 (7% of wave 7 participants) met criteria for DSM—IV cannabis dependence within the past 12 months. Participants with cannabis dependence were less likely to be female (10.3% of males and 4.5% of females; OR=0.41, 95% CI 0.27-0.61). Individuals of Australian birth (OR=2.5, 95% CI 1.1-5.4), with parental divorce or separation (OR=1.7, 95% CI 1.1-2.6) and neither studying nor employed (OR=2.9, 95% CI 1.7-4.9) were at increased risk of dependence.

Overall, 32% (95% CI 30-35) of the 1601 wave 7 participants reported cannabis use in the adolescent waves 1-6. Eighteen per cent (95% CI 14-21) of wave 1-6 users and 32% (95% CI 25-39) of those reporting at least weekly use later met criteria for cannabis dependence. Conversely, of the 115 with cannabis dependence at wave 7: 17% (95% CI 10-25) reported occasional use in waves 1-6; 22% (95% CI 10-34) weekly use; 38% (95% CI 27-49) daily use; and 22% (95% CI 14-30) initiated cannabis use after wave 6.

Univariate associations between young-adult cannabis dependence (wave 7) and adolescent exposures (waves 1-6)

The frequencies of a range of adolescent factors were estimated and crude associations between these and cannabis dependence were assessed (Table 1).

Table 1 Estimated frequency of time-varying adolescent measures and their association with cannabis dependence at age 20 years (n=1601): odds ratios (OR) from univariate logistic regression models

Adolescent measure: waves 1 to 6 Estimated frequency Cannabis dependence at age 20 years
Category n 95% CI OR 95% CI P
Maximum frequency
Cannabis use None 1083 1046-1120 1 <0.011
Less than weekly 332 299-364 4.7 2.7-8.2
Weekly 127 106-149 20 11-35
Daily 59 43-75 23 11-47
Cigarette smoking None 831 790-871 1 <0.011
Less than daily 401 366-435 4.3 1.9-9.6
Daily 370 336-403 13 6.8-23
Persistence
Any cannabis use None 1083 1046-1120 1 <0.011
1 wave 140 114-166 3.9 1.8-8.2
2 or 3 waves 151 126-177 6.2 3.2-12
4 to 6 waves 227 198-255 17 9.9-27
Linear effect2 2.5 2.1-2.9
Any cigarette None 831 790-871 1 <0.011
smoking 1 wave 171 145-197 2.2 0.70-7.0
2 or 3 waves 186 158-214 4.5 2.0-10
4 to 6 waves 413 378-448 13 6.8-24
Linear effect2 2.3 2.0-2.8 <0.01
High-dose alcohol None 890 850-931 1 <0.011
use 1 wave 308 275-342 3.5 1.7-7.1
2 or 3 waves 276 246-306 5.6 2.8-11
4 to 6 waves 126 105-148 10 5.2-20
Linear effect2 2.1 1.7-2.6 <0.01
Frequent alcohol use None 1358 1328-1387 1 <0.011
1 wave 162 135-188 2.5 1.3-4.5
2 or 3 waves 71 54-88 2.7 1.1-6.8
4 to 6 waves 11 4-17 4.0 0.80-20
Linear effect2 1.8 1.3-2.3 <0.01
Psychiatric morbidity None 857 808-905 1
(CIS score > 11) 1 wave 245 202-288 1.6 0.84-3.1 0.031
2 or 3 waves 272 241-302 1.8 0.84-3.7
4 to 6 waves 228 199-257 2.1 1.1-3.8
Linear effect2 1.3 1.1-1.5 <0.01
Two or more None 1300 1269-1331 1 <0.011
antisocial 1 wave 153 130-177 3.7 2.1-6.6
behaviours 2 or 3 waves 104 83-124 5.8 3.2-11
4 to 6 waves 44 29-59 11 5.2-25
Linear effect2 2.3 1.9-2.8 <0.01

Maximum frequency of cannabis use and cigarette smoking

Maximum frequency of cannabis use in waves 1-6 showed strong association with cannabis dependence in wave 7, with both weekly and daily maximum use carrying about a 20-fold increase in odds, indicating evidence of a threshold at weekly use. There was a strong increase in frequency of dependence with increase in maximum frequency of cigarette smoking from occasional to daily.

Persistence of adolescent behaviours

Strong associations, with evidence of linear relationships, were observed for the number of waves in which cannabis use, cigarette smoking, high-dose drinking and antisocial behaviour were reported, with a two-fold or greater average odds increase with each increase in level of reporting frequency. For all four measures the most persistent levels carried elevated odds of ten-fold or greater. A weaker association, but still with some evidence of a linear relationship, was observed with the number of waves in which psychiatric morbidity was identified, with an average increase in odds of 1.3 with increasing level of reporting frequency. With frequent drinking the clearest difference was between ‘none’ and ‘some’, with weak evidence for a dose-related effect.

Independent associations between young-adult cannabis dependence (wave 7) and adolescent exposures (waves 1-6)

We used multiple logistic regression to quantify the independent predictive associations and to adjust for possible confounding. To aid parsimony, measures of persistence (all of which showed univariate linear relationships) were entered in the multivariate model as linear effects. After adjustment, the only adolescent measures (apart from gender) demonstrating an independent relationship with cannabis dependence were: maximum frequency of cannabis use; and the number of waves in which each of cigarette smoking and antisocial behaviour were reported (Table 2). There was no evidence of first-order interaction effects between gender and any explanatory variable.

Table 2 Independent predictive associations between background and adolescent time-varying factors and cannabis dependence at age 20 years: odds ratios (OR) from multiple logistic regression

Adolescent measure: waves 1 to 6 Category Cannabis dependence at age 20 years
OR 95%CI P
Background factors
Female gender 0.38 0.22-0.66 <0.01
Australian birth 2.0 0.82-4.8 0.13
Parental divorce/separation 1.0 0.63-1.72 0.87
Maximum frequency of use
Cannabis use None 1 0.021
Less than weekly 1.7 0.59-4.7
Weekly 4.9 1.3-19
Daily 4.6 1.0-21
Cigarette smoking None 1 0.831
Less than daily 0.73 0.18-3.0
Daily 0.71 0.13-3.8
Persistence
Any cannabis use Linear effect2 1.1 0.70-1.7 0.71
Any cigarette smoking Linear effect2 1.9 1.1-3.2 0.02
Frequent alcohol use Linear effect2 0.69 0.46-1.0 0.07
High-dose alcohol use Linear effect2 1.1 0.81-1.5 0.55
Psychiatric morbidity Linear effect2 1.1 0.91-1.4 0.26
Two or more antisocial behaviours Linear effect2 1.3 1.0-1.7 0.03

The relationship between cannabis dependence and persistent frequent drinking in adolescence changed direction, from a risk association in the univariate model to a protective association in the adjusted model. We therefore examined the interaction between this factor and maximum cannabis use, adjusting only for factors influential in the multivariate model reported in Table 2. We selected individuals reporting frequent drinking in two or more waves, and identified evidence of an interaction between this characteristic and maximum weekly or daily cannabis use (Wald χ2 P=0.01). Elevated risk for later dependence associated with maximum weekly or daily cannabis use was evident only in participants not reporting frequent drinking in two or more waves (OR=7.4, 95% CI 3.9-14; P <0.01). There was no evidence that those reporting both weekly or daily cannabis use and multiple waves of frequent drinking were at risk of later cannabis dependence (OR=1.2, 95% CI 0.28-5.0; P=0.81).

Confounding by cigarette smoking and antisocial behaviour on the effect of early-onset cannabis use

The reason for a lack of independent association between cannabis dependence and early cannabis use was explored in three further models. We characterised individuals who reported using cannabis in the first three waves of follow-up, i.e. in year 9 or year 10 (average 359 of a total of 517 users in waves 1 to 6). We compared the association of early use v. later onset only in young adult participants reporting any adolescent use, progressively adjusting for the persistence of smoking and antisocial behaviour (Table 3). Both cigarette smoking and antisocial behaviour confounded the effect of early cannabis use. Persistent cigarette smoking showed the greater confounding effect, particularly when reported in four or more waves, that is, with early onset. After adjusting for these factors there was no evidence of an independent association between early cannabis use and later dependence.

Table 3 Secondary analysis of the association between early cannabis uptake and cannabis dependence in adolescent cannabis users (=517), showing the effect of adjusting for the number of waves of cigarette smoking and antisocial behaviour: odds ratios (OR) from univariate and multivariate logistic regression models

Adolescent measure (waves 1 to 6) Association with cannabis dependence at age 20 years
Unadjusted Adjusted for smoking Adjusted for antisocial behaviour Adjusted for smoking and antisocial behaviour
OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI OR 95% CI
Any cannabis use in waves 1 to 3 (=359) 2.1 1.1-4.0 1.7 0.85-3.3 1.8 0.95-3.6 1.4 0.70-2.9
Cigarette smoking
None 1 1
1 wave 1.3 0.35-4.8 1.2 0.31-4.4
2 or 3 waves 1.8 0.63-5.3 1.8 0.61-5.1
4 to 6 waves 2.9 1.1-7.2 2.7 1.1-7.0
Antisocial behaviour
None 1 1
1 wave 1.8 1.0-3.5 1.9 1.0-3.6
2 or 3 waves 2.2 1.1-4.4 2.2 1.1-4.5
4 to 6 waves 4.0 1.7-9.2 3.8 1.7-8.7

DISCUSSION

Almost 60% of a representative sample of young adults aged 20-21 years in Victoria, Australia, reported ever having used cannabis and 7% met DSM—IV criteria for cannabis dependence in the 12 months prior to survey. Progression to dependence was common, in that one in five adolescent users were later classified as dependent in young adulthood. Weekly cannabis use was an even stronger predictor, with one in three meeting the criteria for dependence. Additional predictors were male gender, early and persistent cigarette smoking, and early and persistent antisocial behaviour. In contrast, regular drinking in the teenage years appeared protective against cannabis dependence.

Cannabis dependence was assessed at an age of peak cannabis use in a close-to-representative sample with high participation into young adulthood. To circumvent bias from non-response during the adolescent waves, multiple imputation of missing covariate values was performed using a model based on background measures (available for 96% of the sampling frame). This allowed us to define exposure measures of time-varying adolescent behaviours based on all six waves of data collected in the adolescent phase for all 1601 participants who were interviewed in wave 7 aged 20-21 years.

A potential study limitation was the underspecification of cannabis dependence. First, although the response rate in wave 7 was high, differential under-ascertainment of illicit substance users — a notoriously difficult group to reach — might have occurred. Second, as a third of young adult cannabis users had commenced using only in the preceding 3 years (that is, since wave 6), it is likely that some currently non-dependent participants would develop cannabis dependence in the next few years (Reference Rosenberg and AnthonyRosenberg & Anthony, 2001). We have assumed that the risk profile for cannabis dependence in our sample would be the same for all members of the cohort, but these possible sources of error could result in attenuation of the observed associations.

In defining adolescent measures of smoking we elected not to distinguish between persistent occasional smoking and daily smoking. This decision was taken to aid parsimony and was supported by the similarity in risk association of occasional and daily smoking in the adjusted model describing cannabis dependence. We assessed persistence only in problematic alcohol use, as ‘any’ alcohol use was too common to be informative.

Predictors

Gender

Males were marginally more likely than females to use cannabis overall, but the transition to dependence was considerably more likely in males. We found no evidence of effect modification by gender, indicating that some underlying unmeasured factors were responsible. The suggestion that gender differences might be due to differing opportunity rather than differing transition rates is not supported by our findings (Reference Van Etten and AnthonyVan Etten & Anthony, 2001).

Adolescent cannabis use, antisocial behaviour and cigarette smoking

Early initiation of cannabis use, often preceded by antisocial behaviour and cigarette smoking, is generally accepted as an important predictor of escalation in drug use (Fergusson & Horwood, Reference Fergusson and Horwood1997, Reference Fergusson and Horwood1999). Although we found that early cannabis uptake predicted later dependence in the crude analysis, cigarette smoking and antisocial behaviour largely accounted for this effect in the adjusted model. Furthermore, as no dose effect was evident with frequency of cigarette smoking, our findings are consistent with the suggestion of Bierut et al (Reference Bierut, Dinwiddie and Begleiter1998) that daily smoking is not a specific marker for an underlying vulnerability to cannabis dependence. This non-specific association with cigarette smoking probably reflects the social environment in which both activities occur, rather than individual biological susceptibility.

Why does early deviant behaviour predict cannabis dependence? It is possible that the prolonged cannabis exposure that often accompanies early deviant behaviour might bring forward the transitions from occasional use to regular use and thence to dependent use evident in our young adult sample. If this is so, the effect could moderate as the cohort ages, because older initiators might make the transition to dependence later.

The threshold of risk that we observed with weekly cannabis use indicates that it is the transition to regular use that provides sufficient drug exposure in the development of early dependent use. The slow metabolism of cannabis results in the persistence of measurable physical and psychological changes well beyond the duration of the subjective effects (Reference AmeriAmeri, 1999). The maintenance of a low but stable frequency of intake might be sufficient to produce longlasting neuro-adaptive changes thought to be associated with the ‘drug-wanting, seeking and taking’ process which occurs with the initiation of addictive behaviour (Reference Hyman and MalenkaHyman & Malenka, 2001). Interestingly, out-of-control use early in the cannabisusing career has been reported to distinguish individuals who make the transition to dependence from non-dependent users, supporting the notion of an early biological response (Reference Rosenberg and AnthonyRosenberg & Anthony, 2001).

Adolescent alcohol use

An apparently counterintuitive finding was that persistent frequent alcohol use as a teenager negated the risk of developing cannabis dependence in regular cannabis users. It is well established that problematic adolescent alcohol use is one of the constellation of behaviours associated with cannabis initiation (e.g. Reference Donovan and JessorDonovan & Jessor, 1985), but our findings indicate that a different picture emerges, with escalation of use in the transition between adolescence and adulthood. This reflects the divergence in criminality in the transition to young adulthood observed in early drug users compared with adolescent alcohol users identified by Newcomb & Bentler (Reference Newcomb and Bentler1988: pp. 102-119). Our findings may therefore illustrate a social process whereby individuals select into either a predominantly alcohol-using or a cannabis-using lifestyle. From the physiological perspective, preferential cannabis use as an early indication of dependence is consistent with a substance-specific biological susceptibility to addiction (Reference Hyman and MalenkaHyman & Malenka, 2001). Selective regular cannabis use during adolescence may mark a neurophysiological and psychological precursor of dependence.

Adolescent psychiatric morbidity

Although cannabis use has been linked with increased rates of depression and anxiety cross-sectionally (Reference JohnsJohns, 2001), we did not find that adolescent psychiatric morbidity independently predicted cannabis dependence. This observation argues against self-medication as a mechanism for continuing problematic cannabis use beyond the teenage years and is consistent with earlier findings (Reference McGee, Williams and PoultonMcGee et al, 2000). Conversely, we have reported separately that regular cannabis use in adolescence predicts later psychiatric morbidity in young women (Reference Patton, Coffey and CarlinPatton et al, 2002).

Implications

Hall & Babor (Reference Hall and Babor2000) pointed out that we have not yet adequately explored the patho-physiological consequences of cannabis use — a process that took many years with tobacco and eventually led to broadranging policies aimed at reducing consumption. The recent reclassification of cannabis from a class B drug to a class C drug by the Home Office in the UK in part reflects a view that cannabis use poses a lesser public health problem than use of other illicit substances. The lethality and withdrawal severity of cannabis may indeed differ from other drugs, but its use is far more common (Reference Hall, Teesson and LynskeyHall et al, 1999; Reference Johnston, O'Malley and BachmanJohnston et al, 2002). As well as the increasing prevalence of cannabis use in young people, the transition rate to dependence would appear to be increasing, with concomitant personal, social and physical harms resulting from prolonged heavy use and addictive behaviour (Reference Hall and BaborHall & Babor, 2000; Reference AshtonAshton, 2002). In 1990-1992 it was estimated that 9% of ever-users were at life-time risk of dependence (Reference Anthony, Warner and KesslerAnthony et al, 1994) but more recent estimates report that between 13% and 16% of users are at risk by their early 20s (Reference Poulton, Brooke and MoffittPoulton et al, 1997; Reference Fergusson and HorwoodFergusson & Horwood, 2000; Reference Coffey, Carlin and LynskeyCoffey et al, 2002). The case for a more concerted public health response seems strong.

Clinical Implications and Limitations

CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

  1. Progression from adolescent cannabis use to dependence in young adulthood is common.

  2. Adolescent weekly cannabis use marked a threshold of risk for later dependence, particularly in the absence of persistent frequent alcohol use.

  3. Self-medication did not appear to mediate the escalation of cannabis use in the transition from adolescence to young adulthood.

LIMITATIONS

  1. Missing observations during the adolescent phase of follow-up might have biased estimates of time-varying exposures.

  2. Differential loss to follow-up of illicit substance users in the young-adult phase might have occurred.

  3. Cannabis dependence might have been underspecified, resulting in attenuation of observed associations.

Acknowledgement

The authors acknowledge the support of the National Health and Medical Research Council.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Ameri, A. (1999) The effects of cannabinoids on the brain. Progress in Neurobiology, 58, 315348.Google Scholar
American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edn) (DSM–IV). Washington, DC: APA.Google Scholar
Anthony, J. C., Warner, L. A. & Kessler, R. C. (1994) Comparative epidemiology of dependence on tobacco, alcohol, controlled substances, and inhalents: basic findings from the National Comorbidity Survey. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 2, 244268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ashton, H. (2002) Cannabis or health? Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 15, 247253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Australian Bureau of Statistics (1993) Australia's Young People. Canberra: Australian Government Publishing Service.Google Scholar
Bierut, L. J., Dinwiddie, S. H., Begleiter, H., et al (1998) Familial transmission of substance dependence: alcohol, marijuana, cocaine, and habitual smoking: a report from the Collaborative Study of the Genetics of Alcoholism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 55, 982988.Google Scholar
Coffey, C., Carlin, J. B., Lynskey, M., et al (2002) Cannabis dependence in young adults: an Australian population study. Addiction, 97, 187194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donovan, J. E. & Jessor, R. (1985) Structure of problem behaviour in adolescence and young adulthood. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53, 890904.Google Scholar
Fergusson, D. M. & Horwood, L. J. (1997) Early onset cannabis use and psychosocial adjustment in young adults. Addiction, 92, 279296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fergusson, D. M. & Horwood, L. J. (1999) Prospective childhood predictors of deviant peer affiliations in adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 581592.Google Scholar
Fergusson, D. M. & Horwood, L. J. (2000) Cannabis use and dependence in a New Zealand birth cohort. New Zealand Medical Journal, 113, 156158.Google Scholar
Hall, W. & Babor, T. F. (2000) Cannabis use and public health: assessing the burden (editorial). Addiction, 95, 485490.Google Scholar
Hall, W., Teesson, M., Lynskey, M., et al (1999) The 12-month prevalence of substance use and ICD-10 substance use disorders in Australian adults: findings from the National Survey of Mental Health and Well-Being. Addiction, 94, 15411550.Google Scholar
Hyman, S. E. & Malenka, R. C. (2001) Addiction and the brain: the neurobiology of compulsion and its persistence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 695703.Google Scholar
Johns, A. (2001) Psychiatric effects of cannabis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 178, 116122.Google Scholar
Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M. & Bachman, J. G. (2002) Monitoring the Future: National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975–2001. Vol. II: College Students and Adults Aged 19–40 (NIH Publication no. 02–5107). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse.Google Scholar
Lewis, G., Pelosi, A. J., Araya, R., et al (1992) Measuring psychiatric disorder in the community: a standardized assessment for use by lay interviewers. Psychological Medicine, 22, 465486.Google Scholar
McGee, R., Williams, S., Poulton, R. G., et al (2000) A longitudinal study of cannabis use and mental health from adolescence to early adulthood. Addiction, 95, 491503.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moffitt, T. E. & Silva, P. A. (1988) Self-reported delinquency: results from an instrument for New Zealand. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Criminology, 21, 227240.Google Scholar
Newcomb, M. D. & Bentler, P. M. (1988) Consequences of Adolescent Use: Impact on the Lives of Young Adults. Newbury Park: Sage.Google Scholar
Patton, G. P., Coffey, C., Carlin, J. B., et al (2002) The mental health of young cannabis users: findings from the Victorian Adolescent Health Cohort Study. BMJ, 325, 11991212.Google Scholar
Poulton, R. G., Brooke, M., Moffitt, T. E., et al (1997) Prevalence and correlates of cannabis use and dependence in young New Zealanders. New Zealand Medical Journal, 110, 6870.Google ScholarPubMed
Rosenberg, M. & Anthony, J. C. (2001) Early clinical manifestations of cannabis dependence in a community sample. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 64, 123131.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. B. (1987) Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. New York: John Wiley & Sons.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schafer, J. L. & Yucel, R. M. (2002) Computational strategies for multivariate linear mixed-effects models with missing values. Journal of Computational and Graphical Statistics, 11, 437457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stata, (2001) Stata Statistical Software: Release 7.0. College Station, TX: Stata Corporation.Google Scholar
Taylor, D. R., Poulton, R., Moffitt, T. E., et al (2000) The respiratory effects of cannabis dependence in young adults. Addiction, 95, 16691677.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Etten, M. L. & Anthony, J. C. (2001) Male–female differences in transitions from first drug opportunity to first use: searching for subgroup variation by age, race, region and urban status. Journal of Women's Health and Gender-based Medicine, 10, 797804.Google Scholar
Webb, E., Ashton, C. H., Kelly, P., et al (1996) Alcohol and drug use in UK university students. Lancet, 348, 922925.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Participation rates of 2032 secondary school students in the Victorian Adolescent Health Cohort Study.

Figure 1

Table 1 Estimated frequency of time-varying adolescent measures and their association with cannabis dependence at age 20 years (n=1601): odds ratios (OR) from univariate logistic regression models

Figure 2

Table 2 Independent predictive associations between background and adolescent time-varying factors and cannabis dependence at age 20 years: odds ratios (OR) from multiple logistic regression

Figure 3

Table 3 Secondary analysis of the association between early cannabis uptake and cannabis dependence in adolescent cannabis users (=517), showing the effect of adjusting for the number of waves of cigarette smoking and antisocial behaviour: odds ratios (OR) from univariate and multivariate logistic regression models

Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.