Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T16:15:08.312Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The effect of accumulated losses on perceptions of legitimacy

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 January 2024

Anna Kern*
Affiliation:
Department of Political Sciences, Ghent University, Gent, Belgium
Sofie Marien
Affiliation:
Centre for Political Research, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium
Lala Muradova
Affiliation:
Department of Political and Social Sciences, Universitat Pompeu Fabra, Barcelona, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Anna Kern; Email: anna.kern@ugent.be
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

How do citizens react to repeated losses in politics? This paper argues that experiencing accumulated losses creates strong incentives to externalize responsibility for these losses to the decision-making procedure, which can, in turn, erode legitimacy perceptions among the public. Using a survey experiment (N = 2,146) simulating accumulated losses in a series of direct votes among Irish citizens, we find that decision acceptance and the perceived legitimacy of the decision-making procedure diminish with every loss. Three accumulated losses depress the perceived legitimacy of the political system. These effects are mediated by procedural fairness perceptions, suggesting that even when democratic procedures are used, accumulated losses can induce a belief that the process and system are rigged.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Consortium for Political Research

Introduction

Coping with the experience of loss is inherent in politics, yet challenging to most people (Mercer, Reference Mercer2005; Pierce et al., Reference Pierce, Rogers and Snyder2016). At the same time, democracies’ viability depends on their ability to secure the support of those on the losing side of a political decision (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Ola2005; Clayton et al., Reference Clayton, Davis, Nyhan, Porter, Ryan and Wood2021). It is critical that losers recognize ‘the legitimacy of a procedure that has produced an outcome deemed to be undesirable’, as Nadeau and Blais (Reference Nadeau and Blais1993: 553) noted in their seminal work on losers’ consent. Today democracies are experiencing multiple crises, accentuated by the erosion of basic democratic norms and electoral participation, and, the rise of political polarization and fake news (Vosoughi et al., Reference Vosoughi, Deb and Aral2018; Przeworski, Reference Przeworski2019). In this context, the long-standing democratic challenge to keep political losers on board and to obtain their acceptance and compliance with decisions voluntarily becomes ever more difficult (Ward and Tavits, Reference Ward and Tavits2019; Spina, Reference Spina2021).

The use of democratic procedures can alleviate the negative effects that come with the experience of loss through the reassurance that there will be a fair chance to win the next time around (Esaiasson, Reference Esaiasson2011; see also Mauk, Reference Mauk2020; Werner and Marien, Reference Werner and Marien2020). But what if this next time does not offer the anticipated win? Extensive research has been carried out on citizens’ reactions to winning or losing in politics once,Footnote 1 but we know surprisingly little about how the public reacts to the experience of accumulated political losses. In a similar vein, the conditions under which the use of democratic procedures could alleviate the negative effects associated with political loss remain vastly understudied.

There are indications in the context of the electoral competition that accumulated losses can be detrimental to perceptions of legitimacy (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Ola2005; Curini et al., Reference Curini, Jou and Memoli2012; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Chu, Wu and Thomassen2014; Daniller and Mutz, Reference Daniller and Mutz2019; Kern and Kölln, Reference Kern and Kölln2022), but it is largely unclear when and why this happens. The extant literature needs to be advanced in at least three important directions. First, when working with cross-sectional electoral data, causal inference is difficult, because election losers might differ from election winners apart from their electoral status and years pass by between elections with other developments, such as political scandals or crises, also taking place (Bowler and Karp, Reference Bowler and Karp2004; Chanley et al., Reference Chanley, Rudolph and Rahn2000). Second, to our knowledge, studies investigating accumulated losses are limited in the number of incidences of loss studied. How people react to more than two losses in a row remains unclear. Finally, despite the increased popularity of the direct involvement of citizens in political decision-making (Qvortrup, Reference Qvortrup2017) and the oft-held belief that it can strengthen perceptions of legitimacy, the studies on accumulated losses are limited to the electoral setting.Footnote 2 This study is specifically designed to tackle these challenges.

This paper advances a theoretical argument on the effect of accumulated losses. As accumulated loss is exceptionally painful, we expect this experience to be distinct from losing just once or nonconsecutively. To cope psychologically with accumulated losses, individuals engage in post hoc rationalization, whereby they start doubting the fairness of the decision-making process that produces a series of unfavorable outcomes. We expect this to happen independently of its actual fairness. Once citizens start doubting the fairness of the process, their perceptions of the legitimacy of the system as a whole are affected negatively, putting losers’ consent at risk.

We test this theoretical argument’s main attitudinal predictions and assumptions with a design that is based on voting in referendums. This design presents important advantages. First, it enables stronger causal inference to address several methodological challenges in the available research on losing that heavily relies on cross-sectional election studies data (e.g., self-selection, endogeneity, reciprocal causation, spurious relationships). Second, it allows us to extend the research on losing in political decision-making beyond elections. Our analysis leverages data from an original preregistered survey experiment conducted in Ireland (N = 2,146) in January 2019 in the run-up to a real-world referendum to be held in May 2019. In the experiment, we manipulated the number of losses in direct votes by randomly assigning participants to winners or losers of three referendums.

We find that with every additional loss, the acceptance of a political decision and the perceived legitimacy of the political decision-making process diminishes. Three accumulated losses depress the perceived legitimacy of the political system as a whole. Also, the effect of losing on perceptions of legitimacy is potentially mediated by perceptions of procedural fairness. Beyond its implications for the theory and empirics on losers’ consent, this study offers a blueprint for an experimental design that is able to capture the causal effects of the repetitive nature of winning and losing in politics and provides guidance for interpreting data on real-world elections and referendums.

Legitimacy perceptions and accumulated losses

Perceptions of legitimacy can be defined as ‘a psychological property of an authority, institution, or social arrangement that leads those connected to it to believe that it is appropriate, proper, and just’ (Tyler, Reference Tyler2006: 375). In addition to their normative value, these perceptions allow political leaders and political systems to implement authoritative rules for the regulation of society (Van Ham et al., Reference Van Ham, Thomassen, Aarts and Andeweg2017). The lower level of perceptions of legitimacy among losers compared to winners is well documented in the electoral context (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Ola2005; Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Klemmensen and Serritzlew2019). The evidence beyond the electoral context is scarcer. Empirical studies on the effect of participatory decision-making procedures on perceptions of legitimacy often rely on aggregate data or do not consider the effect of outcome favorability. However, a handful of recent studies have revealed substantial winner–loser gaps in perceptions of legitimacy following referendums and deliberative processes (Marien and Kern Reference Marien and Kern2018; Arnesen et al., Reference Arnesen, Broderstad, Johannesson and Linde2019; Esaiasson et al., Reference Esaiasson, Persson, Gilljam and Lindholm2019; Brummel Reference Brummel2020; Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Himmelroos and Setälä2020). In line with these studies, we argue that also in the context of a referendum, perceptions of legitimacy of losers are lower than legitimacy perceptions of winners. This leads to our first hypothesis.

HYPOTHESIS 1. (One-time) Losing decreases individuals’ legitimacy perceptions.

But what happens when losses accumulate? Empirical insights from electoral studies on accumulated losses show that those who lose twice are less satisfied with democratic performance than those who lose only one of the two elections under study (Curini et al., Reference Curini, Jou and Memoli2012; Chang et al., Reference Chang, Chu, Wu and Thomassen2014; Hansen et al., Reference Hansen, Klemmensen and Serritzlew2019). The underlying mechanisms remain unclear.Footnote 3 We argue that the effect of accumulated losses on perceived legitimacy is different from the effect of a single loss or several nonconsecutive losses. When citizens lose in an accumulated way (i.e., repeatedly and consecutively), something more is happening.

Losing in general results in strong affective and biological reactions. The intensity of these reactions may vary depending on the type of losing and the stakes involved, but the psychological underpinning of losing is the same: losing hurts. This has been shown in the context of sports (Wilson and Kerr, Reference Wilson and Kerr1999) as well as in the context of elections (Pierce et al., Reference Pierce, Rogers and Snyder2016; Toshkov and Mazepus, Reference Toshkov and Mazepus2023). Pierce and colleagues (Reference Pierce, Rogers and Snyder2016), for instance, show that losing strongly affected the emotions of partisans after the 2012 USA. Presidential Election. Losing is detrimental to individuals’ self-esteem and self-worth and can have physiological consequences such as changes in blood pressure, compromised decision-making, increased stress levels, and a reduced level of testosterone (Wilson and Kerr, Reference Wilson and Kerr1999; Robertson, Reference Robertson2013). In retaliation, individuals not only try to navigate these strong and negative emotions, but they also try to regain their self-image. They seek cognitive consistency to cope with loss (see Festinger, Reference Festinger1957 on dissonance theory). While one-time loss can elicit these reactions, when people lose more than once, the feelings of frustration can have greater negative downstream effects on their perceptions of the fairness of the process. Losing once or twice in a row could just be bad luck, but when losses accumulate, the feelings of frustration may intensify and people may start losing faith that they have a fair chance of winning the next time. This is in line with Anderson and colleagues’ (Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Ola2005) argument, that accumulated losses lead to an erosion of legitimacy perceptions because of the greater frustration that arises among losers with every loss.

To cope with accumulated losses, citizens will have a stronger impetus to rationalize their repeated losses and put forward post hoc arguments that attribute responsibility for the losses to the external procedures that led to these losses. In doing so, individuals frame the loss to limit the damage caused to their own self-esteem and dignity. A similar argument has been made in the context of sporting competitions (Hastorf and Cantril, Reference Hastorf and Cantril1954), conspiratorial thinking (Miller et al., Reference Miller, Farhart and Saunders2021), and elections and electoral integrity (Daniller and Mutz, Reference Daniller and Mutz2019). While one-time losers may already evaluate procedural characteristics more negatively (as compared to winners) (Craig et al., Reference Craig, Martinez, Gainous and Kane2006; Esaiasson et al., Reference Esaiasson, Persson, Gilljam and Lindholm2019; Van der Eijk and Rose, Reference Van der Eijk and Rose2021), we expect this effect to be much stronger when losses accumulate and the need to find face-saving explanations becomes much more pressing compared to experiencing a single or occasional loss. Hence, we argue that accumulated losses would enhance the negative effect of losing on individuals´ legitimacy perceptions. This reasoning leads to the second hypothesis.

HYPOTHESIS 2. The effect of accumulated losses on legitimacy perceptions is greater than the effect of one-time or two-times losing.Footnote 4

The context: losing in referendums

While most of the literature on which we built has focused on losing in elections, we study this argument in the context of losing in a (hypothetical) referendum. Notably, in referendums, the effect of losing legitimacy perceptions is more likely to depend on how one is affected by a decision than in elections. When the salience of the issue is high the effect of losing might be stronger as compared to the effect of losing in elections. The issue is likely to be addressed after the decision has been taken and is unlikely (with some exceptions, i.e., the issue of abortion in Ireland) to re-appear on the agenda soon. Hence the consequences of the decision may transcend the electoral cycle (for a similar argument see Van der Eijk and Rose, Reference Van der Eijk and Rose2021), meaning that losers remain losers for an indefinite period. Electoral losers, however, get the chance to become winners in the following election. On the other hand, when losers in a referendum vote on an issue that is not very salient to them, losing in elections might have a stronger and/or longer-lasting impact. Despite these differences, we hypothesize that when losses accumulate (i.e., individuals lose repeatedly and consecutively), this experience spurs in both types of contexts frustration as well as doubts about a fair chance of winning the next time which in turn affects individuals’ perceptions of the legitimacy of the political system more generally.

Research design

To examine the main attitudinal predictions and primary assumptions of our theory we employ an experimental design that is based on voting in referendums because it allows us to combine a strong test for causal inference with ecological validity. We designed and preregistered a between- and within-subject online survey experiment (n = 2,146) (Druckman et al., Reference Druckman, Green, Kuklinski and Lupia2011).Footnote 5 In the embedded vignette, respondents were invited to participate in hypothetical referendums on three different policy issues resulting in eight experimental conditions of winning-losing combinations (see Fig. 1; complete guide in online Appendix A).

Figure 1. Example referendum ballot paper used in the vignette.

There are several reasons why we expect to find similar effects of losing in both hypothetical and real-world referendums. First, although people do not lose anything in mock referendums, they are reminded about real-world referendums, and this experimental design closely resembles the voting at real-world referendums in Ireland (see the subsequent section on the experimental design). Second, previous research shows that concern about the potentially different effects of hypothetical as opposed to real-world scenarios may be misplaced; people’s reactions to hypothetical scenarios do not differ much from their reactions to real-world scenarios (see for example, Berinsky, Reference Berinsky2009: 124; Tomz and Weeks, Reference Tomz and Weeks2013). Third, we tested a possible emotional reaction to winning or losing after the three majority outcomes were announced and found that frustration accumulated with each loss, whereas enthusiasm increased with each win. The magnitude of frustration was larger than the magnitude of positive emotions, which is consistent with previous literature on losing emotions (e.g., Pierce et al., Reference Pierce, Rogers and Snyder2016).

The country case and the sample

The experiment was fielded in Ireland between January and February 2019. Respondents were recruited by Dynata using a quota-based sampling procedure to ensure demographic similarity of the sample to the Irish census. Respondents ranged in age from 18 to 81, with a mean age of 46.7. Fifty-two percent of respondents self-identified as female, and 58% reported having finished the third level of education (see also online Appendix B).Footnote 6

Fielding this study in Ireland increases the ecological validity of the experiment. First, the Irish electorate is well-acquainted with referendums, which increases the credibility of the experimental protocol. Since 1947, as many as 40 referendums have been held in Ireland, and there has been a recent surge in important socio-political issues being decided by referendums (e.g., abortion law, marriage equality).Footnote 7 Second, the policy issues we used for the experiment had already been approved for future referendums. The experiment was fielded in the run-up to a referendum (to be held in May 2019) on the issue of changing the Irish constitution to reduce the waiting time for initiating divorce proceedings. The two other issues (lowering the voting age and removing the ‘women’s life within the home’ article from the constitution) were scheduled for the same referendum, although these were eventually postponed. The amendment to the constitution was aimed at liberalizing the divorce law in Ireland. 82% of the voters in the referendum approved the change to shorten the required minimum separation period before divorce proceedings can be initiated (Horgan-Jonse, Reference Horgan-Jonse2019). A poll conducted in December 2017 revealed that only 16% of respondents approved of reducing the voting age from 18 to 16 in Ireland (https://www.thejournal.ie/poll-lowering-voting-age−3737603-Dec2017/). The proposal on women’s role in the household was to remove an article from the Irish constitution that referred to a woman’s place being in the home. The referendum was eventually postponed because of a lack of consensus on the exact type of amendment that should be made to the Constitution. The issues were policy matters that had been put forward for discussion by the Irish Citizens’ Assembly on Gender Equality. All three policy issues have high public importance and attract extensive media coverage. As they require constitutional amendments, they belong to the broader issue category of constitutional issues and institutional reforms, a category that has commonly occurred in the context of referendums in Western Europe in the past 30 years (Qvortrup, Reference Qvortrup and Smith2021).Footnote 8

Experimental procedure

The experiment proceeded as follows. First, respondents were asked questions on sociodemographic variables, political trust, satisfaction with democracy, political ideology, and partisanship. Next, we explained the rules of the hypothetical referendum. Respondents were told they would be asked to cast a vote for three different policy proposals and each time they could either accept or reject the proposal. They were informed that several thousand citizens were participating in this referendum study and that they would find out after each vote they cast whether their vote choice was shared by the majority of the citizens taking part. After receiving their instructions, participants proceeded to make their three vote choices. Each time they read a short vignette with general information about the policy issue, cast a vote and received information on the outcome (see Table A.1, online Appendix A).Footnote 9

In each referendum, the majority vote either agreed with or disagreed with the vote choice of the respondents. Hence, there were eight possible combinations of the winning–losing treatment: winning three times (WWW) (n = 283), winning–winning–losing (WWL) (n = 254), winning–losing–winning (WLW) (n = 265), winning–losing–losing (WLL) (n = 271), losing–winning–winning (LWW) (n = 280), losing–winning–losing (LWL) (n = 249), losing–losing–winning (LLW) (n = 288) or losing three times (LLL) (n = 256).Footnote 10 Respondents were randomly assigned to one of these eight experimental conditions. The design did not allow us to randomize the order of the three policy issues; however, we measured the salience of every issue and ran robustness analyses to check the possible effect of issue salience (see online Appendix D.2). The vignette was designed to match the ballot paper commonly encountered in Irish referendums (see Fig. 1).

After the voting procedure, respondents were asked for their opinions and feelings about the results. This allowed us to measure to what degree respondents were (emotionally) affected by the wins and/or losses in the mock referendums, that is, how much they were roused by the treatment. This offers an indication of the ecological validity of the experimental setup and gives an insight into the underlying mechanisms. Next, the outcome variables were measured. Further, we embedded a manipulation check into the survey to test how well the treatment of ‘losing’ was taken (Kane and Barabas, Reference Kane and Barabas2019), and respondents were asked to indicate how many of the majority votes were consistent with their own preferences with regard to the policy issues.Footnote 11 Finally, respondents were debriefed about the objectives of the study, and we emphasized again that the votes and results were fictitious.

Ireland has held different referendums on the same day but we should note that in other contexts there might be more time between wins and losses compared to this experiment, which might lower the effect.

Measures

We measure four commonly used proxies of perceptions of legitimacy that tap into the perceived legitimacy of the decision, the decision-making process, and the political system (e.g., Esaiasson et al., Reference Esaiasson, Persson, Gilljam and Lindholm2019). The first proxy, decision acceptance, is directly related to the decision at hand and reads: ‘How willing are you to accept and comply with this decision?’ 1 = ‘not willing at all’ to 7 = ‘very willing’. The question was asked each time respondents cast their vote and learned about the (fictitious) majority outcome because decision acceptance is contingent upon the policy decision. The second perceived legitimacy proxy taps into the perceived legitimacy of the decision-making procedure (satisfaction with the use of referendums for arriving at a decision; 1 = ‘not satisfied at all’ to 7 = ‘very satisfied’) (M = 5.26; SD = 1.52) and was asked after the respondent knew the outcome of the three votes. The third and fourth proxies tap into more general opinions of the political system. At the start of the survey, we measured satisfaction with the functioning of democracy in Ireland (SWD, 0 = ‘extremely dissatisfied’, 10 = ‘extremely satisfied’; M = 5.02; SD = 2.64) and political trust.Footnote 12 To measure political trust we use a rescaled sum index measure that is based on four items, namely trust in Dáil Éireann (i.e., the Irish House of Representatives), trust in the government, trust in politicians, and trust in political parties. These four items form a strong one-dimensional scale (α = 0.95) and the index variable ranges from 0 to 40 (M = 16; SD = 9.4). After the outcomes of the three votes were announced, we measured satisfaction with democracy (M = 5.50; SD = 2.64) and political trust (M = 16.01; SD = 9.82) again.Footnote 13 These latter two proxies relate to the perceived legitimacy of the political system and are not directly connected with the specific decisions or decision-making procedure and, thereby, are the least likely to be affected by winning or losing. Figure 2 summarizes the measurement of the different proxies for perceptions of legitimacy.

Figure 2. Flow of the survey.

Bivariate Pearson correlational analyses show that these outcome variables relate to each other positively and significantly. The strongest association is observed between (post-treatment) political trust and (post-treatment) satisfaction with democracy (r = 0.74), followed by a relationship between the perceived legitimacy of the decision-making procedure and satisfaction with democracy (r = 0.34). The weakest relationship is observed between third-time decision acceptance and (post-treatment) political trust (r = 0.11) (see online Appendix C). This shows that the legitimacy indicators are related and are not in opposition to each other. They capture the same construct, although from different angles. Rather than losing variation, by creating an index, we will examine the effect of losing on each of these proxies separately.

To tap into perceptions of procedural fairness, we relied on respondents scoring the item ‘The process was fair’ on a seven-point scale (1 = ‘completely disagree’ to 7 = ‘completely agree’; M = 5.47; SD = 1.30). Post-treatment respondents were also inquired about their experienced feelings in the form of a battery of emotions. Four of the listed emotions capture frustration (anger, outrage, frustration and alarm; α = 0.90). We created a sum index scale and rescaled it to 0–1.

Identification and empirical strategy

We estimate the effect of losing on perceptions of legitimacy using difference-in-means t-tests and simple linear regression analyses. We start by examining the perceptions of legitimacy of first-time losers and first-time winners (HYPOTHESIS 1).

Subsequently, we investigate whether the number of losses affects four different indicators of legitimacy perceptions (HYPOTHESIS 2). We do so in different steps. In the first step, we compare the decision acceptance of those groups who received an unfavorable outcome in the third and last round of voting. In doing so, we can take the number of previous losses into account. In the second step, we repeat this analysis for the perceived legitimacy of the decision-making procedure, which was measured after the three decision outcomes were known. In the third step, we turn to the indicators that capture the legitimacy of the political system as a whole (i.e., satisfaction with democracy and political trust). We expect that citizens who lose three times in a row are not only less inclined to accept the decision itself and more inclined to start questioning the legitimacy of the decision-making procedure, but they will also start doubting the legitimacy of the political system more generally. To test this, we plot regression coefficients (together with their 95% CIs) from linear regression analyses estimating the effect of the number of losses on changes in satisfaction with democracy and on changes in political trust. For ease of interpretation, we combine this analysis of the experimental conditions into four different groups: losing once (WWL, WLW, LWW); losing twice (WLL, LLW, LWL); and losing three times (LLL) and compare them to those who never lost (WWW, reference category). We furthermore also examine whether accumulated losses elicit increased feelings of frustration in individuals. To do so, we employ post-treatment measures on respondents’ experienced feelings of frustration (anger, outrage, frustration, and alarm; α = 0.90).

Results

To test HYPOTHESIS 1 we conduct difference-in-means t-tests. These tests show that the decision acceptance of first-time losers is significantly lower than the decision acceptance of first-time winners (both groups n = 1,073). Losers score 1.79 points lower on a 1–7 point scale for decision acceptance compared to winners (95% CI: [1.66 to 1.92]) (equivalent to more than 1SD in standardized effect), confirming previous studies and providing support for the first hypothesis.

Does this winner–loser gap increase when losses accumulate? To answer this question, we proceed to investigate the effect of the number of losses on different perceptions of legitimacy (HYPOTHESIS 2). Figure 3 presents a comparison between the mean decision acceptance with 95% CIs of the three groups that experienced different numbers of losses (i.e., no previous loss, one previous loss, two previous losses; difference-in-means t-tests). We find that the number of previous losses exerts a negative, and substantively and statistically significant effect on an individual’s decision acceptance, with the effect of the accumulation of losses being twice as big as that of a single loss (i.e., no previous loss vs. two previous losses).

Figure 3. Decision acceptance of a loss taking previous losses into account.

Note: Only respondents who have lost in the third vote are depicted. Nonstandardized mean decision acceptance with 95% CIs.

Figure 4 shows the perceived legitimacy of the decision-making procedure according to the number of losses participants experienced. It clearly shows that with each additional loss, satisfaction with the decision-making procedure diminishes. Similar to decision acceptance, the drop is steady and accumulates with each loss.

Figure 4. Perceived legitimacy of the decision-making procedure taking number of losses into account.

Note: All respondents are depicted by the number of losses. Nonstandardized mean decision acceptance with 95% CIs.

We turn to the effect of losing the legitimacy perceptions of the political system more generally. Figure 5 presents the regression coefficients (together with their 95% CIs) from linear regression analyses estimating the effect of the number of losses on change in satisfaction with democracy (Fig. 5, Model 1) and on change in political trust (Fig. 5, Model 2). We combine the experimental conditions based on how many times participants lost and differentiate between those who lost once (WWL, WLW, LWW); those who lost twice (WLL, LLW, LWL); and those who lost three times (LLL) (no loss represents the reference category). As Fig. 5 shows, losing three times in a row (M swd = 0.21, SD swd = 1.44; M poltrust = −0.41, SDpoltrust = 3.27) has a significant negative effect on the change in satisfaction with democracy by β = −0.26 (CI: [−0.5 to −0.02]), and the change in political trust by β = −0.70 (CI: [−1.24 to −0.15]). There is, however, no significant difference between those who did not lose any of the three votes (Mswd = 0.47, SDswd = 1.36; Mpoltrust = 0.26; SDpoltrust = 3.15), those who lost once (Mswd = 0.61, SDswd = 1.55; Mpoltrust = 0.13, SDpoltrust = 3.54) and those who lost twice (Mswd = 0.42, SDswd = 1.41; Mpoltrust =  −0.06, SDpoltrust = 3.24).Footnote 14 In other words, it is only with the third loss that the legitimacy of the system as a whole starts to crumble. One explanation for this finding is that possibly it really takes three losses in a row in order to weaken legitimacy perceptions, another explanation however might lie in the fact that this is the only group that lost exclusively. In contrast, the group that lost twice did experience one win, which might reinforce their belief that they have a fair chance to win the next time.

Figure 5. The effect of accumulated losses on perceived legitimacy of the political system.

Note: SWD: satisfaction with democracy; PolTrust: political trust.

Causal mechanisms (exploratory analyses)

Our design does not allow us to study the causal mechanisms. Yet, we proceed to present descriptive and indirect evidence for the association between frustration, procedural fairness perceptions, and legitimacy perceptions. Table 1 presents the mean frustration associated with different amounts of losses: Losing three times increases frustration four-fold. The results of difference-in-means t-tests show that these changes are statistically significant at a two-tailed P  < 0.00 level. In other words, accumulated losses elicit much stronger negative feelings in individuals than occasional losses.

Table 1. Frustration associated with losing

But why do accumulated losses diminish legitimacy perceptions and how does the frustration connect to legitimacy perceptions? In an exploratory analysis that was not preregistered, we attempted to answer this question in two steps. First, we present the average perceived fairness of the four groups of participants that experienced different amounts of losses (i.e., no loss, losing once, losing twice, losing three times) (Table 2). Public perceptions of the fairness of the process change for the worse with every additional loss. People question the fairness of the process after the first loss, and after each loss, the negative effect accumulates.

Table 2. Perceived fairness of the decision-making process taking the number of losses into account

Second, we examine whether frustration is related to perceptions about the fairness of the process. Table 3 reports the result of simple OLS regression analyses in which we regress the levels of frustration on fairness perceptions (Model 1) and subsequently, the number of losses on four different proxies for legitimacy perceptions while controlling for fairness perceptions (Models 2–5).

Table 3. Predicting fairness perceptions and (changes in) legitimacy

Note: Entries are coefficients estimated with OLS regression models. Standard errors are in parentheses.*P < 0.05, **P < 0.01, ***P < 0.001.

In Model 2 we take the third decision acceptance as an outcome variable, controlling for the first and second decision acceptance. As the first column shows, frustration and fairness perceptions are negatively associated. The more frustrated the individuals are, the lower their beliefs about the fairness of the process. Procedural fairness perceptions and legitimacy perceptions, on the other hand, are positively associated.

These exploratory findings should be replicated with different experimental designs, e.g., ‘parallel designs’, suggested by Imai and Yamamoto (Reference Imai and Yamamoto2013). Future designs could also incorporate the measures capturing other theoretically important pathways, such as differences in utility and cognitive consistency. Research disentangling these pathways holds important implications for shedding light on the scope conditions for the research on losers’ consent.

Robustness checks

We conducted additional analyses to evaluate the robustness of our findings and present further insights into individuals’ reactions to a series of (un)favorable political decisions. The main results stay robust. See the online Appendix D for minor exceptions.

Discussion and conclusion

Democracies struggle to keep people on board who disagree with political decisions. The temporary nature of the loser status features central in the seminal works on losers’ consent (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Ola2005; Przeworski, Reference Przeworski2015). Yet, to date, the consequences of accumulated losses remain vastly undertheorized and unexplored. For example, Citrin and Stoker (Reference Citrin and Stoker2018: 59) wrote: ‘The hypothesis is that when the government treats people with respect and gives them a fair hearing, citizens are willing to accept painful outcomes and still retain faith in institutions. […] Whether this positive effect of procedural justice would endure if individuals found themselves consistently on the losing side of official decisions or policy outcomes is unclear.’ This study provides evidence to fill the identified gap.

We theorized that to cope with accumulated losses, individuals have a strong urge to externalize responsibility for these. As a result, they will start doubting the fairness of the decision-making process, which then undermines perceptions of legitimacy. The experimental results provide evidence that losing erodes perceptions of legitimacy and that this effect may be mediated by perceptions of procedural fairness. In contrast to our argument, the between-respondent comparison also shows that individuals’ perceptions of the fairness of the process are already lower after the first loss and decline steadily with each additional loss. Moreover, the effect of losing three times is not exponentially larger than losing once or twice. This suggests that the painful experience of losing creates a need to externalize responsibility, even when a single loss is experienced.

Simultaneously, the results reveal that although unaffected by one or two losses, after three losses in a row, individuals’ support for the political system starts to crumble. Moreover, a win in between two losses is less harmful than two losses in a row. This is very much in line with Easton’s reasoning (Reference Easton1975: 446), which states that diffuse support should not be affected by specific political decisions but could be affected by accumulated experience. As such, accumulated losses have fundamentally different consequences compared with occasional losses. This study offers empirical evidence that, quite rightly, the temporary and transformable status of a loser is a critical element in the definitions of democracy (Przeworski, Reference Przeworski2015). Likewise, in nonpolitical contexts, we can also observe similar reasoning. To comfort someone dealing with one or two losses, several proverbs point to its temporary nature: ‘lightning doesn’t strike twice,’ the ‘third time’s the charm’ or ‘third time lucky’.

This study does not come without limitations. We take a first step in theorizing and testing the general effect of accumulated losses on perceptions of legitimacy, and we draw on the context of referendums because of their methodological advantages. However, the order of the referendum issues was not randomized. Future studies could randomize the order of the issues to ensure that the content of a specific issue does not affect respondents’ reactions. Also, it remains to be studied whether similar patterns appear in different contexts of political decision-making. Even with regard to referendums, there are many more variations worth exploring (e.g., bottom-up/top-down process, level of politicization, technicality and salience of issue, margin of loss, etc.). As far as a highly salient issue such as Brexit is concerned, one loss might already have diminished perceptions of legitimacy for some people (Van der Eijk and Rose, Reference Van der Eijk and Rose2021). This study did not include such an extremely salient issue. The study also lacks the statistical power to investigate the heterogeneity of the effects. Future studies could look into how factors such as issue salience, process characteristics, party identities or personality traits affect individuals’ reactions to accumulated losses (see e.g., Arnesen et al., Reference Arnesen, Broderstad, Johannesson and Linde2019; Clayton et al., Reference Clayton, Davis, Nyhan, Porter, Ryan and Wood2021).

Furthermore, as there are no observational data available to test the causal argument we developed adequately, conducting an experiment appeared a suitable alternative because it enabled the randomization of the losing/winning effect from nonrandom political characteristics, and also the study of within- and between-subject effects. We aimed to maximize ecological validity, and we observed clear engagement with the treatment by participants; nevertheless, the setting remains hypothetical, respondents do not really win or lose anything and the wins and losses occur consecutively in a short period of time. As no real decision is being made, it might be possible that our study underestimates the effect of winning and losing in real life. On the other hand, however, in this study, wins and losses followed quickly after each other and it is possible that the effects of winning and losing are weaker or fade away entirely when wins/losses are further apart.

Finally, this study is limited to a single-country context. The Irish context allowed us to increase the experiment’s realism, but in order to draw conclusions on the generalizability of our findings, replications in other contexts are needed. That being said, we speculate that citizens might have similar experiences in countries in which elections on different levels are held on the same day, such as Belgium (Bol et al., Reference Bol, Blais, Gillard, Nunez Lopez and Pilet2018) or Britain (Heath et al., Reference Heath, McLean, Taylor and Curtice1999) or when elections are combined with direct democratic decision-making procedures on the same day, as it can occur in certain Swiss cantons (Freitag and Stadelmann-Steffen, Reference Freitag and Stadelmann-Steffen2010) and in some states in the USA.

Apart from replicating this study in different contexts, future research could also investigate whether a similar mechanism affects legitimacy perceptions of ethnic minorities and/or other structurally excluded groups.

Beyond its contribution to our understanding of the effects of accumulated losses, this study contributes to the broader debates about political decision-making, psychological reactions to losing, and citizen involvement in politics. These findings carry implications for our understanding of how voters perceive and react to the democratic decision-making process. In effect, in the past years, concerns about ‘sore losers’ and the prospects of democratic procedures to gather consent among political losers heightened considerably following former president Trump’s discourse (I will accept election results ‘if I win’) and the January 6 insurrection. There is a clear narrative among scholars and pundits that interprets the winner–loser gap in perceptions of legitimacy in terms of ‘sore losers.’Footnote 15 Emerging empirical evidence problematizes optimistic ideas on the potential of democratic (participatory) procedures to mitigate the negative effect of losing (e.g., Esaiasson et al., Reference Esaiasson, Persson, Gilljam and Lindholm2019; Christensen et al., Reference Christensen, Himmelroos and Setälä2020). At the same time, procedures that are more participatory can increase both winners’ and losers’ perceptions of legitimacy and decrease the democratic satisfaction gap between electoral winners and losers (Werner and Marien, Reference Werner and Marien2020; Leemann and Stadelmann-Steffen, Reference Leemann and Stadelmann-Steffen2021). Recent longitudinal studies also reveal that an increase in winners’ perceptions of legitimacy plays a much bigger role in the creation of winner–loser gaps than is often assumed, questioning this ‘sore loser’ narrative (Van der Meer and Steenvoorden, Reference Van der Meer and Steenvoorden2018; Daniller and Mutz, Reference Daniller and Mutz2019; but see Brummel, Reference Brummel2020).

Our study adds to this ongoing debate. Based on the findings of this study, we argue that when participatory processes such as referendums are used in political decision-making, it is beneficial to use them occasionally so losers have the opportunity to become winners. At the same time, our findings suggest that referendums, or other democratic decision-making processes that deliver unfavorable outcomes, consecutively start losing their potential to mitigate the negative effects of losing and become part of the perceived problem. As Guinier argues (Reference Guinier1995: 1): ‘Yet, sometimes, even when rules are perfectly fair in form, they serve in practice to exclude particular groups from meaningful participation. When they do not encourage everyone to play, or when, over the long haul, they do not make the losers feel as good about the outcome as the winners, they can seem as unfair.’ The narrative on sore losers rarely questions the fairness of the outcomes for different groups in society or the structural inequalities present in contemporary societies. Of course, we cannot exclude the possibility that people experience accumulated losses. Hence, it is critical to understand that a fair process might not be sufficient to console individuals if they lose repeatedly. These findings urge us to think more about how to minimize accumulated losses and/or look for ways that will enable people to cope with accumulated losses other than a loss of faith in the fairness and legitimacy of the system, for example by strengthening election monitoring and/or by media reporting on how electoral integrity is ensured.

We expect that questions surrounding losers’ consent will become increasingly important in the current context of strong affective polarization, decreasing duty-based citizenship norms and concerns about procedural fairness and democratic legitimacy (Van Ham et al., Reference Van Ham, Thomassen, Aarts and Andeweg2017; Westwood et al., Reference Westwood, Iyengar, Walgrave, Leonisio, Miller and Strijbis2018). This study offered a first step toward a better understanding of how individuals react to accumulated losses and the conditions of losers’ consent.

Supplementary material

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S175577392300036X.

Acknowledgments

This project has received funding from the European Research Council under the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program (grant agreement n° 759736) as well as Research Foundation – Flanders (Belgium) (FWO, file n° 40370). Lala Muradova also wishes to thank the Research Foundation – Flanders (Belgium) (FWO, Grant agreement n° 759736). We wish to thank André Bächtiger, Peter Essaiasson, David Farrell, Ross James Gildea, Peter Miller, Jane Suiter, the participants of the 2019 ECPR General Conference (Wroclaw), the KU Leuven Democratic Innovations and Legitimacy research group seminar and the UGent CLP-GASPAR seminar for their valuable input on the experimental design and the manuscript.

Footnotes

All authors contributed equally.

1 For reactions to one-time winning and losing, see for example, Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Blais, Bowler, Donovan and Ola2005), Craig et al. (Reference Craig, Martinez, Gainous and Kane2006), Dahlberg and Linde (Reference Dahlberg and Linde2017), Esaiasson Reference Esaiasson2011, Esaiasson et al. (Reference Esaiasson, Persson, Gilljam and Lindholm2019), Plescia (Reference Plescia2019) and Singh et al. (Reference Singh, Karakoç and Blais2012).

2 The only other study to our knowledge is Morrell (Reference Morrell1999), in which American college students discussed and voted on three issues over the course of four weeks. Interestingly, students’ commitment to and satisfaction with the outcome was not affected by the favorability of the outcome. Given that the theoretical focus of this study was not accumulated losses, and losses were not randomly assigned nor were different groups of losers compared with each other (e.g., occasional to consecutive losers), the question of the causal effect of accumulated losses on perceived legitimacy remains unanswered in this study.

3 There is also a theoretical expectation that structural exclusion from power diminishes perceptions of legitimacy among social and political minorities (Lijphart, Reference Lijphart1999). While the results from this field can give some insights into the effects of accumulated losses, many additional factors are likely to affect perceptions of legitimacy among minority and disadvantaged groups beyond the repeated confrontation with political decisions that do not align with their interests (Pérez, Reference Pérez2015; Koch, Reference Koch2019). In this study, we are not aiming to capture the experience or the effects of systematic exclusion from power, but rather to gain insights into the experience of accumulated losses in democratic decision-making and how this affects perceptions of legitimacy.

4 The hypotheses were worded differently in the pre-registration plan. The exact formulation of the hypotheses was as follows. H1: Legitimacy beliefs of winners are stronger than legitimacy beliefs of losers after direct democratic decision-making. H2: Legitimacy beliefs of occasional losers are stronger than legitimacy beliefs of repeated losers after several rounds of direct democratic decision-making. Furthermore, we preregistered five hypotheses (the rest being: Legitimacy beliefs of consecutive losers are stronger than legitimacy beliefs of discontinuous losers after several rounds of direct democratic decision-making; The gap between winners and losers is asymmetric, as losing hurts more than winning eases; and The effect of all types of losing is moderated by citizens´ degree of news consumption, as the more aware they are of current political issues, the less credible they may find losing in the voting study). Developing and testing all five in the scope of one paper turned out to be unfeasible; therefore, in this paper, we decided to focus on the first two hypotheses and leave the remaining for the online Appendix E.

5 Replication materials are available at: https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/N6SASN.

6 Our sample is hence slightly older, slightly more female, and more educated than the Irish population. Most recent census data from 2016 revealed that the average age of the population was 37 years, that about 50% is female and that 42% of the population attained third level education (Central Statistics Office Ireland, 2017).

7 According to Irish law, constitutional amendments must be decided by direct public voting. Although a referendum can also be convened for matters unrelated to amending the Irish constitution (so-called ordinary referendums), such a referendum has never been convened up to now. Besides, while salient issues have gained more prominence and exposure in the international media (for example the referendum on abortion), Ireland has also convened referendums on less controversial issues such as parliamentary inquiries and amendment of the bail restrictions (Henley et al., Reference Henley, Carroll and Rice-Oxley2019; Elkink et al., Reference Elkink, Farrell, Marien, Reidy and Suiter2020).

8 It is possible that voters’ reaction to losing is influenced by their perception of the distribution of policy preferences on the respective issues in the electorate. To account for this possibility, we have rerun our main analyses among the subgroup of people for whom the majority preferences presented as part of the study clashed with the real majority preferences around the time of holding the survey for the issue on divorce and voting age (see online Appendix D.4) Unfortunately, we could not find polling data for the “women´s place in the home” issue for this time period. We find that the results remain robust and in fact almost exactly the same.

9 This part was manipulated and, thus, fictional; the assignment of respondents to either the winner or loser groups in relation to the policy decision was completely random and independent of their vote choices. We debriefed the subjects about this at the end of the survey, informing them of the experimental design (see online Appendix A). This study was approved by the Social and Societal Ethics Committee of KU Leuven.

10 Public opinion on each policy issue could influence citizens’ reactions toward winning or losing. If citizens were aware of public opinion on policy issues and the majority vote announced in our study disagreed with public opinion, the treatment may have confused respondents and, thus, confounded the effect. To potentially account for this, we collected qualitative feedback from respondents (responses to open-ended questions asking what they thought about the study) after having measured, post-treatment, the variables of interest. A systematic analysis of this qualitative data revealed that the study was perceived as credible, and no discernible pattern with regard to confusion was identified. Also, we explicitly mentioned in the debriefing that the outcomes of the vote were entirely fictitious, and not a reflection of Irish citizens’ actual thinking on these three topics.

11 For the purposes of robustness, we ran a complier average causal effect (CACE) analysis (see online Appendix D.1), taking compliance with the treatment as an endogenous variable and assignment to the treatment as an instrumental variable. The results remain robust and strong.

12 The debate on the true meaning of the concept satisfaction with democracy is still ongoing (see Canache et al., Reference Canache, Mondak and Seligson2001; Linde and Ekman, Reference Linde and Ekman2003); nevertheless, the concept is widely considered to be an important evaluative attitude toward the functioning of political systems and it is a dominant measure in electoral studies of the winner–loser gap. Together with political trust, these two indicators will give a good indication of whether winning or losing can affect attitudes toward the political system.

13 We measured decision acceptance after each vote. To avoid revealing the purpose of the experiment or overburdening the respondents with legitimacy questions, the other proxies were not assessed after each vote. Perceptions of legitimacy regarding the decision-making procedure were assessed after the outcomes of all three votes were announced. Satisfaction with democracy and political trust were assessed pre- and post-treatment.

14 Another important observation here is that post-treatment, within-subject change in satisfaction with democracy is positive, although this change is a lot less among (repeated) losers. One possible interpretation for this finding is that participation in decision-making per se increases people’s satisfaction with democracy overall as it might remind them of the opportunities in Ireland to participate in referendums. However, our design does not allow us to test this conjecture empirically, because we do not manipulate citizens’ involvement in the referendum.

References

Anderson, Christopher J., Blais, André, Bowler, Shaun, Donovan, Todd, and Ola, Listhaug. Losers’ Consent: Elections and Democratic Legitimacy. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2005.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnesen, Sveinung, Broderstad, Troy S., Johannesson, Mikael P., and Linde, Jonas. “Conditional Legitimacy: How Turnout, Majority Size, and Outcome Affect Perceptions of Legitimacy in European Union Membership Referendums.” European Union Politics 20.2 (2019): 176–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berinsky, Adam J. In Time of War. Chicago: U Chicago P, 2009.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bol, Damien, Blais, André, Gillard, Xavier, Nunez Lopez, Lidia, and Pilet, Jean-Benoit. “Voting and Satisfaction with Democracy in Flexible-List PR.” Electoral Studies 56 (2018): 2334.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowler, Shaun, and Karp, Jeffrey A.. “Politicians, Scandals and Trust in Government.” Political Behavior 26.3 (2004): 271–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brummel, Lars. “‘You Can’t Always Get What You Want’: The Effects of Winning and Losing in a Referendum on Citizens’ Referendum Support.” Electoral Studies 65 (2020): 102155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.electstud.2020.102155 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Canache, Damarys, Mondak, Jeffery J., and Seligson, Mitchell A.. “Meaning and Measurement in Cross-National Research on Satisfaction with Democracy.” Public Opinion Quarterly 65.4 (2001): 506–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Central Statistics Office Ireland. “Census 2016 Reports.” 2017. <https://www.cso.ie/en/census/census2016reports/> (last accessed 7 July 2022).+(last+accessed+7+July+2022).>Google Scholar
Chang, Eric, Chu, Yun-hand, and Wu, Wen-chin. “Consenting to Lose or Expecting to Win? Inter-Temporal Changes in Voters’ Winner-Loser Status and Satisfaction with Democracy.” In Thomassen, Jacques (Ed.), Elections and Democracy: Representation and Accountability (pp. 232253). Oxford: Oxford UP, 2014.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chanley, Virginia A., Rudolph, Thomas J., and Rahn, Wendy M.. “The Origins and Consequences of Public Trust in Government: A Time Series Analysis.” Public Opinion Quarterly 64.3 (2000): 239–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christensen, Henrik S., Himmelroos, Staffan, and Setälä, Maija. “A Matter of Life or Death: A Survey Experiment on the Perceived Legitimacy of Political Decision-Making on Euthanasia.” Parliamentary Affairs 73.3 (2020): 627–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Citrin, Jack, and Stoker, Laura. “Political Trust in a Cynical Age.” Annual Review of Political Science 21 (2018): 4970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clayton, Katherine, Davis, Nicholas T., Nyhan, Brendan, Porter, Ethan, Ryan, Timothy J., and Wood, Thomas J.. “Elite Rhetoric Can Undermine Democratic Norms.” PNAS 118.23 (2021): 16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, Stephen C., Martinez, Michael D., Gainous, Jason, and Kane, James G.. “Winners, Losers, and Election Context: Voter Responses to the 2000 Presidential Election.” Political Research Quarterly 59.4 (2006): 579–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curini, Luigi, Jou, Willy, and Memoli, Vincenzo. “Satisfaction with Democracy and the Winner/Loser Debate: the Role of Policy Preferences and Past Experience.” British Journal of Political Science 42 (2012): 241–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dahlberg, Stefan, and Linde, Jonas. “The Dynamics of the Winner–Loser Gap in Satisfaction with Democracy: Evidence from a Swedish Citizen Panel.” International Political Science Review 38.5 (2017): 625–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daniller, Andrew M., and Mutz, Diana C.. “The Dynamics of Electoral Integrity: A Three-Election Panel Study.” Public Opinion Quarterly 83.1 (2019): 4667.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Druckman, James N., Green, Donald P., Kuklinski, James H., and Lupia, Arthur. (Eds.) Cambridge Handbook of Experimental Political Science. Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 2011.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Easton, David. “A Re-Assessment of the Concept of Political Support.” British Journal of Political Science 5.4 (1975): 435–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elkink, Johan A., Farrell, David M., Marien, Sofie, Reidy, Theresa, and Suiter, Jane. “The Death of Conservative Ireland? The 2018 Abortion Referendum.” Electoral Studies 65 (2020): in press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esaiasson, Peter. “Electoral Losers Revisited – How Citizens React to Defeat at the Ballot Box.” Electoral Studies 30 (2011): 102–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esaiasson, Peter, Persson, Mikael, Gilljam, Mikael, and Lindholm, Torun. “Reconsidering the Role of Procedures for Decision Acceptance.” British Journal of Political Science 49.1 (2019): 291314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Festinger, Leon. A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance. Stanford: Stanford UP, 1957.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freitag, Markus, and Stadelmann-Steffen, Isabelle. “Stumbling Block or Stepping Stone? The Influence of Direct Democracy on Individual Participation in Parliamentary Elections.” Electoral Studies 29.3 (2010): 472–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guinier, Lani. The Tyranny of the Majority: Fundamental Fairness in Representative Democracy. New York: Free Press, 1995.Google Scholar
Hansen, Sune W., Klemmensen, Robert, and Serritzlew, Søren. “Losers Lose More Than Winners Win: Asymmetrical Effects of Winning and Losing in Elections.” European Journal of Political Research 58 (2019): 1172–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hastorf, Albert H., and Cantril, Hadley. “They Saw a Game; a Case Study.” The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 49.1 (1954): 129–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heath, Anthony, McLean, Ian, Taylor, Bridget, and Curtice, John. “Between First and Second Order: A Comparison of Voting Behaviour in European and Local Elections in Britain.” European Journal of Political Research 35.3 (1999): 389414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henley, Jon, Carroll, Rory, and Rice-Oxley, Mark. “Referendums: Who Holds Them, Why, and Are They Always a Dog’s Brexit?” The Guardian, 2019. <https://www.theguardian.com/news/2019/mar/11/referendums-who-holds-them-why-and-are-they-always-a-dogs-brexit> (last accessed 7 July 2022).+(last+accessed+7+July+2022).>Google Scholar
Horgan-Jonse, Jack. “Divorce Referendum to Reduce Required Wait Period Passes by Huge Margin.” Irish Times, 2019. <https://www.irishtimes.com/news/politics/divorce-referendum/divorce-referendum-to-reduce-required-wait-period-passes-by-huge-margin−1.3904660> (last accessed 7 July 2022).+(last+accessed+7+July+2022).>Google Scholar
Imai, Kosuke, and Yamamoto, Teppei. “Identification and Sensitivity Analysis for Multiple Causal Mechanisms: Revisiting Evidence from Framing Experiments.” Political Analysis 21 (2013): 141–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kane, John. V., and Barabas, Jason. “Checks to Assess Attentiveness in Experiments.” American Journal of Political Science 63.1 (2019): 234–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kern, Anna, and Kölln, Ann-Kristin. “The Consequences of Repeatedly Losing on Legitimacy Beliefs.” European Journal of Political Research (2022). doi: 10.1111/1475-6765.12512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koch, Jeffrey W.Racial Minorities’ Trust in Government and Government Decisionmakers.” Social Science Quarterly 100 (2019): 1937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leemann, Lucas, and Stadelmann-Steffen, Isabelle. “Satisfaction with Democracy: When Government by the People Brings Electoral Losers and Winners Together.” Comparative Political Studies 55.1 (2021): 93121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lijphart, Arend. Patterns of Democracy. Government Forms and Performance in Thirty-Six Countries. New Haven, CT: Yale UP, 1999.Google Scholar
Linde, Jonas, and Ekman, Joakim. “Satisfaction with Democracy: A Note on a Frequently Used Indicator in Comparative Politics.” European Journal of Political Research 42.3 (2003): 391408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marien, Sofie, and Kern, Anna. “The Winner Takes It All: revisiting the Effect of Direct Democracy on Citizens’ Political Support.” Political Behavior 40 (2018): 857–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mauk, Marlene. “Electoral Integrity Matters: How Electoral Process Conditions the Relationship Between Political Losing and Political Trust.” Quality & Quantity 56 (2020): 1709–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mercer, Jonathan. “Prospect Theory and Political Science.” Annual Review of Political Science 8 (2005): 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, Joanne M., Farhart, Christina E., and Saunders, Kyle L.. “Losers’ Conspiracy: Elections and Conspiratorial Thinking.” Paper presented at the New York Area Political Psychology Meeting, May 8, 2021, 2021.Google Scholar
Morrell, Michael E.Citizens’ Evaluations of Participatory Democratic Procedures: Normative Theory Meets Empirical Science.” Political Research Quarterly 52.2 (1999): 293.Google Scholar
Nadeau, Richard, and Blais, André. “Accepting the Election Outcome: the Effect of Participation on Losers’ Consent.” British Journal of Political Science 23.4 (1993): 553–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez, Efrén O.Xenophobic Rhetoric and Its Political Effects on Immigrants and Their Co-Ethnics.” American Journal of Political Science 59.3 (2015): 549–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierce, Lamar, Rogers, Todd, and Snyder, Jason A.. “Losing Hurts: The Happiness Impact of Partisan Electoral Loss.” Journal of Experimental Political Science 3.1 (2016): 4459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plescia, Carolina. “On the Subjectivity of the Experience of Victory: who Are the Election Winners?Political Psychology 40 (2019): 797814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Przeworski, Adam. “Acquiring the Habit of Changing Governments through Elections.” Comparative Political Studies 48.1 (2015): 101–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Przeworski, Adam. Crises of Democracy. Cambridge: Cambridge UP, 2019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qvortrup, Matt. “The Rise of Referendums: Demystifying Direct Democracy.” Journal of Democracy 28.3 (2017): 141–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Qvortrup, Matt. “Two Hundred Years of Direct Democracy: The Referendum in Europe 1793–2018.” In Smith, Julie (Ed.), The Palgrave Handbook of European Referendums (pp. 137–61). Cham: Palgrave Macmillan, 2021.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robertson, Ian. The Winner Effect. London: Bloomsbury Publishing, 2013.Google Scholar
Singh, Shane, Karakoç, Ekrem, and Blais, André. “Differentiating Winners: How Elections Affect Satisfaction with Democracy.” Electoral Studies 31 (2012): 201–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spina, Nicolas. “Partisanship and Generalized Trust in Response to the 2016 U.S. Presidential Election.” Social Science Journal (2021): 113. doi: 10.1080/03623319.2021.18993.Google Scholar
Tomz, Michael, and Weeks, Jessica. “Public Opinion and Democratic Peace.” American Political Science Review 107.4 (2013): 849–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Toshkov, Dimiter, and Mazepus, Honorata. “Does the Election Winner–Loser Gap Extend to Subjective Health and Well-Being?Political Studies Review 21.4 (2023): 783800.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tyler, Tom R.Psychological Perspectives on Legitimacy and Legitimation.” Annual Review of Psychology 57.1 (2006): 375400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Eijk, Cees, and Rose, Jonathan. “Winner–Loser Effects in Contentious Constitutional Referenda: Perceptions of Procedural Fairness and the Brexit Referendum.” British Journal of Politics and International Relations 23.1 (2021): 104–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van der Meer, Tom W. G., and Steenvoorden, Eefje H.. “Going Back to the Well: A Panel Study into the Election Boost of Political Support Among Electoral Winners and Losers.” Electoral Studies 55 (2018): 4053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Ham, Carolien, Thomassen, Jacques, Aarts, Kees, and Andeweg, Rudy (Eds.) Myth and Reality of the Legitimacy Crisis: Explaining Trends and Cross-National Differences in Established Democracies. Oxford: Oxford UP, 2017.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vosoughi, Soroush, Deb, Roy, and Aral, Sinan. “The Spread of True and False News Online.” Science 359.6380 (2018): 1146–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ward, Dalston G., and Tavits, Margit. “How Partisan Affect Shapes Citizens’ Perception of the Political World.” Electoral Studies 60 (2019): 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, Hannah, and Marien, Sofie. “Process vs. Outcome? How to Evaluate the Effects of Participatory Processes on Legitimacy Perceptions.” British Journal of Political Science 52.1 (2020): 429–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westwood, Sean, J., Iyengar, Shanto, Walgrave, Stefaan, Leonisio, Rafaael, Miller, Luis, and Strijbis, Oliver. “The Tie That Divides: Cross-National Evidence on the Primacy of Partyism.” European Journal of Political Research 57 (2018): 333–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, George V., and Kerr, John H.. “Affective Responses to Success and Failure: A Study of Winning and Losing in Competitive Rugby.” Personality and Individual Differences 27.1 (1999): 8599.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Example referendum ballot paper used in the vignette.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Flow of the survey.

Figure 2

Figure 3. Decision acceptance of a loss taking previous losses into account.Note: Only respondents who have lost in the third vote are depicted. Nonstandardized mean decision acceptance with 95% CIs.

Figure 3

Figure 4. Perceived legitimacy of the decision-making procedure taking number of losses into account.Note: All respondents are depicted by the number of losses. Nonstandardized mean decision acceptance with 95% CIs.

Figure 4

Figure 5. The effect of accumulated losses on perceived legitimacy of the political system.Note: SWD: satisfaction with democracy; PolTrust: political trust.

Figure 5

Table 1. Frustration associated with losing

Figure 6

Table 2. Perceived fairness of the decision-making process taking the number of losses into account

Figure 7

Table 3. Predicting fairness perceptions and (changes in) legitimacy

Supplementary material: File

Kern et al. supplementary material

Kern et al. supplementary material

Download Kern et al. supplementary material(File)
File 407.2 KB