Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T02:38:12.011Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

On the dual nature of creativity: Same same but different?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 May 2024

Mathias Benedek*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, University of Graz, Graz, Austria mathias.benedek@uni-graz.at https://creativecognitionlab.uni-graz.at/
*
*Corresponding author.

Abstract

The creativity literature is replete with dualistic constructs, suggesting shared mechanisms but also tempting overinterpretation of their interrelations. An explicit list of relevant concept associations indicates substantial commonality, yet also exposes certain inconsistencies. Dual-process accounts (A and B is relevant) hold promise in resolving discrepancies to the extent that we understand the relative contributions and conditions of A and B.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

Creativity research has substantially progressed over the last few decades by amassing findings of how creativity is related to other psychological constructs. The target article by Ivancovsky et al. adds to this work by reviewing relevant literature and proposing novelty seeking as a shared mechanism underlying creativity and curiosity. This claim is substantiated with an extensive review suggesting similar correlates of both constructs across diverse literatures. Reading the target article, it occurred to me that what the authors have done is to perform a “mental factor analysis” on the correlates of creativity and curiosity to extract the factor explaining most shared variance. Identifying underlying factors and mechanisms that describe observations in a more parsimonious way is a central principle of science; however, this endeavor can run risk of oversimplifying matters by being selective in what evidence is considered or being overinclusive in its interpretation (Benedek & Jauk, Reference Benedek and Jauk2014). Hence, the question arises whether dualistic constructs, which are very popular in the creativity literature, refer to the same underlying factors, or not.

The extensive synthesis of the target article inspired me to explore what dual constructs are commonly associated in the creativity literature. Table 1 lists constructs distinguishing complemental or opposing concepts mentioned by this review as well as by other works (Baas, De Dreu, & Nijstad, Reference Baas, De Dreu and Nijstad2008; Boot, Baas, van Gaal, Cools, & De Dreu, Reference Boot, Baas, van Gaal, Cools and De Dreu2017; Campbell, Reference Campbell1960; De Dreu, Baas, & Nijstad, Reference De Dreu, Baas and Nijstad2008; Kounios & Beeman, Reference Kounios and Beeman2014; Neuberg & Newsom, Reference Neuberg and Newsom1993; Smallwood & Schooler, Reference Smallwood and Schooler2015; Troyer, Moscovitch, & Winocur, Reference Troyer, Moscovitch and Winocur1997; Zuckerman, Reference Zuckerman1984). Concepts listed in column A are often considered related whereas constructs listed in B are commonly considered distinct or opposing to those in A. The table further organizes constructs by overarching themes such as creative thinking, its neuroscientific basis, neophile traits and states (Griffin, Reference Griffin2016), motivation and moods, cognitive processes including (memory, attention, and cognitive control), and psychopathology. Aligning constructs in this way is helpful to uncover underlying, implicit assumptions and to eventually identify new links but also potential inconsistencies. It struck me that we tend to use different, sometimes vague labels for largely the same thing (e.g., defocused, broad, global attention), but then again, we are also very generous in presuming tight relationships across different constructs, together known as the jingle-jangle fallacy (Kelley, Reference Kelley1927). Importantly, although positive associations have been assumed between many constructs in A, it does not seem to work well for them all.

Table 1. Common dualistic constructs (distinguishing complemental or opposing concepts A and B) in creativity research: Are they (all) related?

Note: Concepts in italics were not considered in this context in the target article.

For instance, the target article as well as other works assume that idea generation versus evaluation are related to activation of the default mode network (DMN) and executive control network (ECN), and spontaneous versus controlled forms of thought, respectively. However, on the one hand, recent work suggests that episodic simulations associated with DMN structures also contribute to idea evaluation (Benedek, Beaty, Schacter, & Kenett, Reference Benedek, Beaty, Schacter and Kenett2023; Ren et al., Reference Ren, Huang, Zhou, Zhuang, Xu, Gao and Luo2020). On the other, there is abundant evidence linking idea generation to executive control (Benedek & Jauk, Reference Benedek, Jauk, Kaufman and Sternberg2019), as it involves core- and metacognitive control processes associated with overcoming prepotent responses and cognitive fixation as well as with finding and implementing goal-directed strategies (Lebuda & Benedek, Reference Lebuda and Benedek2023; Smith & Blankenship, Reference Smith and Blankenship1991); in contrast, the empirical evidence on the association between idea evaluation and cognitive control is still relatively scarce (but, see, Benedek et al., Reference Benedek, Nordtvedt, Jauk, Koschmieder, Pretsch, Krammer and Neubauer2016; Karwowski, Czerwonka, & Kaufman, Reference Karwowski, Czerwonka and Kaufman2020). Similarly, exploration versus exploitation of cognitive spaces corresponds to switching versus clustering tendencies (Troyer et al., Reference Troyer, Moscovitch and Winocur1997), with the former being related to higher cognitive control (Ovando-Tellez et al., Reference Ovando-Tellez, Benedek, Kenett, Hills, Bouanane, Bernard and Volle2022; Rubinstein, Meyer, & Evans, Reference Rubinstein, Meyer and Evans2001) – again opposing a simple exploration–spontaneity relationship.

It seems that Ivancovsky et al. also came to the conclusion that cognitive control does not fit well with the other constructs, which motivated them to eventually propose a two-dimensional model with exploration/exploitation and inhibition/disinhibition (akin to cognitive control) as independent factors. This makes sense in my view, although it was not clear why convergent thinking ended up on the exploration side. Another important move was to acknowledge that creativity and curiosity are strongly associated with A, but actually rely on both A and B. This view is consistent with long-standing dual-process models that emphasized the interplay of exploration and exploitation modes (Hart et al., Reference Hart, Goldberg, Striem-Amit, Mayo, Noy and Alon2018; Hills, Jones, & Todd, Reference Hills, Jones and Todd2012) also known as flexibility versus persistence pathways to creativity (De Dreu et al., Reference De Dreu, Baas and Nijstad2008; Nijstad, De Dreu, Rietzschel, & Baas, Reference Nijstad, De Dreu, Rietzschel and Baas2010), as well as arguing for the relevance of an interplay between spontaneous and controlled processes (Benedek & Jauk, Reference Benedek, Jauk, Fox and Christoff2018; Sowden, Pringle, & Gabora, Reference Sowden, Pringle and Gabora2015; Zabelina & Robinson, Reference Zabelina and Robinson2010). Future research is challenged to study the very nature and conditions of these interplays in more detail to avoid oversimplistic conclusions such as that everything is important for creativity. Another question refers to the actual independency of exploration/exploitation with the level of cognitive control, which appears supported by recent work showing that clustering and switching can occur fast and slow (Ovando-Tellez et al., Reference Ovando-Tellez, Kenett, Benedek, Hills, Beranger and Volle2023). The list in Table 1 may prove useful in reconsidering our theorizing on these dualisms toward enhancing our understanding of the nomological network of creativity (Kenett et al., Reference Kenett, Kraemer, Alfred, Colaizzi, Cortes and Green2020).

Competing interest

None.

References

Baas, M., De Dreu, C. K. W., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus? Psychological Bulletin, 134(6), 779806. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012815CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedek, M., Beaty, R. E., Schacter, D. L., & Kenett, Y. N. (2023). The role of memory in creative ideation. Nature Reviews Psychology, 2(4), 246257. https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-023-00158-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., & Jauk, E. (2014). Creativity – Lost in simplification? Creativity. Theories – Research – Applications, 1(2), 213219. https://doi.org/10.15290/ctra.2014.01.02.06CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., & Jauk, E. (2018). Spontaneous and controlled processes in creative cognition. In Fox, K. C. R. & Christoff, K. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of spontaneous thought (pp. 285298). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Benedek, M., & Jauk, E. (2019). Creativity and cognitive control. In Kaufman, J. C. & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 200223). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316979839.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedek, M., Nordtvedt, N., Jauk, E., Koschmieder, C., Pretsch, J., Krammer, G., & Neubauer, A. C. (2016). Assessment of creativity evaluation skills: A psychometric investigation in prospective teachers. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 21, 7584. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.05.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boot, N., Baas, M., van Gaal, S., Cools, R., & De Dreu, C. K. W. (2017). Creative cognition and dopaminergic modulation of fronto-striatal networks: Integrative review and research agenda. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 78, 1323. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind variation and selective retentions in creative thought as in other knowledge processes. Psychological Review, 67(6), 380400. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0040373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Dreu, C. K. W., Baas, M., & Nijstad, B. A. (2008). Hedonic tone and activation level in the mood–creativity link: Toward a dual pathway to creativity model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(5), 739756. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.5.739CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, A. S. (2016). Innovativeness as an emergent property: A new alignment of comparative and experimental research on animal innovation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690), 20150544. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0544CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, Y., Goldberg, H., Striem-Amit, E., Mayo, A. E., Noy, L., & Alon, U. (2018). Creative exploration as a scale-invariant search on a meaning landscape. Nature Communications, 9(1), 111. Article 5411. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-07715-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hills, T. T., Jones, M. N., & Todd, P. M. (2012). Optimal foraging in semantic memory. Psychological Review, 119(2), 431440. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027373CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karwowski, M., Czerwonka, M., & Kaufman, J. (2020). Does intelligence strengthen creative metacognition? Psychology of Aesthetics Creativity and the Arts, 14(3), 353360. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, T. L. (1927). Interpretation of educational measurements. World Book.Google Scholar
Kenett, Y. N., Kraemer, D. J. M., Alfred, K. L., Colaizzi, G. A., Cortes, R. A., & Green, A. E. (2020). Developing a neurally informed ontology of creativity measurement. NeuroImage, 221, 117166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117166CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2014). The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 7193. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115154CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lebuda, I., & Benedek, M. (2023). A systematic framework of creative metacognition. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/s793qCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neuberg, S. L., & Newsom, J. T. (1993). Personal need for structure: Individual differences in the desire for simpler structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(1), 113131. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.1.113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu, C. K. W., Rietzschel, E. F., & Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European Review of Social Psychology, 21(1), 3477. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463281003765323CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ovando-Tellez, M., Benedek, M., Kenett, Y. N., Hills, T., Bouanane, S., Bernard, M., … Volle, E. (2022). An investigation of the cognitive and neural correlates of semantic memory search related to creative ability. Communications Biology, 5(1). Article 604. https://doi.org/10.1038/s42003-022-03547-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ovando-Tellez, M., Kenett, Y. N., Benedek, , M., Hills, T. T., Beranger, B., … Volle, E. (2023). Switching, fast and slow: Deciphering the dynamics of memory search, its brain connectivity patterns, and its role in creativity. https://doi.org/10.21203/rs.3.rs-3826172/v1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ren, J., Huang, F., Zhou, Y., Zhuang, L., Xu, J., Gao, C., … Luo, J. (2020). The function of the hippocampus and middle temporal gyrus in forming new associations and concepts during the processing of novelty and usefulness features in creative designs. NeuroImage, 214, 116751. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116751CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubinstein, J. S., Meyer, D. E., & Evans, J. E. (2001). Executive control of cognitive processes in task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27(4), 763797. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.27.4.763Google ScholarPubMed
Smallwood, J., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 487518. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, S. M., & Blankenship, S. E. (1991). Incubation and the persistence of fixation in problem solving. The American Journal of Psychology, 104(1), 61. https://doi.org/10.2307/1422851CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sowden, P. T., Pringle, A., & Gabora, L. (2015). The shifting sands of creative thinking: Connections to dual-process theory. Thinking & Reasoning, 21(1), 4060. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2014.885464CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Troyer, A. K., Moscovitch, M., & Winocur, G. (1997). Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: Evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 11(1), 138146. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zabelina, D. L., & Robinson, M. D. (2010). Creativity as flexible cognitive control. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 4(3), 136143. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zuckerman, M. (1984). Sensation seeking: A comparative approach to a human trait. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 7(3), 413434. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00018938CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Common dualistic constructs (distinguishing complemental or opposing concepts A and B) in creativity research: Are they (all) related?