Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-19T14:17:39.868Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonetically incomplete neutralisation can be phonologically complete: evidence from Huai’an Mandarin

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 January 2024

Naiyan Du
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Languages, and Cultures, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824, United States of America
Karthik Durvasula*
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics, Languages, and Cultures, Michigan State University, East Lansing, MI 48824, United States of America
*
Corresponding author: Karthik Durvasula; Email: karthikd@msu.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The phenomenon of incomplete neutralisation describes a situation where a putative case of categorical phonological neutralisation is observed to be phonetically non-neutralising. This has been argued to be a problem for phonological theories that employ categorical features. Here, we use two distinct feeding orders of tone sandhi processes from Huai’an Mandarin to show that incomplete phonetic neutralisation is compatible with categorical phonological phenomena. Therefore, incomplete phonetic neutralisation does not automatically inform us of gradient phonological representations. We further show that incomplete phonetic neutralisation can in fact have a large effect size. Such results are not surprising from a classic generative view of phonology where linguistic performance is argued to be a multi-factorial problem, and linguistic knowledge (i.e., competence) is only one of the many factors involved. Furthermore, our results suggest that the observed incompleteness or gradience may have a source outside phonological knowledge.

Type
Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2024. Published by Cambridge University Press

1 Introduction

As has long been recognised in discussions of linguistic competence as abstract knowledge, there are multiple potential interacting factors in performance (Chomsky Reference Chomsky1964, Reference Chomsky1965; Valian Reference Valian, Simon and Scholes1982; Schütze Reference Schütze1996, inter alia). Similarly, there are also multiple interacting sources that affect speech production, for example, lexical knowledge, phonological knowledge and memory and processing constraints (Whalen Reference Whalen1991, Reference Whalen1992; Warner et al. Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004; Wright Reference Wright, Local, Ogden and Temple2004). Consequently, gradience in phonetic manifestations cannot automatically be used as a diagnostic of gradient phonological representations. In this article, we explore the phenomenon of incomplete neutralisation to argue that incomplete phonetic neutralisation does not automatically inform us about phonological representations or phonological knowledge, more generally. We present relevant data from Tone 1 and Tone 4 sandhi processes in Huai’an Mandarin (Huai’an hereafter), both of which crucially participate in feeding orders to trigger other tone sandhi processes, to argue that phonetically incomplete neutralisation can still be phonologically complete.

Since at least the mid-1980s, the effect of incomplete neutralisation has been documented in a variety of languages including Catalan (Dinnsen & Charles-Luce Reference Dinnsen and Charles-Luce1984), Dutch (Warner et al. Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004; Ernestus & Baayen Reference Ernestus, Baayen, Goldstein, Whalen and Best2006), Japanese (Braver & Kawahara Reference Braver, Kawahara, Albright and Fullwood2016), Polish (Slowiaczek & Dinnsen Reference Slowiaczek and Dinnsen1985; Slowiaczek & Szymanska Reference Slowiaczek and Szymanska1989) and Russian (Dmitrieva Reference Dmitrieva2005; Kharlamov Reference Kharlamov2012; Matsui Reference Matsui2015). For example, in German, it has been described that the phonological voicing contrast for obstruents is neutralised at the right edge of a prosodic word (Wagner Reference Wagner2002). A rule-based mapping of the relevant phonological process is stated in (1). However, careful phonetic and perceptual experimentation has shown that the neutralisation is incomplete phonetically (Port & O’Dell Reference Port and O’Dell1985; Roettger et al. Reference Roettger, Winter, Grawunder, Kirby and Grice2014, inter alia). In other words, underlying voiceless stops, derived voiceless stops and underlying voiced stops all have different phonetic distributions.Footnote 1

The observed effect of incomplete neutralisation has been argued by many researchers to pose a challenge to traditional formal phonology where categorical phonological representation and modular feed-forward model are assumed (Manaster Ramer Reference Manaster Ramer1996; Port & Leary Reference Port and Leary2005; Goldrick & Blumstein Reference Goldrick and Blumstein2006; Roettger et al. Reference Roettger, Winter, Grawunder, Kirby and Grice2014; Braver Reference Braver2019; McCollum Reference McCollum, Baek, Takahashi and Yeung2019). It is often assumed that under such a feed-forward framework, the phonological representations are discrete elements that do not contain any gradient phonetic information, and phonetics only has access to the output of phonology (Kenstowicz Reference Kenstowicz1994; Pierrehumbert Reference Pierrehumbert, Gussenhoven and Warner2002). We call this view the Standard generative view of phonology.Footnote 2 As a result, underlying representations that undergo phonological neutralisation process should not have any consequence on phonetic manifestations. However, in cases of incomplete neutralisation, there are traces of the underlying representation in the phonetic manifestation.

One trivial but widely adopted solution to the puzzle posed by incomplete neutralisation has been to simply deny that such an effect can be caused by grammatical knowledge (Dinnsen & Charles-Luce Reference Dinnsen and Charles-Luce1984; Fourakis & Iverson Reference Fourakis and Iverson1984; Manaster Ramer Reference Manaster Ramer1996; Warner et al. Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004, inter alia). To support this claim, several criticisms have been raised against previous experimental designs as well as the interpretation of the results. One main criticism is whether the observed phenomenon of incomplete neutralisation is due to task effects. Among these effects, the most discussed one is orthography. It has been noticed by many researchers (Fourakis & Iverson Reference Fourakis and Iverson1984; Manaster Ramer Reference Manaster Ramer1996, inter alia) that in the seminal work of Port & O’Dell (Reference Port and O’Dell1985), participants were presented stimuli orthographically where minimal pairs were always in contrast. Native speakers of German may have hypercorrected and produced unnatural speech to match the forms of orthography. This suspicion becomes especially disturbing when Warner et al. (Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004) showed a significant production difference in words that are identical in underlying representations but differ in orthography in Dutch.Footnote 3 To circumvent the interference of orthography, two methods have been employed, namely changing the experimental paradigm and looking at languages where the relevant phonological contrast is not reflected orthographically.

In line with changing the experimental paradigm, Fourakis & Iverson (Reference Fourakis and Iverson1984) employed a unique strategy aimed at concealing the morphological forms that native speakers of German are supposed to produce, so the influence of orthography is expected to decrease. The participants were instead presented auditorily with the conjugated form where the contrast is maintained in both underlying and surface representations and asked to decompose and produce the bare form where the incomplete neutralisation is expected to happen. Through this paradigm, Fourakis and Iverson camouflaged the task as a morphological exercise to distract the participants to elicit more natural pronunciations. Interestingly, the effect of incomplete neutralisation was not observed, and Fourakis and Iverson concluded that the previously found incomplete neutralisation was actually a task effect. Using a different strategy, Jassem & Richter (Reference Jassem and Richter1989) asked participants to answer questions designed to elicit the target words in Polish, and observed no evidence of incomplete neutralisation. However, by implementing the same strategy as Fourakis & Iverson (Reference Fourakis and Iverson1984) and increasing the statistical power with more speakers and more test minimal pairs, Roettger et al. (Reference Roettger, Winter, Grawunder, Kirby and Grice2014) found an effect of incomplete neutralisation. However, it is worth noting that, as Roettger et al. (Reference Roettger, Winter, Grawunder, Kirby and Grice2014) themselves pointed out, the strategy employed by Fourakis and Iverson can incur a potential artifact of phonetic accommodation. In the experimental paradigm, the participants hear the conjugated form where neutralisation cannot happen and the voicing contrast is present, and have to produce the form where neutralisation does happen. In such a paradigm, the observed effect of incomplete neutralisation may be due to the participants mirroring vowel duration differences in the stimulus recordings they heard of the conjugated forms. When Roettger et al. (Reference Roettger, Winter, Grawunder, Kirby and Grice2014) controlled for this confound in one of their experiments, they found only a very small, non-significant effect ( ${<}3$ ms) in the right direction. This suggests that there might indeed be no clear evidence for incomplete neutralisation even in their well-powered study. To sum up, this general strategy to solve the problem of orthography by changing the task performed by the participants leads to very weak evidence (if that) for the presence of incomplete neutralisation.

A second method employed to overcome task effects related to orthography has been to use a language where the crucial contrast is not marked in the orthography. For example, Catalan has been claimed to have a devoicing process but no orthographic marking of an underlying voicing contrast under any phonological conditions, and Dinnsen & Charles-Luce (Reference Dinnsen and Charles-Luce1984) did not observe any evidence of incomplete neutralisation in Catalan devoicing. However, later Charles-Luce & Dinnsen (Reference Charles-Luce and Dinnsen1987) reanalysed their data and found incomplete neutralisation in the cue of voicing into closure. Here, it is worth noting that in Catalan, quite a few words actually maintain the underlying voicing contrast in orthography, so the real situation is more complicated and Catalan cannot simply be treated as a language that does not mark underlying voicing contrast orthographically (Badia Margarit Reference Badia Margarit1962; Manaster Ramer Reference Manaster Ramer1996).

In another case, Braver & Kawahara (Reference Braver, Kawahara, Albright and Fullwood2016) observed a putative case of incomplete neutralisation in Japanese. Most of their stimuli were presented in Chinese characters (kanji), which is an orthographic system that is commonly used in Japanese but only has a very weak connection with pronunciation.Footnote 4 Although most Chinese characters were originally created by combining a part that indicates pronunciation and a part that indicates meaning (Yang Reference Yang1995), the connection between characters and pronunciation is largely obscured by historical sound change and character change (Huang & Liao Reference Huang and Liao2017). In Japanese, most Chinese characters are used to represent both borrowed words from China (the Sino-Japanese lexicon) and words that are originated in Japan (the Yamato lexicon) (Japan Broadcasting Corporation 1998; Itô & Mester Reference Itô, Mester and Tsujimura1999), and the resulting multiple pronunciations (onyomi and kunyomi) of many Chinese characters can only further weaken the connection between Chinese characters and pronunciations. So it is hard to imagine that Japanese speakers hypercorrected based on Chinese characters, and Braver and Kawahara still appeared to observe incomplete neutralisation in monomoraic prosodic word lengthening process.

To sum up, although the case of Catalan is controversial, the case of Japanese provides good evidence that at least in some languages, the observed incomplete neutralisation is not caused by orthographic knowledge.

Another source of criticism of incomplete neutralisation is that the observed effect size is typically quite small. Small effect sizes have been argued to likely not be functionally significant and therefore not to need a grammatical explanation (Dinnsen & Charles-Luce Reference Dinnsen and Charles-Luce1984; Mascaró Reference Mascaró1987; Warner et al. Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004).Footnote 5 For example, among the phonetic cues examined by Port & O’Dell (Reference Port and O’Dell1985), preceding vowel duration before underlying voiced stops was only about 15 ms longer than that before underlying voiceless stops, voicing into closure of derived voiceless stops was only 5 ms longer than that of underlying voiceless stops and duration of aspiration noise before underlying voiceless stops was only 15 ms longer than that of derived voiceless stops. Similar effect sizes were also found in Polish (Slowiaczek & Dinnsen Reference Slowiaczek and Dinnsen1985; Jassem & Richter Reference Jassem and Richter1989), Dutch (Warner et al. Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004) and two other studies on German (Piroth & Janker Reference Piroth and Janker2004; Roettger et al. Reference Roettger, Winter, Grawunder, Kirby and Grice2014). To summarise the discussion on the criticisms on incomplete neutralisation, the debate on the existence of incomplete neutralisation is still pretty much ongoing, especially with respect to the issue of effect size.

In this article, as mentioned above, we will argue using data from Huai’an that incomplete phonetic neutralisation can stem from phonologically complete neutralisation. By using Huai’an, we avoid the orthographic confound discussed above, as the stimuli can be presented in Chinese characters, an orthographic system that has only a weak connection with pronunciation (this is similar to the Japanese case discussed above). Furthermore, the language allows us to argue that effect sizes are tangential to the issue of phonological neutralisation. Anticipating our results, we show that although there is a rather large phonetic difference with respect to incomplete phonetic neutralisation, there is clear evidence that the relevant processes are phonologically categorically neutralising, as evidenced by the fact that their outputs feed other sandhi processes.

2 The issue of phonological neutralisation versus phonetic implementation

As introduced in §1, the definition of incomplete neutralisation is twofold, being a combination of phonological neutralisation and phonetically incomplete neutralisation. An issue with many previous studies of incomplete neutralisation is that researchers do not typically show evidence that the examined processes are truly phonological neutralisation, as opposed to phonetic implementation (Cohn Reference Cohn1993; Dunbar Reference Dunbar2013). Under the categorical view of phonological representations, phonological neutralisation entails a change from one phonological category to another phonological category, while phonetic implementation does not result in any categorical changes. To give an example, it is assumed by Port & O’Dell (Reference Port and O’Dell1985) and other previous studies on German that the devoicing process results in a voiceless obstruent category in the phonological surface form.Footnote 6 However, there is no clear evidence, especially evidence from phonological behaviour, that shows a derived voiceless obstruent is actually neutralised with the underlying voiceless obstruent in the phonology. If Dunbar’s (Reference Dunbar2013) suspicion that word-final devoicing in German is actually a phonetic implementational process turns out to be valid, then the so-called ‘devoiced obstruent’ at the right edge of prosodic word remains phonologically unchanged and still belongs to the same ‘voiced’ category with voiced obstruents in other positions. As a result, it would not be surprising according to the Standard generative view of phonology that the so-called ‘devoiced obstruent’ is phonetically different from an underlying voiceless obstruent since they are phonologically different, that is, different in the surface representations.

To the best of our knowledge, the only careful previous study that attempted to establish phonological neutralisation using evidence from phonological behaviour is Braver and Kawahara’s (Reference Braver, Kawahara, Albright and Fullwood2016) study on the lengthening of prosodic words in Japanese. In Japanese, since a prosodic word has been argued to be at least bimoraic (Itô Reference Itô1990; Mester Reference Mester1990; Poser Reference Poser1990; Mori Reference Mori2002; Itô & Mester Reference Itô, Mester, Honma, Okazaki, Tabata and Tanaka2003), an underlying monomoraic prosodic word has been argued to lengthen to be bimoraic. Braver & Kawahara (Reference Braver, Kawahara, Albright and Fullwood2016) showed that this neutralisation is incomplete phonetically, that is, a derived bimoraic prosodic word is still shorter than an underlying bimoraic prosodic word.

The current article utilises a different strategy of examining rules in feeding orders to establish phonological neutralisation. The fact that the output of a process can trigger another process provides evidence that the first process results in complete phonological neutralisation. Yet, despite the categorical neutralisation in the phonology, we will show that there is incomplete neutralisation in the phonetics for each of the feeding processes in Huai’an tone sandhi processes.Footnote 7 We will elaborate the feeding orders in Huai’an in §3 with more background information. And, §§4 and 5 will present the two experiments we have run based on two feeding orders in Huai’an.

3 Background

Huai’an belongs to the Jianghuai Guanhua Group (Lower Yangtze Mandarin) of the Mandarin language family. Native speakers are mainly from Huai’an city, which is located in the northern part of Jiangsu Province (Li Reference Li1989). Huai’an has four phonemic tones, labelled as Tone 1, Tone 2, Tone 3 and Tone 4 (Jiao Reference Jiao2004; Wang & Kang Reference Wang and Kang2012). Following the tradition of tone description in Chinese languages, in Table 1, the four tones are given in tone letters using a scale of 1–5 where 1 is the lowest f0 and 5 is the highest f0 and followed by a contour description in words (Chao Reference Chao1930).Footnote 8 The tonal contours of phonemic tones in isolation are given in Figure 1. The speaker (male, age: 53) pronounced four repetitions of four monosyllabic morphemes that share the same segmental content [sɔ] and only contrast in the tone on the vowel. f0 was extracted only from the vowel at 5% steps with a script in Praat (Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2021). However, it is worth noting that the tonal contours in isolation for Mandarin tones have been noticed to be quite different when compared with their counterparts in context (Shen Reference Shen1990; Xu Reference Xu1994, Reference Xu1997; Jongman et al. Reference Jongman, Wang, Moore, Sereno, Li, Tan, Bates and Tzeng2006, inter alia). So, we expected the same kinds of differences in our experiments where tones are pronounced in sentences.

Table 1 Description of phonemic tones in Huai’an.

Figure 1 Tonal contour of phonemic tones in Huai’an.

In subsequent examples, tones will be identified with just a T before the tone number, as in T3 for Tone 3; we will, however, continue to use full forms such as Tone 3 in the text.

The three tone sandhi rules relevant for this article are shown in (2). At the post-lexical level, the low-register Tone 3 sandhi is mandatory in some contexts and optional in others (we will elaborate at a later point in this article). In contrast, the high-register Tone 1 and Tone 4 sandhis are always optional. Furthermore, Tone 3 undergoes tone sandhi to become Tone 2, and this Tone 3 sandhi process can only happen when immediately preceding Tone 3 (underlying or derived).Footnote 9 As dissimilation processes, the tone sandhis in Huai’an can be straightforwardly explained by the Obligatory Contour Principle (Leben Reference Leben1973; McCarthy Reference McCarthy1986; Yip Reference Yip2002, inter alia). However, some researchers reject the Obligatory Contour Principle as the motivation for tone sandhi processes in Mandarin languages (Duanmu Reference Duanmu1994, Reference Duanmu2007, inter alia). We will not address this debate since it is tangential to the main argument of this article.Footnote 10

Crucially, the Tone 3 output of the high-register tone sandhi processes feeds the low-register Tone 3 sandhi process as in (3). Since high-register tone sandhis are optional and Tone 3 sandhi is also optional for trisyllabic utterances in (3), multiple surface representations are possible for both examples.

The feeding relationships between each of the high-register tone sandhis and Tone 3 sandhi suggest that the high-register tone sandhis result in a Tone 3 category that is phonologically the same as an underlying Tone 3. This interpretation of the data remains the same given a parallel approach to phonology such as Optimality Theory (Prince & Smolensky Reference Prince and Smolensky1993). A usually employed markedness constraint for low-register tone sandhi in Mandarin languages is *33, which is based on the Obligatory Contour Principle. This constraint penalises adjacent Tone 3 syllables (Zhang Reference Zhang1997; Wang & Lin Reference Wang, Lin and Jing-Schmidt2011; see also Chen Reference Chen2000 for an implicit use of this constraint). For this constraint to trigger the structural change in the first syllable (namely, Tone 3 → Tone 2), the second syllable in an underlying /Tone 3 Tone 4 Tone 4/ or /Tone 3 Tone 1 Tone 1/ sequence must surface with Tone 3. Consequently, an Optimality Theory analysis would also maintain the crucial categorical aspects of the feeding order that are focal for the current article.

With regard to this interpretation of phonological identity between derived and underlying Tone 3s, concerns may be raised about application rates, especially when an underlying Tone 3 mandatorily triggers Tone 3 sandhi while a derived Tone 3 can only optionally trigger Tone 3 sandhi when the two types of Tone 3 syllables are the middle syllable of a trisyllabic utterance. The comparison is shown in (3) and (4). And, some researchers may want to ascribe the difference in application rates to a difference between derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3 in the phonology, either as different phonological representations or as the same representations indexed to different variable processes. However, intervening factors are not controlled for when compared in this way. For Tone 3 sandhi to mandatorily apply before an underlying Tone 3, the established planning window only needs to include the two Tone 3 syllables. Therefore, in (4), the established planning window only needs to include the first two syllables. In contrast, for Tone 3 sandhi to apply before a derived Tone 3, the established planning window needs to include at least three syllables to ensure both high-register Tone 1/Tone 4 sandhi and low-register Tone 3 sandhi occur. It is well recognised in the previous literature that a larger planning window has more planning difficulty and is therefore less likely (Ferreira & Swets Reference Ferreira and Swets2002; Wagner et al. Reference Wagner, Jescheniak and Schriefers2010; Kilbourn-Ceron & Goldrick Reference Kilbourn-Ceron and Goldrick2021, inter alia). The reason is the increasing burden on working memory, which can lead to speech errors or delays. Huai’an turns out to not be an exception. Previous experimental study on Tone 3 sandhi in Huai’an does support the existence of the effect of planning difficulty (Du & Lin Reference Du and Lin2021). Due to such an effect, a planning window that extends three syllables long is less likely to be established in (3), which means Tone 3 sandhi is less likely to apply before a derived Tone 3. Overall, the difference in application rates comes naturally from the planning difficulty effect and does not need to be accounted for in the phonology.Footnote 11

As pointed out by two anonymous reviewers, proponents of gradient phonological representation may argue that although both underlying and derived Tone 3s can trigger Tone 3 sandhi, they may still have different phonological representations. By this analysis, the difference in application rates would be explained by the difference in the phonological representations. First, we would like to point out that any analysis that predicts application rates based on gradient phonological representations or phonetic similarity would have to be precise in accounting for not only cases where the process is triggered but also cases where the process is not triggered; namely, it would have to explain why only the derived Tone 3 shows a variation in application rates and not the underlying Tone 3, and not the other way around. Furthermore, it would have to account for the fact that any other tones that are phonetically similar (along the relevant dimensions) do not trigger the process. While an evaluation of such an analysis is not possible without a concrete specification of the proposal, we suspect that, to explain the difference between derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3, one will have to make reference to performance factors anyway. Relatedly, we appeal to the need to prioritise relatively simple categorical phonological representations when they are sufficient to account for the observed patterns (per Occam’s razor/the law of parsimony); in our case, the difference in application rates can be accounted for by independently needed performance factors, namely planning, and therefore we need not complicate our understanding of the relevant phonological (tonal) representations. For this reason, we see the feeding rule interaction as evidence of complete phonological neutralisation of the derived Tone 3 from Tone 1 and Tone 4 sandhi processes. Furthermore, we use the processes to probe the phonetic (acoustic) consequences of the neutralising processes in the case of the derived Tone 3 that in turn trigger Tone 3 sandhi.

To further ensure the phonological equivalence of derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3, we only analyse the derived Tone 3 tokens that actually trigger Tone 3 sandhi in this article, which allows us to have perfect surface minimal pairs in each of our experiments. By doing so, we also exclude the possibility that any identified incomplete phonetic neutralisation patterns arise as a result of averaging the outcomes of an optional phonological process, since we only look at the cases where we have reason to believe that the process has applied. Despite the categorical phonological behaviour of the derived Tone 3 in Huai’an, in the next two sections, we will show that there is substantial incomplete phonetic neutralisation of derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3 for the feeding orders involving Tone 1 sandhi and Tone 4 sandhi.

4 Experiment 1: Tone 1 sandhi

4.1 Participants

We recruited 11 native speakers of Huai’an Mandarin via personal relationships in Huai’an City. The age range was from 37 to 55 years. Among them, eight self-identified as female, and three as male. Due to the language standardisation trend in mainland China (Ramsey Reference Ramsey1989), young speakers in Huai’an are generally bilingual and are native speakers of both Huai’an and Standard Mandarin. To minimise the influence of Standard Mandarin, we recruited older speakers who are only fluent in Huai’an. All the participants were born and raised in Huai’an City. None of them had participated in any linguistic studies before or heard about the concept of incomplete neutralisation.

4.2 Stimuli

The stimuli were composed of trisyllabic sentences with each syllable forming a separate word, to ensure that the tone sandhi processes observed are post-lexical and completely productive. Also, only right-branching utterances as in (3) are employed, simply because not enough left-branching utterances could be constructed that would have all the other characteristics required by the experimental design. The stimuli were divided into four sets as shown in (5). Furthermore, the third syllable was always Tone 1. The second syllable was one of the following: (a) an underlying Tone 1 that optionally underwent Tone 1 sandhi to become Tone 3 or (b) an underlying Tone 3 that did not undergo any tone sandhi in this context. The first syllable was underlyingly Tone 3 or Tone 2. As a consequence of the possibilities in the second syllable, there were a few different possibilities for the first syllable, including (a) an underlying Tone 3 that could undergo Tone 3 sandhi to become Tone 2 with reference to the second syllable and (b) an underlying Tone 2 that did not undergo any tone sandhi in this context. The four sets differed only in tonal patterns and not in segmental content. Furthermore, the crucial second syllable was always a sequence of a voiceless unaspirated stop followed by a vowel. Voiceless unaspirated stops were chosen to make sure that there would be a consistent way to identify the acoustic onset of the vowel by referring to the burst of the stop. The full stimulus list is summarised in Appendix A. It is worth noting that one character, 搭, may be pronounced with the only checked tone in Huai’an (Jiao Reference Jiao2004; Wang & Kang Reference Wang and Kang2012), which is an allophone of Tone 4 and appears only on monomoraic syllables ending with glottal stop. We excluded all checked tone productions when extracting f0 information.

Out of the above set of possibilities, the most crucial comparison is between two tones in the second syllable, namely, an underlying Tone 3 as in (5b) and a derived Tone 3 as in the first possibility in (5d). This particular comparison controls for the preceding surface context (derived Tone 2) and the following surface context (underlying Tone 1) and is therefore a perfect minimal pair. Furthermore, the two cases also show evidence that both tones are in fact categorically Tone 3, as they trigger Tone 3 sandhi on the preceding tone. Finally, as mentioned previously, the comparison allows us to exclude the possibility that any identified incomplete phonetic neutralisation pattern arises as a result of averaging the outcomes of an optional phonological process. This is the crucial pair we will focus on in this experiment.

The set of possibilities also allows us to visually compare the derived Tone 3 against an underlying Tone 1 in the same surface context, as in the second possibility in (5c) (although the preceding syllable in this case is an underlying Tone 2 instead of a derived Tone 2).

Each participant produced four repetitions of 24 test and 27 filler sentences at a natural speech rate, which means each participant read a total of 204 sentences. All stimuli were randomised for each participant.

4.3 Procedure

The experiment was conducted entirely in Huai’an city. Each participant was recorded by a trained research assistant using Audacity (Audacity Team 2019) and a Popu Line BK USB microphone on a Lenovo laptop in a quiet room that was located in the participant’s home or workplace. The participants were told that the purpose of the study was to collect some general information on Huai’an. In post-experiment interviews, none of the participants reported noticing the minimal pairs, or that tones were the real focus of the study. The participants were instructed to read at a normal speech rate using their everyday voice, and the stimuli were presented in Chinese characters. The participants were also encouraged to read through the stimulus list to be familiar with the reading materials before producing them.

4.4 Measurement

Using Praat (Boersma & Weenink Reference Boersma and Weenink2021), the recordings were manually annotated by the first author, who is a native speaker of Huai’an. An example is shown in Figure 2. Only the second syllable was marked, and the annotation file had six tiers in total. The first tier marked the vowel of the second syllable for phonetic analysis. The first zero-crossing at the beginning of the voicing of the target vowel and after the burst of the unaspirated stop was identified as the vowel onset, and the zero-crossing immediately following the vowel’s final glottal pulse was identified as the vowel offset. All other tiers marked the whole second syllable to index phonological information and recording quality. The onset of the second syllable was marked just before the release burst of the initial stop, and the offset of the second syllable corresponded with the offset of the nuclear vowel. The second tier indicated the whole sentence in pinyin, which is the official romanisation system for Chinese characters in China. The third tier was the tone sandhi condition where ‘yes’ meant the second syllable had undergone tone sandhi and ‘no’ meant it had not; the fourth and fifth tiers indicated the underlying tones and surface tones, respectively; and the last tier had the quality of the recording. We only used productions that were marked ‘good’ in the analysis. The reasons that productions were not marked as ‘good’ included background noise, speech errors, any long delay while producing the utterance, and checked tone pronunciation. f0 was extracted only from the vowel at 5% steps with a script in Praat.

Figure 2 Annotation scheme of Experiment 1 (Tone 1).

To compare across different speakers and different vowels, z-score transformation was performed for each vowel of each speaker based on Hz scale (Laplace Reference Laplace1820; Lobanov Reference Lobanov1971).

4.5 Results and statistical modelling

All data analyses in this article were performed in R (R Core Team 2021) using the tidyverse suite of packages (Wickham et al. Reference Wickham, Averick, Bryan, Chang, McGowan, François, Grolemund, Hayes, Henry, Hester, Kuhn, Pedersen, Miller, Bache, Müller, Ooms, Robinson, Seidel, Spinu, Takahashi, Vaughan, Wilke, Woo and Yutani2019). The statistical modelling was done using the lme4 package (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Mächler, Bolker and Walker2021).Footnote 12

The number of tokens for each possible combination of underlying representation and surface representation is summarised in Table 2. The application rate of Tone 3 sandhi before underlying Tone 3 is 97.2%, while the application rate before derived Tone 3 is 74.0%.Footnote 13 Seventy-one tokens were not marked as ‘good’ and excluded, which accounts for 6.7% of all test stimuli.

Table 2 Number of tokens for each UR and SR combination in Experiment 1.

The z-score transformed f0 contours on the crucial second syllable are shown in Figure 3. As a reminder, the crucial comparison is between a derived Tone 3 and an underlying Tone 3 after derived Tone 2s in the same surface context – the context establishes that both the Tone 3s are categorically Tone 3 phonologically, as they trigger Tone 3 sandhi. We also present the tone contour for an underlying Tone 1 in the same surface context for visual comparison with the two crucial Tone 3s.

Figure 3 Comparison of second-syllable contours in Experiment 1 (Tone 1; error bars indicate standard error).

Based on visual inspection of the data, the derived Tone 3 seems to start like an underlying Tone 3 and end like an underlying Tone 1. And, the contour shape of the derived Tone 3 is close to that of an underlying Tone 3. Furthermore, the comparison between underlying Tone 3 and derived Tone 3 clearly shows that the neutralisation is incomplete.Footnote 14

For the purposes of statistical modelling, we used just the two-group factor (underlying Tone 3 vs. derived Tone 3), and ignored underlying Tone 1, in order to simplify the modelling and address only the crucial question of whether or not the underlying and derived Tone 3s have incompletely neutralised. The results turn out to support the observation that the neutralisation is indeed incomplete phonetically.

In dealing with time-course data, traditional techniques like t-tests and ANOVA have to divide continuous time into multiple time bins and therefore have to make multiple comparisons. This method has been argued by Mirman (Reference Mirman2017) to be problematic for increasing the risk of ‘false positives’. Since each time bin incurs the nominal 5% false positive rate implied by ‘ $p < 0.05$ ’, overall, the false positive rate with multiple time bins and multiple comparisons will be much higher than a single comparison.

To solve this problem, multiple analysis methods have been developed, including Smooth Spline Analysis of Variance (Wang Reference Wang1998), generalised additive model (Hastie & Tibshirani Reference Hastie and Tibshirani1990) and growth curve analysis (Mirman et al. Reference Mirman, Dixon and Magnuson2008; Mirman Reference Mirman2017). In this article, we follow Chen et al. (Reference Chen, Zhang, McCollum and Wayland2017) in modelling f0 contours using growth curve analysis. Growth curve analysis uses multilevel linear regression to avoid multiple comparisons, and has been argued to be a useful modelling technique in different fields (Baldwin & Hoffmann Reference Baldwin and Hoffmann2002; McArdle & Nesselroade Reference McArdle, Nesselroade, Velicer and Schinka2003, inter alia). To apply growth curve analysis in Huai’an tones, we started with a simple model as in (6) (Mirman et al. Reference Mirman, Dixon and Magnuson2008).

Here, i is the ith f0 (z-score transformed) contour and j is the jth time point; $Y_{{ij}}$ is the f0 (z-score transformed) value for the ith contour at the jth time point. ${\gamma}_{00}$ is the population average value for the intercept, ${\zeta}_{{0i}}$ is individual variation on the intercept, ${\gamma}_{{10}}$ is the population average value for the fixed effect of time, ${\zeta}_{{1i}}$ is individual variation on the fixed effect of time and ${\varepsilon}_{{ij}}$ is the error term.Footnote 15 To optimise the model for the data, we employed higher-order polynomial functions, and allowed individuals to vary on each term only when those terms reached significance according to chi-square likelihood ratio tests (Chen et al. Reference Chen, Zhang, McCollum and Wayland2017; Chen & Li Reference Chen and Li2021, inter alia). In Mandarin languages, a tone-bearing unit, which is assumed to be the syllable, the rhyme or the nucleus, has been widely argued to be associated with at most three tonal targets (Bao Reference Bao1990, Reference Bao1992; Duanmu Reference Duanmu1994, inter alia). Therefore, the most complex tones can only have one change of direction, which will produce U-shaped contours, such as high-low-high and low-high-low. To conform to this observation, we only considered up to second-order functions to ensure that the final model is no more complex than a U-shaped contour. Also, orthogonal polynomials were used to make sure that the linear and quadratic terms were not correlated (Mirman Reference Mirman2017). After optimising the model by including all significant terms, we first treated underlying Tone 3 and derived Tone 3 as the same and modelled them as one single contour to get Model 1. Then we built models that treat them as different, namely, models that include a tone sandhi condition (underlying Tone 3 vs. derived Tone 3) to do model comparison. Based on Model 1, tone sandhi condition is first allowed to affect only intercept to get Model 2. Then tone sandhi condition is allowed to affect both intercept and linear term to get Model 3. Finally, tone sandhi condition is allowed to affect all fixed effects, which include intercept, linear term and quadratic term, and the outcome is Model 4. A chi-square likelihood ratio test was used to determine whether two minimally different models differ significantly.

The result shows that the difference between underlying Tone 3 and derived Tone 3 is in fact supported by model comparisons. The addition of a tone sandhi condition improves the model on the intercept as shown by comparing Model 1 and Model 2 ( $\chi ^2(1)=331.81$ , $p<0.01$ ), on the linear term as shown by comparing Model 2 and Model 3 ( $\chi ^2(1)=118.34$ , $p<0.01$ ) and on the quadratic term as shown by comparing Model 3 and Model 4 ( $\chi ^2(1)=14.99$ , $p<0.01$ ). Figure 4 shows how the full model (Model 4) fits the observed data. The parameter estimates for the full model are summarised in Table 3.

Figure 4 Observed data and growth curve model fits for derived and underlying Tone 3 (error bars indicate standard error).

Table 3 Parameter estimates of the full model with the assumption of tone sandhi affecting every fixed effect (baseline: derived Tone 3).

Moreover, the effect size of incomplete neutralisation is large in Tone 1 sandhi. The mean difference in f0 between underlying Tone 3 and derived Tone 3 across all steps is 18 Hz, which is more than two times the just-noticeable difference (JND) of f0 value (7 Hz) for Mandarin speakers (Jongman et al. Reference Jongman, Qin, Zhang and Sereno2017). Furthermore, across the last 10 steps (steps 11–20), the f0 difference is over 22 Hz, which is more than three times the JND. The f0 difference (f0 of derived Tone 3 minus f0 of underlying Tone 3) of each step is summarised in Table 4. Recall that the underlying premise of those who criticise the small effect size of incomplete neutralisation is that only if the differences were robust and large in size, the existence of such an effect should be accepted as functionally relevant.Footnote 16 According to that standard, Huai’an Tone 1 sandhi is clearly a case of phonetically incomplete neutralisation.

Table 4 f0 difference of each step in Experiment 1 (Tone 1).

5 Experiment 2: Tone 4 sandhi

To show that the pattern is not unique to Tone 1 sandhi, and to extend the scope of the current study, we ran a second experiment on Tone 4 sandhi process in Huai’an.

5.1 Participants

We recruited 20 native speakers of Huai’an Mandarin, again via personal relationships in Huai’an City. The age range was from 33 to 57 years old. Again, to minimise the influence of Standard Mandarin, we avoided younger speakers in this study. Among them, 16 self-identified as female, and 4 as male. Five of these speakers had also participated in Experiment 1. The interval between the two experiments was about 7 months; the five participants from Experiment 1 failed to guess, and were not told, the purpose of Experiment 2. As in Experiment 1, all the participants were born and raised in Huai’an City. Other speakers had not participated in any linguistic studies before or heard about the concept of incomplete neutralisation.

5.2 Stimuli

The stimuli were organised in the same way as in Experiment 1. The four sets of trisyllabic sentences are shown in (7), and the full stimulus list is summarised in Appendix B.

As with Experiment 1, the crucial comparison is between two tones in the second syllable, namely the underlying Tone 3 in (7b) and the derived Tone 3 as in the first possibility in (7d). This comparison allows us to control for the surface context, while also establishing that the two tones are indeed categorical Tone 3s, since they trigger Tone 3 sandhi on the preceding tone. Furthermore, as mentioned previously, the comparison allows us to exclude the possibility that any identified incomplete phonetic neutralisation pattern arises as a result of averaging the outcomes of an optional phonological process.

The set of possibilities also allows us to look at an underlying Tone 4 in roughly the same surface context, as in the second possibility in (7c), for visual comparison.

Each participant produced four repetitions of 20 test sentences at a natural speech rate with 20 fillers, which means that each participant read a total of 160 sentences.

5.3 Procedure

The procedure was identical to that of Experiment 1.

5.4 Measurement

The recordings were manually annotated by the first author but with a somewhat different scheme. For this experiment, both the first and second syllables were marked. The first syllable was marked to confirm that derived Tone 3 can in fact trigger Tone 3 sandhi on this syllable. An example is shown in Figure 5. The annotation file had five tiers in total. The criteria for marking vowels and syllables remained the same. The first tier marked the vowel of the syllable. All other tiers marked the whole second syllable to index phonological information and recording quality. The second tier indicated the position of the syllable inside the sentence, where a first syllable was marked ‘1’ and a second syllable was marked ‘2’. The third tier contained the pinyin romanisation of the whole sentence followed by the underlying tone of the syllable. The fourth tier marked whether the syllable underwent tone sandhi. And, the last tier indicated the quality of the recording. Similar to the previous experiment, we only used productions from recordings that were marked ‘good’. The f0 extraction, normalisation and visualisation processes are identical to those in the previous experiment.

Figure 5 Annotation scheme of Experiment 2 (Tone 4).

5.5 Results and statistical modelling

The number of tokens for each possible combination of underlying representation and surface representation is summarised in Table 5. The application rate of Tone 3 sandhi before underlying Tone 3 is 94.8%, while the application rate before derived Tone 3 is 24.2%.Footnote 17 Seventy-nine tokens were not marked as ‘good’ and excluded, which accounts for 5.0% of all test stimuli.

Table 5 Number of tokens for each UR and SR combination in Experiment 2.

The z-score transformed f0 contours on the crucial second syllable are shown in Figure 6. Again, the crucial comparison is between a derived Tone 3 and an underlying Tone 3 after a derived Tone 2 in the same surface context. We also present the tone contour for an underlying Tone 4 in the same surface context for visual comparison with the two crucial Tone 3s.

Figure 6 Comparison of second-syllable contours in Experiment 2 (Tone 4; error bars indicate standard error).

Based on visual inspection of the data, the pattern seems to be different from the case of Tone 1 sandhi. The derived Tone 3 seems to start like an underlying Tone 4,Footnote 18 instead of like an underlying Tone 3 as in Experiment 1. Furthermore, the derived Tone 3 gradually deviates from underlying Tone 4 through the whole contour; note that this is in contrast to Experiment 1, where the derived Tone 3 ended up at a value almost identical to the underlying Tone 1. However, the contour shape of the derived Tone 3 is again close to that of an underlying Tone 3, as in Experiment 1. Despite the difference, incomplete phonetic neutralisation is again clearly observed in the comparison between underlying Tone 3 and derived Tone 3.Footnote 19

The modelling method remains the same as in Experiment 1, and four models are generated. The observation of incomplete phonetic neutralisation is again supported by model comparisons. The addition of a tone sandhi condition improves the model on the intercept as shown by comparing Model 1 and Model 2 ( $\chi ^2(1)=1,429.23$ , $p<0.01$ ), the linear term as shown by comparing Model 2 and Model 3 ( $\chi ^2(1)= 66.22$ , $p<0.01$ ) and the quadratic term as shown by comparing Model 3 and Model 4 ( $\chi ^2(1)=32.67$ , $p<0.01$ ). Figure 7 shows how the full model with the assumption of tone sandhi affecting every fixed effect fits the observed data. And, the parameter estimates for full model are summarised in Table 6.

Figure 7 Observed data and growth curve model fits for derived and underlying Tone 3 (error bars indicate standard error).

Table 6 Parameter estimates of the full model with the assumption of tone sandhi affecting every fixed effect (baseline: derived Tone 3).

Again, the effect size of incomplete neutralisation is also large in Tone 4 sandhi. The mean difference in f0 between underlying Tone 3 and derived Tone 3 across all steps is 17 Hz, which is more than two times the JND of f0 value (7 Hz) for Mandarin speakers (Jongman et al. Reference Jongman, Qin, Zhang and Sereno2017). Also, across the last 11 steps (steps 9–20), the f0 difference is over 21 Hz, which is more than three times the just noticeable difference. The f0 difference (f0 of derived Tone 3 minus f0 of underlying Tone 3) of each step is summarised in Table 7. Therefore, the case of Huai’an Tone 4 sandhi can also be safely identified as phonetically incomplete neutralisation, and not susceptible to the criticism of a small effect size.

Table 7 f0 Difference of each step in Experiment 2 (Tone 4).

The coding in Experiment 2 also allowed us to answer another question that we did not answer for Experiment 1. In Experiment 1, we impressionistically coded whether or not the first syllable was in fact subject to Tone 3 sandhi. One could have argued that this impressionistic coding could have been inaccurate, and was based on a perceptual bias of the annotator (first author). To address this concern, it would have been optimal if we could have shown through phonological behaviour that the derived Tone 2 is indeed phonologically identical to underlying Tone 2. Although historically Tone 2 sandhi (Tone 2 + Tone 2 → Tone 3 + Tone 2) existed in Huai’an (Wang & Kang Reference Wang and Kang2012), this tone sandhi rule was not observed in our fieldwork in early 2020, probably because of influence from the standard language, as is generally observed in other languages (Labov Reference Labov1963; Milroy Reference Milroy2001, inter alia). And, no researchers before have tested if derived Tone 2 can trigger another tone sandhi process. Therefore, we cannot verify if the derived Tone 2 can trigger Tone 2 sandhi like an underlying Tone 2. Furthermore, we are not aware of any other phonological processes in the language that are triggered by Tone 2. As a result, it is not possible to establish Tone 2 category by phonological behaviour in Huai’an and we turn to provide phonetic evidence for the Tone 2 identity of the derived rising tone.

To make some inroads into the question of the phonological nature of the (putatively) derived Tone 2 in initial position, in Experiment 2, we also annotated the first syllable, and are therefore able to observe the f0 contours for derived Tone 2 (from underlying Tone 3) and compare it to an underlying Tone 2 to see if the impressionistic coding was appropriate. The tone contours of the z-score transformed f0 for the relevant first syllables are shown in Figure 8. For comparison, we also present the tone contour for an underlying Tone 3 on the first syllable that comes from a derived Tone 3 failing to trigger Tone 3 sandhi on the preceding syllable. By doing so, a three-way visual comparison is possible at the position of the first syllable under the same phonological environment, that is, before derived Tone 3.

Figure 8 Comparison of first-syllable contours in Experiment 2 (Tone 4; error bars indicate standard error).

Based on the visual inspection of the data, the derived Tone 2 that undergoes Tone 3 sandhi with reference to the following derived Tone 3 is phonetically highly similar to an underlying Tone 2 with regard to the f0 contour. Both derived Tone 2 and underlying Tone 2 f0 contours are phonetically very different from underlying Tone 3. Furthermore, as with the other tone sandhi processes discussed in this article, there is incomplete phonetic neutralisation of the derived Tone 2 (from an underlying Tone 3) and the underlying Tone 2 in the first syllable. With the modelling method introduced in §4.5, the addition of a tone sandhi condition improves the model on the quadratic term as shown by comparing Model 3 and Model 4 ( $\chi ^2(1)=4.96$ , $p=0.03$ ), but not on the intercept as shown by comparing Model 1 and Model 2 ( $\chi ^2(1)=2.16$ , $p=0.14$ ) or the linear term as shown by comparing Model 2 and Model 3 ( $\chi ^2(1)=1.10$ , $p=0.29$ ). Figure 9 shows how the full model (Model 4) with the assumption of tone sandhi affecting every fixed effect fits the observed data. And, the parameter estimates for the full model are summarised in Table 8.

Figure 9 Observed data and growth curve model fits for derived and underlying Tone 2 before derived Tone 3 (error bars indicate standard error).

Table 8 Parameter estimates of the full model with the assumption of tone sandhi affecting every fixed effect (baseline: derived Tone 2).

However, consistent with our larger claim, this should not be interpreted as incomplete phonological neutralisation. The mean difference in f0 between underlying Tone 2 and derived Tone 2 across all steps is only 1 Hz, which is much lower than the JND of f0 value (7 Hz) for Mandarin speakers (Jongman et al. Reference Jongman, Qin, Zhang and Sereno2017). The f0 difference (f0 of underlying Tone 2 minus f0 of derived Tone 2) of each step is summarised in Table 9. This indicates that native speakers of Huai’an may not be able to distinguish underlying versus derived Tone 2s and therefore are likely to analyse them as belonging to the same phonological category. It is worth noting that an assumption has been made here that a phonetic difference that is much smaller than or around JND means phonologically complete neutralisation, and a phonetic difference that is much bigger than the JND is compatible with both phonologically complete neutralisation (as in Huai’an Tone 1 and Tone 4 sandhis) and phonologically incomplete neutralisation. We acknowledge that some previous studies on incomplete neutralisation have shown that phonetic differences that are smaller than the relevant JND are still perceptually distinguishable (Port & O’Dell Reference Port and O’Dell1985; Warner et al. Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004, inter alia). However, the substantial phonetic difference between derived Tone 2 and underlying Tone 3 and the phonetic similarity between derived Tone 2 and underlying Tone 2 are difficult to account for by any mechanism known to us other than Tone 3 sandhi – it cannot simply be random variation or a coarticulatory change. Therefore, the impressionistic coding was in our opinion appropriate.

Table 9 f0 Difference of each step for first syllable in Experiment 2 (Tone 2).

To summarise the results of Experiment 2, we showed, using the feeding interaction between Tone 4 sandhi and Tone 3 sandhi, that the Tone 4 sandhi results in a phonological completely derived Tone 3. Despite this phonologically complete neutralisation, we observed a (rather large) incomplete neutralisation between the derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3 in the same surface tonal context. The experiment therefore replicates the results of Experiment 1.

6 Discussion

This article offers two clear cases of incomplete neutralisation based on data from Huai’an high-register tone sandhi processes. We observed robust phonetic differences (with large effect sizes) between a derived Tone 3 and an underlying Tone 3 in two independent experiments. This indicates that the observed effect is not likely to be a ‘false positive’ or functionally unimportant. Moreover, the Huai’an cases avoid any potential interference of orthography by presenting stimuli in Chinese characters. Therefore, some previous criticisms related to experimental design and the interpretation of data do not apply to the current Huai’an evidence.

A crucial aspect of the article is that we first established that the relevant tone sandhi processes are in fact phonological processes. To establish this fact, we look at the phonological behaviour of the derived tones, which to us is the best way of establishing phonological representations. More specifically, we looked at cases of tone sandhi that had feeding interactions, namely high-register tone sandhis including Tone 1 sandhi (Experiment 1) and Tone 4 sandhi (Experiment 2) feeding Tone 3 sandhi in Huai’an Mandarin. This establishes the fact that the Tone 1 and Tone 4 sandhi processes are indeed cases of phonological neutralisation. Despite this, we observed incomplete phonetic neutralisation between underlying Tone 3 and derived Tone 3s stemming from the two tone sandhi processes. Consequently, our results establish the fact that phonologically complete neutralisation can still be phonetically incomplete.

6.1 The phonological representation of Mandarin tone

It is worth noting that the interpretation of Huai’an tone sandhi cases as incomplete neutralisation relies on the general consensus that a Mandarin tone is a single phonological unit even though it is realised phonetically as a tonal contour (Yip Reference Yip1989; Bao Reference Bao1990, Reference Bao1992, inter alia). Under this view, it is not phonologically possible for part of a Mandarin contour tone to neutralise while another part of the tone remains unchanged. Perhaps the most convincing evidence for this single-phonological-unit representation in Mandarin languages comes from contour tone spreading. The most discussed case is undoubtedly Danyang (Chan Reference Chan1991; Chen Reference Chen1991; Yip Reference Yip1989; data from Lü Reference Lü1980). The pattern of interest is given in (8):

According to Yip’s (Reference Yip1989) analysis, in these cases, a falling tone is associated with the first syllable, and a rising tone is associated with the last syllable. Then the falling tone spreads rightwards over the domain as one single unit. If the falling tone is not a unit in phonology, one would not expect the whole contour to spread, but only the low tone at its right edge. A similar phenomenon of tone spreading is also found in Changzhi (Hou Reference Hou1983). It is worth noting that Duanmu (Reference Duanmu1994) challenges the above evidence by pointing out that contour tone spreading examples are only found in two languages and restricted to certain morphosyntactic structures. However, since Changzhi City and Danyang City are geographically far away from each other (roughly 734 km apart), tone spreading may be discovered in more languages and potentially more morphosyntactic structures. To summarise, despite dispute, the tone spreading pattern itself offers strong support for phonological contour tone. It is also worth pointing out that despite disagreement with the single-unit analysis of contour tones, Duanmu (Reference Duanmu1994) claims that tone sandhi results in a categorical change, which is argued in this article to support the interpretation of incomplete neutralisation in Huai’an.

With the above phonological viewpoint of tonal representations as backdrop, in Huai’an, the fact that both derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3 can trigger Tone 3 sandhi suggests that a derived Tone 3 is phonologically identical to underlying tone 3. In fact, to the best of our knowledge, we are not aware of any Mandarin languages where only underlying Tone 3 triggers Tone 3 sandhi, and not derived Tone 3 – this correlation would be accounted for by phonological neutralisation. However, a phonetic difference on any part of the contour between a derived contour tone and its underlying counterpart indicates phonetic incomplete neutralisation of the whole contour tone unit. In the case of Tone 1 and Tone 4 sandhis in Huai’an, there is a clear phonetic difference at the tonal offset position as shown in Experiment 1 and 2.

Based on the above, we would like to explicitly acknowledge that our claims in the article about incomplete phonetic neutralisation in the face of complete phonological neutralisation are contingent on the phonological representations we have assumed. As we see it, it cannot be any other way. The argument for incomplete neutralisation in any language depends on a certain set of assumed phonological representations. For example, in German, the interpretation of incomplete neutralisation depends on the devoicing rule actually resulting in a [ $-$ voice] feature (or equivalent). If the devoicing process results in some other phonological representation with similar phonetics, then the whole issue of incomplete neutralisation vanishes, and there is no need to entertain any more gradience in the phonological system to explain the observed phonetic patterns. In fact, a version of such a featural account is implied by Hale et al. (Reference Hale, Kissock and Reiss2007), who argue that language-specific phonetics can in fact be accounted for by different phonological feature combinations. Similarly, in Huai’an, it is possible to explain what is observed in the phonetics by changing or adding new phonological representations, but then of course independent evidence of the same representations in the language or in other related languages generally needs to be provided; otherwise it becomes an ad hoc, and therefore unjustified, claim. More generally, any set of representations or computations cannot simply be post hoc accounts of the data/patterns but need to be independently justified claims.

6.2 Desiderata for any explanation for incomplete neutralisation

With the two clear cases of incomplete neutralisation, the next step is naturally the explanation for incomplete neutralisation. Due to the limitation of the current study, the exact source of incomplete neutralisation cannot be pinpointed. However, we would like to lay out the desiderata that we think any explanation of incomplete neutralisation must achieve and illustrate the problems with previous explanations alongside.

First, to ensure the priority of a relatively simple theoretical model, explanations that can solve the problem while retaining a relatively simple phonological model should be considered first (Occam’s razor/the law of parsimony). Consequently, if independently needed performance mechanisms have the potential to account for the observation of incomplete phonetic neutralisation, they should be prioritised. Consistent with this principle, in the current study, the difference in Tone 3 sandhi application rates is assigned to independently needed performance factors of phonological planning, and therefore there is no need to complicate our understanding of the relevant phonological (tonal) representations. For the explanation of incomplete neutralisation, beyond previously identified factors such as orthography and task effects, the best performance factors in our opinion that need to be explored further are again phonological planning (Wagner Reference Wagner2012; Tanner et al. Reference Tanner, Sonderegger and Wagner2017; Kilbourn-Ceron & Goldrick Reference Kilbourn-Ceron and Goldrick2021) and cascaded activation of morphemes during production (Goldrick & Blumstein Reference Goldrick and Blumstein2006). If they are able to account for the patterns, we would be able to maintain a much simpler and, consequently, more predictive phonological theory.

The second challenge (9b) facing theories of incomplete neutralisation is the systematic disparity in effect sizes. Any proposed theory should explain among the observed cases why effect sizes of incomplete neutralisation are rather small in devoicing processes (as in German, Dutch, Russian, etc.), but can be quite large as in Huai’an tone sandhis or Japanese vowel lengthening. Moreover, the proposed explanation should also account for the newly found disparity in effect sizes within a single phonological process as in two Huai’an tone sandhis. In Experiment 1, the effect size is very small at the tonal onset position, as shown in Table 4, and becomes quite large as the contour progresses. A similar pattern is also found in Experiment 2, as shown in Table 7. A model that can simply account for a variety of effect sizes misses the systematic nature among different neutralisation processes and within a single time-varying neutralisation process.

The third challenge (9c) is that the proposed explanation should not only predict cases of incomplete neutralisation where the derived category is phonetically close to an underlying category (and in fact, between the phonetic manifestation of two underlying categories – its own UR and the phonological representation it is putatively changing to), but also avoid predicting cases of ‘over-neutralisation’ where the degree of application is beyond the phonetic distribution of the underlying category it is neutralising to. To return to the case of German devoicing, under the scenario of incomplete neutralisation, the phonetic cues of derived voiceless stops fall between underlying voiceless stops and underlying voiced stops. In the scenario of over-neutralisation, the phonetic cues of underlying voiceless stops would fall between derived voiceless stops and underlying voiced stops. However, only incomplete neutralisation has been observed in examined languages including Huai’an. This observation would be particularly problematic for purely exemplar representations (Brown & McNeill Reference Brown and McNeill1966; Bybee Reference Bybee1994; Goldinger Reference Goldinger1996, Reference Goldinger, Johnson and Mullennix1997; Port & Leary Reference Port and Leary2005; Roettger et al. Reference Roettger, Winter, Grawunder, Kirby and Grice2014, inter alia). Many previous theories account for the absence of over-neutralisation by proposing some mechanism whereby phonetically incomplete neutralisation is simply intermediate between two representations as it results from a blend of all phonetic cues of two distinct representations (Gafos & Benus Reference Gafos and Benus2006; van Oostendorp Reference van Oostendorp2008; Smolensky et al. Reference Smolensky, Goldrick and Mathis2014; Braver Reference Braver2019).Footnote 20 Either such theories are not specific enough, or other independently needed mechanisms must be incorporated to capture the systematic disparity in effect sizes in (9b).

The fourth challenge (9d) that any theory of incomplete neutralisation faces is to explain how a feeding interaction is possible when the derived representation still incompletely neutralises with the element that triggers the process. In the case of Huai’an, the Tone 3 output of the high-register tone sandhi processes can feed the low-register Tone 3 sandhi process as in (3) despite incompletely neutralising with underlying Tone 3 in the phonetics. Any categorical theory of phonological representations naturally accounts for this as process/rule interactions. Of course, it is possible for a theory of gradient phonological representations to do so too; however, to assess the effectiveness of such a theory, one needs to grapple with the specifics of the representations and computations proposed. To return to the Tone 3 sandhi application rate difference, if one were to propose that the differential application rates are a consequence of gradient phonological representations, where phonetic proximity triggers application of a process, then one has to address two things. First, why do we see the gradience in application rates with the derived category but not with the underlying category, though both vary in terms of phonetic manifestations? Second, we need to ensure that other phonetically similar sounds do not trigger the process too (9e). For example, in German, though both voiced obstruents and sonorants are phonetically voiced, only obstruents devoice at the end of a prosodic word. One may grant that the distinction between obstruents and sonorants is a difference in phonological representations; however, by making use of such distinction, a view of category is implicitly implemented.

We raise these challenges here to move the goalpost in a constructive direction on the debate about incomplete neutralisation. Given the above desiderata, we believe that previous explanations are not perfectly satisfying, and therefore the phenomenon of incomplete neutralisation remains an open problem.

7 Conclusion

The primary goal of this article is to offer two clear cases of incomplete neutralisation using data from Huai’an. Our results suggest that incomplete phonetic neutralisation can in fact have a large effect size, and more importantly that the phenomenon does not automatically reflect (gradient) phonological representations. Furthermore, echoing the general advice of Roettger et al. (Reference Roettger, Winter, Grawunder, Kirby and Grice2014), we would like to encourage more work on the topic and on our particular claim, since the acceptance of any phenomenon should not be based on a single study or a single language, and only by accumulating converging evidence from different methodologies can we be more certain of it.

Finally, the phenomenon of incomplete neutralisation highlights a discrepancy between the Standard generative view of phonology (Kenstowicz Reference Kenstowicz1994; Pierrehumbert Reference Pierrehumbert, Gussenhoven and Warner2002), wherein the output of phonological computation (the surface phonological representation) uniquely feeds into a phonetics module, and the Classic generative view of phonology, where phonology is seen as knowledge (Chomsky Reference Chomsky1965; Chomsky & Halle Reference Chomsky and Halle1965, Reference Chomsky and Halle1968, inter alia). Note that both views represent feed-forward models, where phonological computation feeds into phonetic manifestations, but phonetic manifestations cannot feed into phonological computation. However, per the latter view, linguistic performance is a multi-factorial problem, and linguistic knowledge (i.e. competence) is only one of the many factors involved (Chomsky Reference Chomsky1964, Reference Chomsky1965; Valian Reference Valian, Simon and Scholes1982; Schütze Reference Schütze1996; Warner et al. Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004, inter alia).Footnote 21

Our results from Huai’an tone neutralisations are problematic for the Standard generative view of phonology – if phonetic manifestations depend solely on the output of phonology and nothing else, then it is of course the case that such a view cannot account for cases where phonological neutralisation can still result in distinctness in the phonetics. However, our results are not in conflict with the Classic generative view of phonology. Phonology, per this latter view, is conceived of as grammatical knowledge that is used by a speaker to map a string of lexical items in a specific syntactic structure to articulations, and the use of this knowledge is affected by multiple other performance factors. Consequently, gradience in performance, and more specifically differences in speech production between two identical surface phonological representations, are not surprising. That is, there is no tension between incomplete phonetic neutralisation and categorical phonological neutralisation for the Classic generative view of phonology; instead, the actual mystery as per this view has always been with any observed cases of complete phonetic neutralisation stemming from a process of phonological neutralisation.

A Stimuli for Experiment 1 on Tone 1

B Stimuli for Experiment 2 on Tone 4

Supplementary material

The online supplement to this article provides figures showing the distribution of underlying Tone 1, derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3 in each step in Experiment 1, and the distribution of underlying Tone 4, derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3 in each step in Experiment 2. The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675723000192.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to our two research assistants Zhang Huaying and Yang Chenyuliang for their diligent work in this project. We also thank an associate editor of Phonology and three anonymous reviewers, whose comments and critiques have improved the quality and clarity of this article greatly. For helpful discussions, we thank Yen-Hwei Lin, Silvina Bongiovanni, Suzanne Wagner and members of the Michigan State University Phonology & Phonetics Group, as well as audiences at the 2020 Berkeley Linguistic Society Workshop and the Annual Meeting Phonology 2020, where the initial experimental results of this article were presented.

Competing interest

The author declares no competing interest.

Footnotes

1 To be succinct, here we use ‘underlying voiceless stop’ to mean surface voiceless stops that map from underlying voiceless stops, ‘derived voiceless stop’ to mean surface voiceless stops that map from underlying voiced stops, and ‘underlying voiced stops’ to mean surface voiced stops that map from underlying voiced stops. We will use the terms ‘underlying’ and ‘derived’ in the same fashion to describe stops and tones for the rest of the article.

2 While this conception of phonology is relatively standard in our opinion, it is actually quite different from the Classic generative view of phonology where phonology is seen as knowledge. We return to this issue in §7, where we point out that the latter notion has no trouble in accounting for facts related to incomplete neutralisation.

3 In support of our larger point in this article, we would like to point out that their observation in fact shows how powerful performance (i.e., non-phonological) factors can be in accounting for phonetic manifestations.

4 In Braver and Kawahara’s experiment, 10 out of 13 sets of stimuli were presented only in Chinese characters, while the other three sets included some kana (two in katakana and one in hiragana). In current usage, kana refers to two syllabaries where each character represents a mora, which in Japanese phonology may be an onset–vowel combination, a coda, or the second half of a long vowel. It is also worth noting that geminates and some long vowels are marked with diacritics in kana. Braver and Kawahara observed incomplete phonetic neutralisation for each set of stimuli.

5 To be clear, we are not claiming that the effect size must be large for incomplete neutralisation that is caused by grammatical knowledge. We are only recognising here that it is a reasonable concern that incomplete neutralisation with a small effect size may not even be captured by grammatical knowledge and therefore may not be able to pose any challenges to traditional formal phonology.

6 To be fair to them, this assumption is consistent with, and likely based on, what many phonologists have claimed in the prior literature.

7 As pointed out by an anonymous reviewer, Ernestus et al. (Reference Ernestus, Mybeth Lahey and Baayen2006) showed that Dutch has optional progressive voicing assimilation across word boundaries for obstruents. A word-final devoicing process feeds into the assimilation process and causes the initial obstruent of the next word to become voiceless, which suggests that the devoiced obstruent in the preceding word belongs to the same phonological category as its underlyingly voiceless counterpart. In relation to this, Ernestus et al. (Reference Ernestus, Mybeth Lahey and Baayen2006) showed in a separate experiment that there is incomplete phonetic neutralisation in word-final devoicing process in Dutch (see also Warner et al. Reference Warner, Jongman, Sereno and Kemps2004). Further study is needed to show that a devoiced obstruent that can trigger another devoicing process is actually incompletely neutralised in the phonetics. If this is indeed the case, Dutch will serve as another clear case of incomplete phonetic neutralisation under the condition of complete phonological neutralisation.

8 Note that Huai’an phonemic tones are different from those in Standard Mandarin. In Huai’an, Tone 1 is a high falling tone and Tone 4 is a high level tone. While in Standard Mandarin, Tone 1 is a high level tone (tone letter: 55) and Tone 4 is a falling tone (tone letter: 51). Tone 2s and Tone 3s in Huai’an and Standard Mandarin are largely similar.

9 Consistent with our use of the terms ‘underlying’ and ‘derived’ throughout this article, ‘underlying Tone 3’ means surface Tone 3 that maps from underlying Tone 3, and ‘derived Tone 3’ means surface Tone 3 that maps from underlying Tone 1 or Tone 4.

10 Depending on different proposed representations of the tones (Chen Reference Chen2000; Duanmu Reference Duanmu2007; Yip Reference Yip2002, to name a few), the motivation of tone sandhis can be different. As we will discuss in §6.1, our assumption is that each Mandarin tone is represented as a single phonological unit.

11 It has been noted in previous literature that tone sandhi patterns in many Mandarin languages are sensitive to prosodic structures (Zhang Reference Zhang1997; Chen Reference Chen2000; Duanmu Reference Duanmu2007; Wang & Lin Reference Wang, Lin and Jing-Schmidt2011, inter alia). Therefore, the observed difference in application rates between derived and underlying Tone 3s can also be cashed out in terms of planning difficulty related to prosodic phrases.

12 All the data presented in this article are available in the form of csv files with the extracted measurements at the permanent link https://osf.io/9qcbr. The repository also includes the R script used to analyse and plot the data.

13 As mentioned before, the difference in application rates does not necessarily inform us of any differences in phonological representations, since the difference is explicable by independently needed mechanisms, namely the difficulty in planning longer utterances.

14 To further address the concern that incomplete neutralisation patterns identified in this study may arise as a result of averaging the outcomes of an optional phonological process, the distributions of underlying Tone 1, derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 3 are shown for each time step in the Supplementary Material. Crucially, the derived Tone 3 distribution is generally unimodal, and distinct from the other two distributions, across the time steps. Thus, there is no evidence of an averaging artifact over optional surface representations for the derived Tone 3 case.

15 The individual variation terms, ${\zeta}_{{0i}}$ and ${\zeta}_{{1i}}$ , are akin to the random intercept by participant and random slope of time by participant.

16 We acknowledge that it is not entirely clear to us what is intended by the use of phrases such as ‘functional relevance’, since many aspects of linguistic behaviour might be important to the speaker/listener while not stemming from the grammar per se; however, we retain the phrase here to reflect the terminology in the subfield.

17 Again, we point out that the difference in application rates does not necessarily inform us of any differences in phonological representations. The difference can be accounted for by the difficulty in planning longer utterances. We also recognise that the application rate of Tone 3 derived from Tone 4 here is different from Tone 3 derived from Tone 1 in Experiment 1 (74.0%), and as one anonymous reviewer pointed out, such a difference could be used to argue for a phonological difference between the Tone 3s from different underlying sources. However, such a difference may simply be due to different groups of speakers in two independent experiments, or even more simply, the observed difference in effect sizes may simply be random variation, as would be expected between any two experiments measuring the same phenomenon. Future study is needed to compare Huai’an Tone 1 sandhi and Tone 4 sandhi on the same group of speakers in one single experiment. If the application rates are indeed replicable, an intriguing possibility that we note for the readers is that the phonetic difference between derived Tone 3 from Tone 1 and derived Tone 3 from Tone 4 may itself serve as a performance factor (related to the differential difficulty in implementing different tones, which in turn effects planning) that can account for the difference of application rate outside phonology. At the moment, the explanations based on planning difficulty are simply speculative, since it is not clear how phonological planning can affect tone sandhi application rate, and future study is needed to quantify the size of variation caused by planning difficulty.

18 As observed by one anonymous reviewer, derived Tone 3 from Tone 4 actually starts higher than underlying Tone 4, although the difference in raw pitch between derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 4 is very small (less than 5 Hz for the first four steps). As suggested by the reviewer, this pattern may be due to some effort to maintain contrast between derived Tone 3 and underlying Tone 4, since the remainder of derived Tone 3 is relatively high and close to underlying Tone 4.

19 We recognise that the underlying Tone 3 in Experiment 2 appears to be slightly phonetically different from its counterpart in Experiment 1, especially at the tonal offset position. Tone 3, which is usually used to represent low tone in Mandarin languages, generally involves creakiness. The small difference between underlying Tone 3s in Experiments 1 and 2 may be caused by inconsistency related to the Praat f0 estimation algorithms for creaky sounds. Whether it is due to this issue or to simple random variation is something we leave for further inquiry.

Again, to further address the concern that incomplete neutralisation patterns identified in this study may arise as a result of averaging the outcomes of an optional phonological process, the distributions of underlying Tone 4, underlying Tone 3, and derived Tone 3 are shown for each time step in the Supplementary Material. Again, crucially, the derived Tone 3 distribution is generally unimodal, and distinct from the other two distributions, across the time steps. Thus, there is no evidence of an averaging artifact over optional surface representations for the derived Tone 3 case.

20 Incomplete neutralisation is typically argued to be a blend of the surface representation and the underlying representation, or of the surface representation and a base representation, or of two co-activated surface representations.

21 We are not aware of any explicit argumentation that has ever been put forward in support of the Standard generative view over the Classic generative view. So, we are at a loss as to precisely when and, more importantly, why this change in viewpoints occurred. Here, we simply note the discrepancy.

References

Audacity Team (2019). Audacity. Version 2.3.2, retrieved 3 October 2019 from https://www.audacityteam.org/.Google Scholar
Badia Margarit, Antonio M. (1962). Gramática catalana, volume 1. Madrid: Editorial Gredos.Google Scholar
Baldwin, Scott A. & Hoffmann, John P. (2002). The dynamics of self-esteem: a growth-curve analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence 31. 101113.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bao, Zhiming (1990). On the nature of tone. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Bao, Zhiming (1992). Toward a typology of tone sandhi. BLS 18. 112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Mächler, Martin, Bolker, Ben & Walker, Steven (2021). Lme4: linear mixed-effects models using ‘Eigen’ and S4. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.Google Scholar
Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David (2021). Praat: doing phonetics by computer. Version 6.1.41, retrieved 25 March 2021 from http://www.praat.org/.Google Scholar
Braver, Aaron (2019). Modelling incomplete neutralisation with weighted phonetic constraints. Phonology 36. 136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braver, Aaron & Kawahara, Shigeto (2016). Incomplete neutralization in Japanese monomoraic lengthening. In Albright, Adam & Fullwood, Michelle A. (eds.) Supplemental proceedings of the 2014 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. 12 pp.Google Scholar
Brown, Roger & McNeill, David (1966). The ‘tip of the tongue’ phenomenon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 5. 325337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. (1994). A view of phonology from a cognitive and functional perspective. Cognitive Linguistics 5. 285305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chan, Marjorie K. (1991). Contour-tone spreading and tone sandhi in Danyang Chinese. Phonology 8. 237259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chao, Yuen-Ren (1930). A system of tone-letters. Le maître phonétique 30. 2427.Google Scholar
Charles-Luce, Jan & Dinnsen, Daniel A. (1987). A reanalysis of Catalan devoicing. JPh 15. 187190.Google Scholar
Chen, Matthew Y. (1991). An overview of tone sandhi phenomena across Chinese dialects. Journal of Chinese Linguistics Monograph Series 3. 111156.Google Scholar
Chen, Matthew Y. (2000). Tone sandhi: patterns across Chinese dialects. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Si & Li, Bin (2021). Statistical modeling of application completeness of two tone sandhi rules. Journal of Chinese Linguistics 49. 106141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chen, Si, Zhang, Caicai, McCollum, Adam G. & Wayland, Ratree (2017). Statistical modelling of phonetic and phonologised perturbation effects in tonal and non-tonal languages. Speech Communication 88. 1738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam (1964). The development of grammar in child language: formal discussion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 29. 3539.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, Noam (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris (1965). Some controversial questions in phonological theory. JL 1. 97138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, Noam & Halle, Morris (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Cohn, Abigail C. (1993). Nasalisation in English: phonology or phonetics. Phonology 10. 4381.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinnsen, Daniel A. & Charles-Luce, Jan (1984). Phonological neutralization, phonetic implementation and individual differences. JPh 12. 4960.Google Scholar
Dmitrieva, Olga (2005). Incomplete neutralization in Russian final devoicing: acoustic evidence from native speakers and second language learners. PhD dissertation, University of Kansas.Google Scholar
Du, Naiyan & Lin, Yen-Hwei (2021). Post-lexical tone 3 sandhi domain-building in Huai’an Mandarin: multiple domain types and free application. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics 27. 6.Google Scholar
Duanmu, San (1994). Against contour tone units. LI 25. 555608.Google Scholar
Duanmu, San (2007). The phonology of Standard Chinese. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunbar, Ewan (2013). Statistical knowledge and learning in phonology. PhD dissertation, University of Maryland, College Park.Google Scholar
Ernestus, Mirjam & Baayen, R. Harald (2006). The functionality of incomplete neutralization in Dutch: the case of past tense formation. In Goldstein, Louis M., Whalen, Douglas H. & Best, Catherine T. (eds.) Laboratory Phonology 8. Berlin, NY: Mouton de Gruyter. 2749.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ernestus, Mirjam, Mybeth Lahey, Femke Verhees & Baayen, R. Harald (2006). Lexical frequency and voice assimilation. JASA 120. 10401051.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, Fernanda & Swets, Benjamin (2002). How incremental is language production? Evidence from the production of utterances requiring the computation of arithmetic sums. Journal of Memory and Language 46. 5784.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fourakis, Marios & Iverson, Gregory K. (1984). On the ‘incomplete neutralization’ of German final obstruents. Phonetica 41. 140149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gafos, Adamantios I. & Benus, Stefan (2006). Dynamics of phonological cognition. Cognitive Science 30. 905943.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldinger, Stephen D. (1996). Words and voices: episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22. 11661183.Google Scholar
Goldinger, Stephen D. (1997). Words and voices: perception and production in an episodic lexicon. In Johnson, Keith & Mullennix, John W. (eds.) Talker variability in speech processing. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 3365.Google Scholar
Goldrick, Matthew & Blumstein, Sheila E. (2006). Cascading activation from phonological planning to articulatory processes: evidence from tongue twisters. Language and Cognitive Processes 21. 649683.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, Mark, Kissock, Madelyn & Reiss, Charles (2007). Microvariation, variation, and the features of universal grammar. Lingua 117. 645665.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hastie, Trevor & Tibshirani, Robert (1990). Generalized additive models. London: Chapman and Hall.Google Scholar
Hou, Jingyi (1983). Changzhi fangyan jilüe [Notes on the Changzhi dialect]. Fangyan [Dialect] 1983. 260274.Google Scholar
Huang, Borong & Liao, Xudong (2017). Xiandai hanyu [Contemporary Chinese]. Beijing: Higher Education Press.Google Scholar
Itô, Junko (1990). Prosodic minimality in Japanese. CLS 26. 213239.Google Scholar
Itô, Junko & Mester, Armin (1999). The phonological lexicon. In Tsujimura, Natsuko (ed.) The handbook of Japanese linguistics. Maiden, MA: Blackwell. 62100.Google Scholar
Itô, Junko & Mester, Armin (2003). Weak layering and word binarity. In Honma, Takeru, Okazaki, Masao, Tabata, Toshiyuki & Tanaka, Shin’ichi (eds.) A new century of phonology and phonological theory: a Festschrift for Professor Shosuke Haraguchi on the occasion of his sixtieth birthday. Tokyo: Kaitakusha. 2665.Google Scholar
Japan Broadcasting Corporation (1998). The Japanese Language Pronunciation and Accent Dictionary. Tokyo: NHK.Google Scholar
Jassem, Wiktor & Richter, Lutosława (1989). Neutralization of voicing in Polish obstruents. JPh 17. 317325.Google Scholar
Jiao, Lidong (2004). Huai’an fangyan de shengdiao fenxi [An analysis of tones in Huai’an dialect]. Master’s thesis, Tianjin Normal University.Google Scholar
Jongman, Allard, Qin, Zhen, Zhang, Jie & Sereno, Joan A. (2017). Just noticeable differences for pitch direction, height, and slope for Mandarin and English listeners. JASA 142. EL163EL169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jongman, Allard, Wang, Yue, Moore, Corinne B. & Sereno, Joan A. (2006). Perception and production of Mandarin Chinese tones. In Li, Ping, Tan, Lihai, Bates, Elizabeth & Tzeng, Ovid J. L. (eds.) Handbook of East Asian psycholinguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 209217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenstowicz, Michael J. (1994). Phonology in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kharlamov, Viktor (2012). Incomplete neutralization and task effects in experimentally-elicited speech: evidence from the production and perception of word-final devoicing in Russian. PhD dissertation, Université d’Ottawa/University of Ottawa.Google Scholar
Kilbourn-Ceron, Oriana & Goldrick, Matthew (2021). Variable pronunciations reveal dynamic intra-speaker variation in speech planning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 28. 13651380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Labov, William (1963). The social motivation of a sound change. Word 19. 273309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laplace, Pierre S. (1820). Théorie analytique des probabilités. Paris: Courcier.Google Scholar
Leben, William R. (1973). Suprasegmental phonology. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Li, Rong (1989). Hanyu fangyan de fenqu [The geographic division of Chinese dialects]. Fangyan [Dialect] 4. 19.Google Scholar
Lobanov, Boris M. (1971). Classification of Russian vowels spoken by different speakers. JASA 49. 606608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
, Shuxiang (1980). Danyang fangyan de shengdiao xitong [The tonal system of the Danyang dialect]. Fangyan [Dialect] 1980. 85122.Google Scholar
Manaster Ramer, Alexis (1996). A letter from an incompletely neutral phonologist. JPh 24. 477489.Google Scholar
Mascaró, Joan (1987). Underlying voicing recoverability of finally devoiced obstruents in Catalan. JPh 15. 183186.Google Scholar
Matsui, Mayuki (2015). Roshiago ni okeru yuuseisei no tairitsu to tairitsu no jakka: onkyo to chikaku [Voicing contrast and contrast reduction in Russian: acoustics and perception]. PhD dissertation, Hiroshima University.Google Scholar
McArdle, John J. & Nesselroade, John R. (2003). Growth curve analysis in contemporary psychological research. In Velicer, Wayne F. & Schinka, John A. (eds.) Handbook of psychology: research methods in psychology, volume 2. New York: Wiley. 447480.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, John J. (1986). OCP effects: gemination and antigemination. LI 17. 207263.Google Scholar
McCollum, Adam (2019). Gradient morphophonology: evidence from Uyghur vowel harmony. In Baek, Hyunah, Takahashi, Chikako & Yeung, Alex Hong-Lun (eds.) Proceedings of the 2019 Annual Meetings on Phonology, volume 7. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. 12 pp.Google Scholar
Mester, Armin (1990). Patterns of truncation. LI 23. 478485.Google Scholar
Milroy, James (2001). Language ideologies and the consequences of standardization. Journal of Sociolinguistics 5. 530555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirman, Daniel (2017). Growth curve analysis and visualization using R. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman and Hall/CRC.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mirman, Daniel, Dixon, James A. & Magnuson, James S. (2008). Statistical and computational models of the visual world paradigm: growth curves and individual differences. Journal of Memory and Language 59. 475494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mori, Yoko (2002). Lengthening of Japanese monomoraic nouns. JPh 30. 689708.Google Scholar
van Oostendorp, Marc (2008). Incomplete devoicing in formal phonology. Lingua 118. 13621374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. (2002). Word-specific phonetics. In Gussenhoven, Carlos & Warner, Natasha (eds.) Laboratory Phonology 7. Berlin, NY: Mouton de Gruyter. 101139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piroth, Hans G. & Janker, Peter M. (2004). Speaker-dependent differences in voicing and devoicing of German obstruents. JPh 32. 81109.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F. & Leary, Adam P. (2005). Against formal phonology. Lg 81. 927964.Google Scholar
Port, Robert F. & O’Dell, Michael L. (1985). Neutralization of syllable-final voicing in German. JPh 13. 455471.Google Scholar
Poser, William J. (1990). Evidence for foot structure in Japanese. Lg 66. 78105.Google Scholar
Prince, Alan & Smolensky, Paul (1993). Optimality Theory: constraint interaction in generative grammar . Technical Report 2, Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
R Core Team (2021). R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Version 1.4.1106, retrieved 25 March 2021 from https://www.rstudio.com.Google Scholar
Ramsey, S. Robert (1989). The languages of China. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Roettger, Timo B., Winter, Bodo, Grawunder, Sven, Kirby, James & Grice, Martine (2014). Assessing incomplete neutralization of final devoicing in German. JPh 43. 1125.Google Scholar
Schütze, Carson T. (1996). The empirical base of linguistics: grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shen, Xiaonan S. (1990). Tonal coarticulation in Mandarin. JPh 18. 281295.Google Scholar
Slowiaczek, Louisa M. & Dinnsen, Daniel A. (1985). On the neutralizing status of Polish word-final devoicing. JPh 13. 325341.Google Scholar
Slowiaczek, Louisa M. & Szymanska, Helena J. (1989). Perception of word-final devoicing in Polish. JPh 17. 205212.Google Scholar
Smolensky, Paul, Goldrick, Matthew & Mathis, Donald (2014). Optimization and quantization in gradient symbol systems: a framework for integrating the continuous and the discrete in cognition. Cognitive Science 38. 10021138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanner, James, Sonderegger, Morgan & Wagner, Michael (2017). Production planning and coronal stop deletion in spontaneous speech. Laboratory Phonology 8. 39 pp.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valian, Virginia (1982). Psycholinguistic experiment and linguistic intuition. In Simon, Thomas W. & Scholes, Robert J. (eds.) Language, mind, and brain. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 179188.Google Scholar
Wagner, Michael (2002). The role of prosody in laryngeal neutralization. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 42. 373392.Google Scholar
Wagner, Michael (2012). Locality in phonology and production planning. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics 22. 118.Google Scholar
Wagner, Valentin, Jescheniak, Jörg D. & Schriefers, Herbert (2010). On the flexibility of grammatical advance planning during sentence production: effects of cognitive load on multiple lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 36. 423440.Google ScholarPubMed
Wang, Chiung-Yao & Lin, Yen-Hwei (2011). Variation in Tone 3 Sandhi: the case of prepositions and pronouns. In Jing-Schmidt, Zhuo (ed.) Proceedings of the 23rd North American Conference on Chinese Linguistics. Eugene, OR: University of Oregon. 138155.Google Scholar
Wang, Yifeng & Kang, Jian (2012). Huai’an nanpian fangyan liangzizu lianxu biaodiao fenxi [An analysis of disyllabic tone sandhi in southern Huai’an dialect]. Journal of Shenyang Institute of Engineering (Social Sciences) 8. 357359.Google Scholar
Wang, Yuedong (1998). Mixed effects smoothing spline analysis of variance. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B: Statistical Methodology 60. 159174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, Natasha, Jongman, Allard, Sereno, Joan & Kemps, Rachèl (2004). Incomplete neutralization and other sub-phonemic durational differences in production and perception: evidence from Dutch. JPh 32. 251276.Google Scholar
Whalen, Douglas H. (1991). Infrequent words are longer in duration than frequent words. JASA 90. 23112311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whalen, Douglas H. (1992). Further results on the duration of infrequent and frequent words. JASA 91. 23392340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wickham, Hadley, Averick, Mara, Bryan, Jennifer, Chang, Winston, McGowan, Lucy D’Agostino, François, Romain, Grolemund, Garrett, Hayes, Alex, Henry, Lionel, Hester, Jim, Kuhn, Max, Pedersen, Thomas Lin, Miller, Evan, Bache, Stephan Milton, Müller, Kirill, Ooms, Jeroen, Robinson, David, Seidel, Dana Paige, Spinu, Vitalie, Takahashi, Kohske, Vaughan, Davis, Wilke, Claus, Woo, Kara & Yutani, Hiroaki (2019). Welcome to the Tidyverse. Journal of Open Source Software 4. 1686.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, Richard (2004). Factors of lexical competition in vowel articulation. In Local, John, Ogden, Richard & Temple, Rosalind (eds.) Papers in laboratory phonology VI. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 7587.Google Scholar
Xu, Yi (1994). Production and perception of coarticulated tones. JASA 95. 22402253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, Yi (1997). Contextual tonal variations in Mandarin. JPh 25. 6183.Google Scholar
Yang, Jilin (1995). Zhongguo zhongxiaoxue baikequanshu [encyclopedia for elementary school and middle school students]. Harbin: Harbin Publishing House.Google Scholar
Yip, Moira J. (1980). The tonal phonology of Chinese. PhD dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Yip, Moira J. (1989). Contour tones. Phonology 6. 149174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yip, Moira J. (2002). Tone. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhang, Ning (1997). The avoidance of the third tone sandhi in Mandarin Chinese. Journal of East Asian Linguistics 6. 293338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1 Description of phonemic tones in Huai’an.

Figure 1

Figure 1 Tonal contour of phonemic tones in Huai’an.

Figure 2

Figure 2 Annotation scheme of Experiment 1 (Tone 1).

Figure 3

Table 2 Number of tokens for each UR and SR combination in Experiment 1.

Figure 4

Figure 3 Comparison of second-syllable contours in Experiment 1 (Tone 1; error bars indicate standard error).

Figure 5

Figure 4 Observed data and growth curve model fits for derived and underlying Tone 3 (error bars indicate standard error).

Figure 6

Table 3 Parameter estimates of the full model with the assumption of tone sandhi affecting every fixed effect (baseline: derived Tone 3).

Figure 7

Table 4 f0 difference of each step in Experiment 1 (Tone 1).

Figure 8

Figure 5 Annotation scheme of Experiment 2 (Tone 4).

Figure 9

Table 5 Number of tokens for each UR and SR combination in Experiment 2.

Figure 10

Figure 6 Comparison of second-syllable contours in Experiment 2 (Tone 4; error bars indicate standard error).

Figure 11

Figure 7 Observed data and growth curve model fits for derived and underlying Tone 3 (error bars indicate standard error).

Figure 12

Table 6 Parameter estimates of the full model with the assumption of tone sandhi affecting every fixed effect (baseline: derived Tone 3).

Figure 13

Table 7 f0 Difference of each step in Experiment 2 (Tone 4).

Figure 14

Figure 8 Comparison of first-syllable contours in Experiment 2 (Tone 4; error bars indicate standard error).

Figure 15

Figure 9 Observed data and growth curve model fits for derived and underlying Tone 2 before derived Tone 3 (error bars indicate standard error).

Figure 16

Table 8 Parameter estimates of the full model with the assumption of tone sandhi affecting every fixed effect (baseline: derived Tone 2).

Figure 17

Table 9 f0 Difference of each step for first syllable in Experiment 2 (Tone 2).

Supplementary material: File

Du and Durvasula supplementary material

Du and Durvasula supplementary material
Download Du and Durvasula supplementary material(File)
File 871.8 KB