Hostname: page-component-5c6d5d7d68-ckgrl Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-08-09T22:24:26.709Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Amendment of traditional assessment measures for the negative symptoms of schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 January 2020

Aida Farreny*
Affiliation:
aParc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, CIBERSAM, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain bUnit for Social and Community Psychiatry, WHO Collaborating Centre for Mental Health Services Development, Queen Mary University of London, London, United Kingdom
Judith Usall
Affiliation:
aParc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, CIBERSAM, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
Jorge Cuevas-Esteban
Affiliation:
aParc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, CIBERSAM, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
Susana Ochoa
Affiliation:
aParc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, CIBERSAM, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
Gildas Brébion
Affiliation:
aParc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, CIBERSAM, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Barcelona, Spain
*
*Corresponding author at: Unit for Social and Community Psychiatry, Newham Centre for Mental Health, London E13 8SP, United Kingdom. E-mail address: a.farreny@qmul.ac.uk (A. Farreny).

Abstract

Schizophrenia research based on traditional assessment measures for negative symptoms appears to be, to some extent, unreliable. The limitations of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) have been extensively acknowledged and should be taken into account. The aim of this study is to show how the PANSS and the SANS conflate negative symptoms and cognition and to offer alternatives for the limitations found.

Methods

A sample of 117 participants with schizophrenia from two independent studies was retrospectively investigated. Linear regression models were computed to explore the effect of negative symptoms and illness duration as predictors of cognitive performance.

Results

For the PANSS, the item “abstract thinking” accounted for the association between negative symptoms and cognition. For the SANS, the “attention” subscale predicted the performance in verbal memory, but illness duration emerged as a stronger predictor than negative symptoms for outcomes of processing speed, verbal and working memory.

Conclusion

Utilizing alternative models to the traditional PANSS and SANS formats, and accounting for illness duration, provide more precise evidence on the relationship between negative symptoms and cognition. Since these measures are still extensively utilized, we recommend adopting more rigorous approaches to avoid misleading results.

Type
Original articles
Copyright
Copyright © 2017 European Psychiatric Association

1. Introduction

“My experience is what I agree to attend to. Only those items which I notice shape my mind.” – William James

During the last decade, there has been increasing interest in negative symptoms (NS) of schizophrenia together with a re-evaluation of the scales measuring them. Novel instruments have been developed although they have yet to be generally adopted, whilst studies based on traditional scales appear to be to some extent unreliable.

The characteristics of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS) [Reference Andreasen2] have been a matter of discussion over the last 20 years. For example, further PANSS-subscales including four, five, or six factors have been proposed with several studies underlining that five-factor models show an adequate reliability when tested in different subgroups of individuals with schizophrenia, confirming the suitability of this approach [Reference Wallwork, Fortgang, Hashimoto, Weinberger and Dickinson3, Reference Nicotra, Casu, Piras and Marchese4]. On the other hand, cross-sectional studies of the SANS identify three, four and five different symptom factors; and longitudinal research has replicated three factors [Reference Levine and Leucht5]. In particular, the NS subscale within the traditional PANSS consists of seven items tapping blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive/apathetic social withdrawal, difficulty in abstract thinking, lack of spontaneity, and stereotyped thinking. And the original SANS consists of 19 items representing five domains: affective flattering, alogia, avolition-apathy, anhedonia-asociality, and attention.

A number of studies have adapted these scales to provide the two dimensions of NS, Diminished Expression and Amotivation/Avolition. For the PANSS, Liemburg et al. [Reference Liemburg, Castelein, Stewart, van der Gaag, Aleman and Knegtering6] studied the two-factor structure for NS in early psychosis participants. These factors were named “core NS”, related to the expressive deficits, and “social emotive withdrawal”, described as social amotivation. Similarly, Fervaha et al. [Reference Fervaha, Foussias, Agid and Remington7] extended these findings to patients with chronic schizophrenia, calling the two factors “diminished expression” and “amotivation”; and comparable two-factor results were recently published by Lim and colleagues [Reference Lim, Lee, Lam, Rapisarda, Kraus and Keefe8]. For the SANS, similar factor models accounting for diminished expression and anhedonia-asociality have emerged. For example Sayers et al. [Reference Sayers, Curran and Mueser9] confirmed a general three factor approach for the SANS including “diminished expression”, “inattention-alogia” and “social amotivation”, while Kelley et al. studied primary and secondary negative symptoms of schizophrenia employing the two factor approach of “affective flattening” and “diminished amotivation” [Reference Kelley, van Kammen and Allen10].

Factor analysis studies have indicated that cognitive items in the PANSS and SANS do not cohere well with the other NS ratings [Reference Blanchard, Kring, Horan and Gur11], and cognitive deficit appears to be conceptually distinct from NS [e.g. [Reference Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg and Bowie12]. Possible confounding instances include items of “difficulty of abstract thinking” and “stereotyped thinking” in the PANSS, and the “attention” subscale in the SANS (See Blanchard and Cohen for a review [Reference Blanchard and Cohen13]). As an illustration, Bell et al. [Reference Bell and Mishara14] demonstrated that performance on neuropsychological tests was associated with the cognitive component of the PANSS (“abstract thinking” and “stereotyped thinking”) but not with other NS items within the PANSS. For the SANS, Vadhan et al. [Reference Vadhan, Serper, Harvey, Chou and Cancro15] found a correlation between the “attention” subscale and neuropsychological tasks which discriminated “attention” from the other SANS subscales. Similarly, Liemburg et al. [Reference Liemburg, Castelein, Stewart, van der Gaag, Aleman and Knegtering6] and Lim et al. [Reference Lim, Lee, Lam, Rapisarda, Kraus and Keefe8] reported an association between the “diminished expression” PANSS factor and cognition.

Both cognitive impairment and NS are formally considered as core features of schizophrenia contributing to poor functional and community outcomes (e.g. [Reference Green, Kern and Heaton16, Reference Milev, Ho, Arndt and Andreasen17]). The present study was motivated by the ongoing utilization of traditional approaches to the PANSS and the SANS albeit the limitations stated above. Our aim is to show possible misleading associations between negative symptoms and cognition when using the original PANSS and SANS factors, and to offer alternatives to overcome them while still utilizing the PANSS and the SANS.

Specifically, our aim is to illustrate how the traditional PANSS and SANS may perform differently on the associations between NS and cognition depending on the factor approaches utilized. Findings from a previous study by our group suggested that NS could hamper the expression of cognition on behavioural tasks and functional outcomes [Reference Farreny, Aguado, Ochoa, Haro and Usall18]. These findings were of interest since common theoretical backgrounds generally assume that cognition would have an effect on NS (e.g., [Reference Cella, Stahl, Morris, Keefe, Bell and Wykes19]) but not vice versa.

Finally, illness duration will be taken into account as a confounder variable since the study included chronic and institutionalized participants and this might perform a detrimental effect on both cognition and NS. Our hypothesis is that longer illness duration may have an impact on the association between NS and cognitive performance, particularly in hospitalized participants. A decline in cognition has been also reported after ten years of illness duration [Reference Øie, Sundet and Rund20] and in geriatric patients with schizophrenia [Reference Harvey, Silverman, Mohs, Parrella, White and Powchik21]. Likewise, chronicity and hypostimulating environments can cause secondary negative symptoms such as decreased spontaneity, reduced curiosity, reduced drive to interact and blunted affect [Reference Oshima, Mino and Inomata22, Reference Kirschner, Aleman and Kaiser23].

2. Method

2.1. Participants

Two samples of participants with schizophrenia were retrospectively studied. Both groups belonged to the same mental health services from Barcelona metropolitan area and were recruited in independent investigations. Group 1 involved outpatients recruited with the purpose of studying the efficacy of Cognitive Remediation group treatment [Reference Farreny, Aguado, Ochoa, Huerta-Ramos, Marsà and López-Carrilero24]. Group 2 included inpatients recruited to study cognitive impairment in schizophrenia [Reference Brébion, Stephan-Otto, Huerta-Ramos, Ochoa, Usall and Abellán-Vega25]. Both studies were approved by the Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu Ethics Committee.

Group 1- Sixty-two participants with a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder following DSM-IV criteria were recruited from Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu community services [Reference American Psychiatric Association26]. To verify the stability of the diagnosis we checked the medical histories to corroborate that the required DSM-IV criteria were appropriately described. Two cases were unconfirmed and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID; [Reference First, Spitzer, Gibbon and Williams27]) was utilized to verify their diagnoses. The participants included were between 18 and 65 years of age, with disease duration of over two years. Patients were excluded if they were suffering acute illness exacerbation that required hospitalization, had intellectual disability or neurological disorder, had switched antipsychotic drugs the month before the assessment, and/or had a diagnosis of alcohol or drug dependence within 6 months prior to inclusion. Initially, 70 participants referred by their community teams or rehabilitation services were assessed for eligibility. Of these, two were excluded for not meeting inclusion criteria (change of diagnosis to bipolar disorder and presence of learning disability), four refused consent, and two were not interested.

Group 2 – Fifty-five participants with schizophrenia were recruited from Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu inpatient services. The diagnosis was made by consensus on the basis of DSM-IV criteria by two experienced psychiatrists who used patient histories and chart reviews. Inclusion criteria were age between 18 and 65, fluency in Spanish, and the capacity to provide informed consent. Exclusion criteria were current or recent alcohol or drug abuse (DSM-IV criteria), organic mental disease, intellectual disability, history of brain injury, dementia, and current severe physical disease. Participants were hospitalized and had been receiving stabilized doses of antipsychotic medication over two weeks at the time of testing. Clinical records were reviewed thoroughly by the psychiatrist recruiting the participants (J Cuevas-Esteban) and only those inpatients meeting all inclusion criteria were asked to participate. The rates of consent were about 75% of those eligible to take part.

For both groups the antipsychotic medication included first-generation antipsychotics (clotiapine, fluphenazine, haloperidol, levomepromazine, zuclopenthixol) as well as second-generation (amisulpride, aripiprazole, clozapine, olanzapine, quetiapine, risperidone). Predominantly within Group 2, participants were taking a combination of two or more antipsychotic drugs and/or were administered benzodiazepines (clonazepam, diazepam, flunitrazepam, lorazepam, lormetazepam) and/or antidepressant (duloxetine, fluoxetine, paroxetine, trazodone) medication.

2.2. Measures

Group 1- The cognitive assessment included the following domains: Executive Function using the Behavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome (BADS) [Reference Wilson, Alderman, Burgess, Emslie and Evans28], which consists of six tests involving cognitive flexibility, inhibition of impulsive responses, planning and organization, working memory, and time-estimation capacity. Attention, processing speed, and cognitive flexibility were measured with The Trail Making Test forms A and B (TMT A; TMT B) [Reference Reitan and Wolfson29]. Verbal memory, both immediate and delayed, was assessed with the Logical Memory I and II subscales respectively, from the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS-III) [Reference Wechsler30].

Negative symptoms were measured with the Spanish validation of the PANSS [Reference Peralta Martín and Cuesta Zorita31]. The negative PANSS factors employed in this study were the original 3-factor approach by Kay et al. [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] including 7 items: blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive-apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity, difficulty in abstract thinking, and stereotyped thinking. Also used was the consensual 5-factor approach by Wallwork et al. [Reference Wallwork, Fortgang, Hashimoto, Weinberger and Dickinson3] including 6 items: blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive-apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity, and motor retardation.

Group 2- Cognition was evaluated through several tasks: Cognitive speed was measured using the WAIS-III [Reference Wechsler32] Digit Symbol Substitution Test (DSST). The time required to read the whole list of the colour reading part of the Stroop test [Reference Stroop33] was recorded to evaluate motor speed. Verbal recall (frequent and rare words) was assessed with 2 lists of words consisting of 16 high-frequency words and 16 low-frequency words [Reference Algarabel, Ruiz and Sanmartin34]. Working memory was measured through the WAIS-III Letter-number span using the total number of correct trials.

Negative symptoms were assessed by means of the Spanish version of the SANS [Reference Peralta and Cuesta35] following the traditional evaluation of 5 domains: affective flattening, alogia, avolition-apathy, anhedonia-asociality, and attention.

2.3. Statistical analysis

Linear regression models were computed to explore the effect of NS on the cognitive variables targeted. The dependent variables were the BADS, TMT A, TMT B, and the WMS-III subscales for Group 1. In contrast, the Digit Symbol Substitution Test, Stroop Test, memory of high-frequency and low-frequency words, and Letter-number span were the dependent variables for Group 2. Independent variables were NS measured with the PANSS and the SANS, respectively.

In a first step we computed the regression model with each NS scale for every cognitive outcome without modifications. For Group 1 this was done using both the PANSS (K) negative by Kay et al. and the PANSS (W) negative by Wallwork et al. For Group 2 we used the traditional SANS. Next, if the association between NS and cognitive outcomes resulted in p < 0.1 in the regression model, the negative factor was computed again without including the cognitive items (“abstract thinking” in the PANSS and “attention” in the SANS) to test them as separate predictors. Therefore, PANSS6 corresponded to the PANSS (K) negative excluding the item of “abstract thinking”, and SANS4 was used to refer the SANS without the “attention” subscale. No modifications were applied to the PANSS (W) negative since this factor already excludes the items of “abstract thinking” and “stereotyped thinking”. Finally, if the association between NS and the cognitive outcome was still significant, illness duration was controlled in the model. The statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS 17.0 [Reference IBM36].

Table 1 Descriptive characteristics by group.

3. Results

Descriptive characteristics for each group are shown in Table 1.

Group 1- Regression models using the PANSS negative factors by Kay et al. [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] and Wallwork et al. [Reference Wallwork, Fortgang, Hashimoto, Weinberger and Dickinson3] are shown in Table 2.

Results were different depending on the factor approach utilized for the PANSS. Following the original 3-factor model, Executive function and Visual and motor speed appeared to be predicted by NS, also showing a tendency towards association with delayed Logical memory (p = 0.08). When the item “abstract thinking” was accounted for separately in the model, the predictive value of negative symptoms over cognitive outcomes lost its significance. The case of the BADS was a clear illustration: the standardized regression coefficient (B) for the predictive value of the PANSS negative was initially −0.31, but when controlling for “abstract thinking”, the value was considerably reduced (B = −0.07) whilst the B coefficient for “abstract thinking” was −0.41.

Table 2 Regression models with the PANSS Negative (Group 1).

B*=Standardized regression coefficient (95% confidence interval); PANSS NEG6: blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive-apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity and stereotyped thinking.

In contrast, the negative factor by Wallwork et al. did not show any significant association with the cognitive outcomes assessed.

Group 2- Results from linear regression using the SANS are shown in Table 3. The regression model applied without controlling for covariables showed that levels of negative symptoms predicted the cognitive outcomes studied, with the exception of Verbal recall of rare words. In the second step, the SANS4 model accounting separately for the “attention” subscale demonstrated a tendency towards significance between Verbal recall of frequent words and “attention” (p = 0.05). In a third step, when illness duration was included in the model as a covariable, the association between Verbal recall of frequent words and “attention” proved stronger, but outcomes for Cognitive processing speed, Motor processing speed and Working memory became significantly predicted by illness chronicity. As an example, the SANS4 standardized B coefficient for Working memory was initially –0.31, but when controlling for “attention” and illness duration it became B = –0.05. However, the B coefficient for illness duration was –0.43.

Table 3 Regression models using the SANS (Group 2).

B*=Standardized regression coefficient (95% confidence interval). SANS4: Affective flattering, alogia, avolition-apathy, anhedonia-asociality.

4. Discussion

Our findings illustrate how traditional factor approaches to NS may actually conflate cognitive processes and negative symptoms (e.g., [Reference Blanchard, Kring, Horan and Gur11, Reference Daniel37]). Additionally, illness duration was demonstrated to predict cognitive performance within the sample of inpatients. According to the present study, the limitations found within the traditional PANSS and SANS can be addressed, so investigations employing these scales are encouraged to adopt more rigorous ways to discriminate NS.

4.1. The PANSS

There is extensive literature covering different factor approaches for the PANSS [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] (see Blanchard and Cohen [Reference Blanchard and Cohen13]). Current research has called for a 5-factor solution demonstrating more precise and homogeneous definition of symptom dimensions [Reference Nicotra, Casu, Piras and Marchese4]; the present research shows that the consensual PANSS Negative factor by Wallwork et al. [Reference Wallwork, Fortgang, Hashimoto, Weinberger and Dickinson3] including 6 items (blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive-apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity, and motor retardation) may be a convenient alternative to the original approach. The factor approach of Marder et al. [Reference Marder, Davis and Chouinard38] is among other popular options to fit PANSS data excluding the items of “abstract thinking” and “stereotyped thinking” from the NS domain. In this study, controlling for “abstract thinking” was enough to demonstrate the misleading association between NS and cognition when employing the Kay et al. [Reference Kay, Fiszbein and Opler1] approach to the PANSS supporting previous findings [Reference Bell, Lysaker, Milstein and Beam-Goulet39].

4.2. The SANS

In the case of the SANS this study was carried out involving chronic institutionalized participants. Both the “attention” subscale and illness duration were demonstrated to predict cognitive performance instead of NS. The “attention” subscale was associated with verbal memory; while chronicity predicted deficits in processing speed and working memory ratifying the impact of illness duration on cognition [Reference Øie, Sundet and Rund20, Reference Harvey, Silverman, Mohs, Parrella, White and Powchik21, Reference Rajji and Mulsant40]. There has been ample discussion about the best factor approach fitting the SANS data, with evidence suggesting that the inclusion of “attention” in ratings of NS is somewhat problematic [Reference Blanchard and Cohen13]. Research suggests the 3-factor approach (affective-flattering, asociality, and alogia/attentiveness) might be more parsimonious, and it showed better validity [Reference Sayers, Curran and Mueser9, Reference Levine and Leucht5]; but this approach includes items from the “attention” subscale and this may not resolve the link with cognition. We suggest taking the “attention” subscale into account especially when investigations aim to distinguish precisely between cognition and NS.

New measures for the assessment of NS have been developed following the NHIM consensus meeting on NS [Reference Kirkpatrick, Fenton, Carpenter and Marder41]. These are the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS) [Reference Horan, Kring, Gur, Reise and Blanchard42, Reference Kring, Gur, Blanchard, Horan and Reise43] and the Brief Negative Symptom Scale (BNSS) [Reference Kirkpatrick, Strauss, Nguyen, Fischer, Daniel and Cienfuegos44, Reference Strauss, Keller, Buchanan, Gold, Fischer and McMahon45]. These scales are a stimulating outcome and are promising for the field although they remain to be generally implemented. Nonetheless, recent studies have shown small to moderate associations between the CAINS and cognition, comparable to the associations found when utilizing other negative symptom scales [Reference Blanchard, Bradshaw, Garcia, Nasrallah, Harvey and Casey46]. The BNSS has also pointed to some overlap between diminished expression and cognition [Reference Hartmann-Riemer, Hager, Kirschner, Bischof, Kluge and Seifritz47]. On the whole, it seems necessary to further explore the association and shared pathophysiology between NS and cognition. According to the present results, NS would not have an effect on participants’ cognitive performance, but chronicity could have a negative impact on cognition in hospitalized patients. The distinction between NS and cognition has been cause for debate over several years. Some authors have argued that NS may be underpinned by cognitive deficits such as the impaired initiation of novel responses (e.g. [Reference Greenwood, Morris, Sigmundsson, Landau and Wykes48]) while others have suggested that patients with higher levels of NS have particular impairments in reasoning and executive function (e.g. [Reference Ventura, Hellemann, Thames, Koellner and Nuechterlein49]). However, several studies have failed to establish a relationship between NS and cognition, leading to the conclusion that they represent semi-autonomous disease processes (e.g. [Reference Kirkpatrick, Fenton, Carpenter and Marder41, Reference Foussias and Remington50]); and the correlation between cognition and NS may vary as a function of the definition of the NS construct [Reference Harvey, Koren, Reichenberg and Bowie12]. Therefore, research and clinical communities are still facing the challenging situation of properly recognising and targeting NS. Understanding NS means identifying them among other confounder variables, and being able to measure them in the most precise way. This matter has not been resolved yet, in particular with regard to their association with cognition.

This study has a number of limitations that need to ne noted. First, this is a retrospective analysis with two samples initially recruited for two different purposes. As such, cognitive and clinical measures employed were chosen according to the aims of each particular investigation so the measurement of cognitive variables was not performed with the same scales. In addition, some of the participants from Group 2 were chronic and had had years of institutionalization, which carries confusion with regard to the specific causes of their deficit (e.g., [Reference Harvey, Silverman, Mohs, Parrella, White and Powchik21, Reference Kirschner, Aleman and Kaiser23]). Third, the alternatives suggested to compensate for the cognitive items within the SANS and the PANSS may not to be exhaustive and we have not considered alternative models, for example the PANSS with four and six factors. Finally, some question marks have already arisen concerning the proposed factor approaches (e.g., reporting an association between cognition and NS although convenient modifications were adopted [Reference Liemburg, Castelein, Stewart, van der Gaag, Aleman and Knegtering6, Reference Lim, Lee, Lam, Rapisarda, Kraus and Keefe8]), casting some doubt on our findings and supporting the idea that NS and cognitive deficits may overlap and may not be independent to each other.

Despite these limitations, the present study has demonstrated that when relying on old but still widely used measures such as the PANSS and the SANS, it is advisable to avoid items too closely related to cognition. This is especially important in comprehending NS in schizophrenia, and in the development of effective targeted treatment approaches.

Conflicts of interest

None.

Acknowledgement

This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Footnotes

1

CIBERSAM: Centro de Investigación Biomédica En Red de Salud Mental (Centre for Biomedical Research and Mental Health Net, Spain).

References

Kay, SRFiszbein, AOpler, LAThe positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 1987; 13:261–76 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/13.2.261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, NCThe Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS): conceptual and theoretical foundations. Br J Psychiatry Suppl 1989; 4958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallwork, RSFortgang, RHashimoto, RWeinberger, DRDickinson, DSearching for a consensus five-factor model of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2012; 137:246–50 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.01.031.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicotra, ECasu, GPiras, SMarchese, GOn the use of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale in randomized clinical trials. Schizophr Res 2015; 165:181–7 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.04.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levine, SZLeucht, SPsychometric analysis in support of shortening the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2013; 23:1051–6 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2012.11.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liemburg, ECastelein, SStewart, Rvan der Gaag, MAleman, AKnegtering, HTwo subdomains of negative symptoms in psychotic disorders: established and confirmed in two large cohorts. J Psychiatr Res 2013; 47:718–25 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.01.024.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fervaha, GFoussias, GAgid, ORemington, GMotivational and neurocognitive deficits are central to the prediction of longitudinal functional outcome in schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2014; 130:290–9 10.1111/acps.12289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lim, JLee, S-ALam, MRapisarda, AKraus, MKeefe, RSE et al. The relationship between negative symptom subdomains and cognition. Psychol Med 2016; 46:2169–77 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291716000726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sayers, SCurran, PMueser, KFactor Structure and Construct Validity of the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms. Psychol Assess 1996; 8:269–80 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//1040-3590.8.3.269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, MEvan Kammen, DPAllen, DNEmpirical validation of primary negative symptoms: independence from effects of medication and psychosis. Am J Psychiatry 1999; 156:406–11 10.1176/ajp.156.3.406.Google ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, JJKring, AMHoran, WPGur, RToward the next generation of negative symptom assessments: the collaboration to advance negative symptom assessment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37:291–9 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq104.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, PDKoren, DReichenberg, ABowie, CRNegative symptoms and cognitive deficits: what is the nature of their relationship?. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32:250–8 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbj011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, JJCohen, ASThe Structure of Negative Symptoms Within Schizophrenia: Implications for Assessment. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32:238–45 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbj013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, MDMishara, ALDoes negative symptom change relate to neurocognitive change in schizophrenia? Implications for targeted treatments. Schizophr Res 2006; 81:1727http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2005.09.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vadhan, NPSerper, MRHarvey, PDChou, JCCancro, RConvergent validity and neuropsychological correlates of the schedule for the assessment of negative symptoms (SANS) attention subscale. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189:637–41 http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005053-200109000-00011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, MFKern, RSHeaton, RKLongitudinal studies of cognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: implications for MATRICS. Schizophr Res 2004; 72:4151http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milev, PHo, B-CArndt, SAndreasen, NCPredictive values of neurocognition and negative symptoms on functional outcome in schizophrenia: a longitudinal first-episode study with 7-year follow-up. Am J Psychiatry 2005; 162:495506 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.3.495.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farreny, AAguado, JOchoa, SHaro, JMUsall, JThe role of negative symptoms in the context of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2013; 150:5863http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.08.008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cella, MStahl, DMorris, SKeefe, ERSBell, MDWykes, TEffects of cognitive remediation on negative symptoms dimensions: exploring the role of working memory. Psychol Med 2017; 4:19http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717000757.Google Scholar
Øie, MSundet, KRund, BRNeurocognitive decline in early-onset schizophrenia compared with ADHD and normal controls: evidence from a 13-year follow-up study. Schizophr Bull 2010; 36:557–65 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harvey, PDSilverman, JMMohs, RCParrella, MWhite, LPowchik, P et al. Cognitive decline in late-life schizophrenia: a longitudinal study of geriatric chronically hospitalized patients. Biol Psychiatry 1999; 45:3240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oshima, IMino, YInomata, YEffects of environmental deprivation on negative symptoms of schizophrenia: A nationwide survey in Japan’s psychiatric hospitals. Psychiatry Res 2005; 136:163–71 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2005.06.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirschner, MAleman, AKaiser, SSecondary negative symptoms — a review of mechanisms, assessment and treatment. Schizophr Res 2017; 186:2938http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2016.05.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farreny, AAguado, JOchoa, SHuerta-Ramos, EMarsà, FLópez-Carrilero, R et al. REPYFLEC cognitive remediation group training in schizophrenia: looking for an integrative approach. Schizophr Res 2012; 142http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.035.Google ScholarPubMed
Brébion, GStephan-Otto, CHuerta-Ramos, EOchoa, SUsall, JAbellán-Vega, H et al. Visual encoding impairment in patients with schizophrenia: contribution of reduced working memory span, decreased processing speed, and affective symptoms. Neuropsychology 2015; 29:1724http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/neu0000104.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association, DSM-IV: Manual diagnóstico y estadístico de los trastornos mentales. Texto revisado 2002, Elsevier Masson Barcelona.Google Scholar
First, Michael BSpitzer, Robert LGibbon, MiriamWilliams, JBStructured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition With Psychotic Screen 2002, Biometrics Research New York.Google Scholar
Wilson, BAlderman, NBurgess, PEmslie, HEvans, JBehavioural Assessment of the Dysexecutive Syndrome 1996, Thames Valley Test Company London.Google Scholar
Reitan, RWolfson, DCategory Test and Trail Making Test as measures of frontal lobe functions. Clin Neuropsychol Neuropsychol 1995; 9:50–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wechsler, DWMS-III- Escala de Memoria de Wechsler. Madrid: T.E.A Ediciones 2004.Google Scholar
Peralta Martín, VCuesta Zorita, MJValidation of positive and negative symptom scale (PANSS) in a sample of Spanish schizophrenic patients. Actas Luso Esp Neurol Psiquiatr Cienc Afines 1994; 22:171–7.Google Scholar
Wechsler, DWAIS-III: Wechsler Adult Intelligence ScaleAdministration and scoring manual3rd ed.1997, Psychological Corporation San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Stroop, JRStudies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935; 18:643–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Algarabel, SRuiz, JCSanmartin, JThe University of Valencia’s computerized word pool. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput 1988; 20:398403 10.3758/BF03202684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peralta, VCuesta, MJDimensional structure of psychotic symptoms: an item-level analysis of SAPS and SANS symptoms in psychotic disorders. Schizophr Res 1999; 38:1326.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
IBM, IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows 2013.Google Scholar
Daniel, DGIssues in selection of instruments to measure negative symptoms. Schizophr Res 2013; 150:343–5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marder, SRDavis, JMChouinard, GThe effects of risperidone on the five dimensions of schizophrenia derived by factor analysis: combined results of the North American trials. J Clin Psychiatry 1997; 58:538–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, MDLysaker, PHMilstein, RMBeam-Goulet, JLConcurrent validity of the cognitive component of schizophrenia: relationship of PANSS scores to neuropsychological assessments. Psychiatry Res 1994; 54:51–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rajji, TKMulsant, BHNature and course of cognitive function in late-life schizophrenia: A systematic review. Schizophr Res 2008; 102:122–40 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2008.03.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkpatrick, BFenton, WSCarpenter, WTMarder, SRThe NIMH-MATRICS consensus statement on negative symptoms. Schizophr Bull 2006; 32:214–9 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbj053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horan, WPKring, AMGur, REReise, SPBlanchard, JJDevelopment and psychometric validation of the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS). Schizophr Res 2011; 132:140–5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.06.030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kring, AMGur, REBlanchard, JJHoran, WPReise, SPThe Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms (CAINS): final development and validation. Am J Psychiatry 2013; 170:165–72 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12010109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirkpatrick, BStrauss, GPNguyen, LFischer, BADaniel, DGCienfuegos, A et al. The brief negative symptom scale: psychometric properties. Schizophr Bull 2011; 37:300–5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, GPKeller, WRBuchanan, RWGold, JMFischer, BAMcMahon, RP et al. Next-generation negative symptom assessment for clinical trials: Validation of the Brief Negative Symptom Scale. Schizophr Res 2012; 142:8892http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2012.10.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanchard, JJBradshaw, KRGarcia, CPNasrallah, HAHarvey, PDCasey, D et al. Examining the reliability and validity of the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms within the Management of Schizophrenia in Clinical Practice (MOSAIC) multisite national study. Schizophr Res 2017; 185:137–43 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2017.01.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartmann-Riemer, MNHager, OMKirschner, MBischof, MKluge, ASeifritz, E et al. The association of neurocognitive impairment with diminished expression and apathy in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 2015; 169:427–32 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.10.032.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwood, KEMorris, RSigmundsson, TLandau, SWykes, TExecutive functioning in schizophrenia and the relationship with symptom profile and chronicity. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008; 14:782–92 http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708081198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ventura, JHellemann, GSThames, ADKoellner, VNuechterlein, KHSymptoms as mediators of the relationship between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res 2009; 113:189–99 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2009.03.035.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foussias, GRemington, GNegative symptoms in schizophrenia: Avolition and occam’s razor. Schizophr Bull 2010; 36:359–69 http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbn094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1 Descriptive characteristics by group.

Figure 1

Table 2 Regression models with the PANSS Negative (Group 1).

B*=Standardized regression coefficient (95% confidence interval); PANSS NEG6: blunted affect, emotional withdrawal, poor rapport, passive-apathetic social withdrawal, lack of spontaneity and stereotyped thinking.
Figure 2

Table 3 Regression models using the SANS (Group 2).

B*=Standardized regression coefficient (95% confidence interval). SANS4: Affective flattering, alogia, avolition-apathy, anhedonia-asociality.
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.