Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-gq7q9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-23T09:32:50.047Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 3 - Methodological Hazards in the Neuroscientific Study of Religion

from I.I - The Natures of Neurology and Religion

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 October 2019

Alasdair Coles
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Joanna Collicutt
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Get access

Summary

The purpose of this chapter is to survey the various approaches and methods that have been employed in the attempt to study the extent to which religious beliefs, practices and experiences are associated with activity in different parts and pathways of the human brain. This includes not only the neurology of religion in the sense of how damage to the brain affects aspects of religious practice and belief, but also studies of neurotypical subjects. My aim has been to cover the full range of modern studies. For good measure I have mentioned some techniques that although not so far used for this purpose, could be used in future.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Azari, N. P., Nickel, J., Wunderlich, G., Niedeggen, M., Hefter, H., Tellman, L., Herzog, H., Stoerig, P., Birnbacher, D and Seitz, R. J.. 2001. Neural correlates of religious experience. European Journal of Neuroscience 13: 1649–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barker, A. T., Jalinous, R and Freeston, I. L.. 1985. Non-invasive magnetic stimulation of human motor cortexThe Lancet 325: 1106–7.Google Scholar
Baylor Institute for Studies of Religion. 2006. American Piety in the 21st Century: New Insights to the Depth and Complexity of Religion in the US. Houston, TX: Baylor Institute for Studies of Religion.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., and Paquette, V.. 2006. Neural correlates of a mystical experience in Carmelite nuns. Neuroscience Letters 405: 186–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bennett, C. M., Wolford, G. L. and Miller, M. B.. 2009. The principled control of false positives in neuroimaging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4: 417–22. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanke, O., Ortigue, S., Landis, T and Seeck, M. 2002. Stimulating illusory own-body perceptions. Nature 419: 269–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brady, M. J., Peterman, A. H, Fitchett, G, Mo, M and Cella, D.. 1999. A case for including spirituality in quality of life measurement in oncology. Psycho-oncology 8: 417–18.Google Scholar
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P and Durso, R. 2010. Deficits in the automatic activation of religious concepts in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 16: 252–61. doi:10.1017/S1355617709991202.Google Scholar
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P., Ghofrani, J. and Durso, R.. 2011a. Disease-associated differences in religious cognition in patients with Parkinson's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 33: 917–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P. and Durso, R.. 2011b. Side of onset in Parkinson's disease and alterations in religiosity: novel behavioral phenotypes. Behavioral Neurology 24: 133–41.Google ScholarPubMed
Cahn, B. R., and Polich, J. 2006. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin 132: 180211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Erritzoe, D, Williams, T, Stone, J. M, Reed, L. J, Colasanti, A. et al. 2012. Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 109: 2138–43. doi:10.1073/pnas.1119598109.Google ScholarPubMed
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Leech, R, Erritzoe, D., Williams, T. M, Stone, J. M, Evans, J et al. 2013. Functional connectivity measures after psilocybin inform a novel hypothesis of early psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin 39: 1343–51. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs117.Google Scholar
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Muthukumaraswamy, S, Roseman, L, Kaelen, M, Droog, W, Murphy, K et al. 2016. Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multimodal neuroimaging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 113: 4853–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.1518377113.Google Scholar
Cloninger, C. R. 1994. Temperament and personality. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 4: 266–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cloninger, C. R., Przybeck, T. R, Svrakic, D. M and Wetzel, R. D. 1994. The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to Its Development and Use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University.Google Scholar
Cook, C. M., and Persinger, M. A. 2001. Geophysical variables and behavior: XCII. Experimental elicitation of the experience of a sentient being by right hemispheric, weak magnetic fields: interaction with temporal lobe sensitivity. Percept. Motor Skills 92: 447–8.Google Scholar
Crescentini, C., Aglioti, S. M, Fabbro, F and Urgesi, C. 2014. Virtual lesions of the inferior parietal cortex induce fast changes of implicit religiousness/spirituality. Cortex 54: 115. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.023.Google Scholar
Crescentini, C., Di Buccahinacio, M, Fabbro, F and Urgesi, C. 2015. Excitatory stimulation of the right inferior parietal cortex lessens implicit religiousness/spirituality. Neuropsychologia 70: 71–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
David, O., Guillemain, I, Saillet, S, Reyr, S, Deransart, C et al. 2008. Identifying neural drivers with functional MRI: an electrophysiological validation. PloS Biology 6: 2683–97. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0060315.Google Scholar
Ellison, C. W. 1983. Spiritual well-being: conceptualization and measurement. Journal of Psychology and Theology 11: 330–40.Google Scholar
Emmons, R. A., and Paloutzian, R. F. 2003. The psychology of religion. Annual Review of Psychology 54: 377402.Google Scholar
Farmer, R. F., and Goldberg, L. R. 2008. A psychometric evaluation of the Revised Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI-R) and the TCi-140. Psychological Assessment 20: 281–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fox, K. C. R., Dixon, M. L, Nijeboer, S, Girna, M, Floman, J. L., Lifshitz, M, Ellamild, M, Sedlmeier, P and Christoff, K. 2016. Functional neuroanatomy of meditation: a review and meta-analysis of 78 functional neuroimaging investigations. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 65: 208–28. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.03.021.Google Scholar
Friston, K. 2009. Causal modelling and brain connectivity in functional magnetic resonance imaging. PLoS Biology 7: e1000033. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.1000033.Google Scholar
Friston, K. J. and Dolan, R. J.. 2010. Computational and dynamic models in neuroimaging. Neuroimage 52: 752–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granqvist, P., Fredrikson, M, Unge, P, Hagenfeldt, A, Valind, S, Larhammar, D and Larsson, M. 2005. Sensed presence and mystical experiences are predicted by suggestibility, not by the application of transcranial weak complex magnetic fields. Neuroscience Letters 379: 16.Google Scholar
Han, S., Mao, L, Gu, X, Zhu, Y, Ge, J and Ma, Y. 2008. Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Social Neuroscience 3: 115. doi:10.1080/17470910701469681.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayward, R. D., Owen, A. D, Koenig, H. G, Steffens, D. C and Payne, M. E. 2011. Associations of religious behavior and experience with extent of regional atrophy in the orbitofrontal cortex during older adulthood. Religion, Brain and Behavior 1: 103–18.Google Scholar
Hill, P. C., and Hood, R. W Jr. 1999. Measures of Religiosity. Birmingham, AL: Religious Education Press.Google Scholar
Hill, D. R., and Persinger, M. A. 2003. Application of transcerebral, weak (1 microT) complex magnetic fields and mystical experiences: are they generated by field-induced dimethyltryptamine release from the pineal organ? Percept. Motor Skills 97: 1049–50.Google Scholar
Johnstone, B., and Glass, B. A. 2008. Support for a neuropsychological model of spirituality in persons with traumatic brain injury. Zygon 43: 861–74. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9744.2008.00964.x.Google Scholar
Johnstone, B., Yoon, D. P, Franklin, K. L., Schopp, L. and Hinkebein, J.. 2009. Re-conceptualizing the Factor Structure of the Brief Multidimensional Measure of Religiousness/Spirituality. Journal of Religion and Health 48: 146–63. doi:10.1007/s10943-008-9179-9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnstone, B., Bodling, A, Cohen, D, Christ, S. E and Wegrzyn, A. 2012. Right parietal lobe-related ‘selflessness’ as the neurophysiological basis of spiritual transcendence. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion 22: 267–84. doi:10.1080/10508619.2011.657524.Google Scholar
Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K, Su, M, Krueger, F. and Grafman, J. 2009a. Neuroanatomical variability religiosity. PLoS One 4(9): e7180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K, Su, M, Zamboni, G, Krueger, F and Grafman, J. 2009b. Cognitive and neural foundations of religious belief. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 106: 4876–81. doi:10.1073/pnas.0811717106.Google Scholar
Kapogiannis, D., Deshpande, G, Krueger, F, Thornburg, M. P and Grafman, J. H. 2014. Brain networks shaping religious belief. Brain Connectivity 4: 7079. doi:10.1089/brain.2013.0172.Google Scholar
Leigh, R. J., and Zee, D. 2015. The Neurology of Eye Movements. 5th edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logothetis, N. K. 2003. The underpinnings of the BOLD functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. Journal of Neuroscience 23: 3963–71.Google Scholar
Logothetis, N. K. 2008. What we can do and what we cannot do with fMRI. Nature 453: 869–78. doi:10.1038/nature06976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, D. A. 2000. Spirituality: description, measurement and relation to the five factor theory of personality. Journal of Personality 68: 153–97.Google Scholar
McNamara, P., Durso, R and Brown, A. 2006. Religiosity in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment 2: 341–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moberg, D. 2002. Assessing and measuring spirituality: confronting dilemmas of universal and particular evaluative criteria. Journal of Adult Development 9: 4760.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Modestino, E. J., O’Toole, P. and Reinhofer, A. 2016. Experiential and doctrinal religious knowledge categorization in Parkinson’s disease: behavioral and brain correlates. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10: 113. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monod, S., Brennan, M, Rochat, E, Martin, E, Rochat, S and Büla, C. J. 2011. Instruments measuring spirituality in clinical research: a systematic review. Journal of General Internal Medicine 26: 1345–57. doi:10.1007/s11606-011-1769-7.Google Scholar
Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. 2000. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Physiology 527: 633–9.Google Scholar
Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G, Wassermann, E. M, Priori, A, Lang, N, Antal, A, Paulus, W, Hummel, F, Boggio, P. S., Fregni, F and Pascual-Leone, A. 2008. Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimulation 1: 206–23. doi:10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palhano-Fontes, F., Andrade, K. C, Tofoli, L. F, Santos, A. C, Crippa, J. A. S and Hallak, J. E. C. et al. (2015). The psychedelic state induced by ayahuasca modulates the activity and connectivity of the default mode network. PLoS One 10: e0118143. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0118143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pascual-Leone, A., Walsh, V and Rothwell, J. 2000. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience – virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 10: 232–7.Google Scholar
Penfield, W., and Erickson, T. C. 1941. Epilepsy and Cerebral Localization. Oxford: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Persinger, A. M., Koren, S. A and O’Connor, R. P. 2001. Geophysical variables and behavior: CIV. Power-frequency magnetic field transients (5 microtesla) and reports of haunt experiences within an electronically dense house. Perceptual and Motor Skills 92673–4.Google Scholar
Roseman, L., Leech, R, Feilding, A, Nutt, D. J and Carhart-Harris, R. L. 2014. The effects of psilocybin and MDMA on between-network resting state functional connectivity in healthy volunteers. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 8: 204. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00204.Google Scholar
Ruff, C. C., Ugazio, G and Fehr, E. 2013. Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulation. Science 342: 482–4. doi:10.1126/science.1241399.Google Scholar
Schjødt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H, Geertz, A. W and Roepstorff, A. 2008. Rewarding prayers. Neuroscience Letters 443: 165–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schjoedt, U. 2009. The religious brain: a general introduction to the experimental neuroscience of religion. Method and Theory in the Study of Religion 21: 301–39.Google Scholar
Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jorgensen, H, Geertz, A. W and Roepstorff, A. 2009. Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience 4: 199207. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn050.Google Scholar
Speth, J., Speth, C, Kaelen, M, Schloerscheidt, A. M, Feilding, A, Nutt, D. J and Carhart-Harris, R. L. 2016. Decreased mental time travel to the past correlates with default-mode network disintegration under lysergic acid diethylamide. Journal of Psychopharmacology 30: 344–53. doi:10.1177/0269881116628430.Google Scholar
Swanson, L. R. 2018. Unifying theories of psychedelic drug effects. Frontiers in Pharmacology 9: 172.Google Scholar
Tagliazucchi, E., Carhart-Harris, R. L, Leech, R, Nutt, D and Chialvo, D. R. 2014. Enhanced repertoire of brain dynamical states during the psychedelic experience. Human Brain Mapping 35: 5442–56. doi:10.1002/hbm.22562.Google Scholar
Tagliazucchi, E., Roseman, L, Kaelen, M, Orban, C, Muthukumaraswamy, S and Murphy, K et al. 2016. Increased global functional connectivity correlates with LSD-induced ego dissolution. Current Biology 26: 1043–50. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.010.Google Scholar
Tong, F. 2003. Out-of-body experiences: from Penfield to present. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 7: 104–6.Google Scholar
Urgesi, C., Aglioti, S. M, Skrap, M and Fabbro, F. 2010. The spiritual brain: selective cortical lesions modulate human self-transcendence. Neuron 65: 309–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.026.Google Scholar
WHOQOL SRPB Group. 2006. A cross-cultural study of spirituality, religion, and personal beliefs as components of quality of life. Social Science and Medicine 62: 1486–97.Google Scholar
Zinnbauer, B. J., Pargament, K. I, Cole, B, Rye, R. S., Butter, E. M, Belavich, T. G, Hipp, K. M, Scott, A. B and Kadar, J. L. 1997. Religion and spirituality: unfuzzying the fuzzy. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion 36: 549–56.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×