Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-x5cpj Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-26T04:31:35.068Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Grammaticalisation: cause or effect?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

David Lightfoot
Affiliation:
Professor of Linguistics Georgetown University, Washington, DC
Raymond Hickey
Affiliation:
Universität-Gesamthochschule-Essen
Get access

Summary

A nineteenth-century legacy

The central research question for nineteenth-century linguists was: how did a language get to be the way it is? Historical linguistics was the only kind of linguistics of the time. Towards the end of the century, Hermann Paul (1880: 20) was able to pontificate that ‘Es ist eingewendet, dass es noch eine andere wissenschaftliche Betrachtung der Sprache gäbe, als die geschichtliche. Ich muss das in Abrede stellen’ (It has been objected that there is another scientific view of language possible besides the historical. I must contradict this). What changed for the nineteenth-century linguists was a language and a language essentially was an inventory of words. There was more to it than that, of course, but everything else was attributable to either a universal ‘logic’ or individually variable ‘habits’, and this didn't interest contemporary linguists much. So there wasn't anything to have a history of except words, their pronunciations and their meanings.

Words are transmitted from one generation to the next and they may change their form in that transmission. Latin pater ‘father’ became padre, père, patre, pai, etc. in the modern Romance languages. One could characterise such changes by writing sound ‘laws’, for example, that a dental stop is voiced in some phonetic environment, between a vowel and a vocalic r in Italian and Spanish, C –> +vd/V_ṛ.

In this view, languages are the basic objects of reality, entities ‘out there’, existing in their own right, waiting to be acquired by speakers, essentially inventories of words.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Barbour, Julian. 2000. The end of time. Oxford University Press
Bauer, Brigitte. 1995. The emergence and development of SVO patterning in Latin and French. Oxford University Press
Bloomfield, Leonard. 1933. Language. New York: Holt
Bruyn, Adrienne. 1995. Grammaticalization in creoles: the development of determiners and relative clauses in Sranan. Studies in Language and Language Use 21. Dordrecht: ICG Printing
Chomsky, Noam. 1986. Knowledge of language: its nature, origin and use. New York: Praeger
Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Darwin, Charles. 1874. The descent of man. [2nd edn, 1889] New York: D. Appleton
Dresher, Elan and Idsardi, William. 1999. ‘Prerequisites for a theory of diachronic adaptation’, Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 18.1: 212–5Google Scholar
Fischer, Olga C. M. 1997. ‘On the status of grammaticalization and the diachronic dimension in explanation’, Transactions of the Philological Society 95.2: 149–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Gelderen, Elly. 1997. Verbal agreement and the grammar behind its ‘breakdown’: minimalist feature checking. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer
Haspelmath, Martin. 1999a. ‘Are there principles of grammatical change?Journal of Linguistics 35.3: 579–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 1999b. ‘Optimality and diachronic adaptation’, Zeitschrift für Sprachwissenschaft 18: 180–205Google Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin. 1999c. Why is grammaticalization irreversible?Linguistics 37.6: 1043–68Google Scholar
Hopper, Paul and Elizabeth Traugott. 1993. Grammaticalization. Cambridge University Press
Janda, Richard D. 2001. ‘Beyond “pathways” and “unidirectionality”: on the discontinuity of language transmission and the counterability of grammaticalization.’ Language Sciences 23.2-3: 265–340Google Scholar
Joseph, Brian. 2001. ‘Is there such a thing as grammaticalization?’, Language Sciences 23.2-3: 163–86Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul. 1996. ‘The shift to head-initial VP in Germanic’, in H. Thrainsson, S. Epstein and S. Peters (eds.), Studies in comparative Germanic syntax, vol. 2. Dordrecht: Kluwer, 140–79
Lakoff, Robin T. 1972. ‘Another look at drift’, in Robert P. Stockwell and Ron Macauley (eds.), Linguistic change and generative theory. Bloomington: Indiana University Press, 172–98
Lasnik, Howard. 1999. Minimalist analysis. Oxford: Blackwell
Lass, Roger. 1980. On explaining linguistic change. Cambridge University Press
Lass, Roger. 1997. Historical linguistics and language change. Cambridge University Press
Lightfoot, David W. 1991. How to set parameters: arguments from language change. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Lightfoot, David W. 1993. ‘Why UG needs a learning theory: triggering verb movement’, in Charles Jones (ed.), Historical linguistics: problems and perspectives. London: Longman [reprinted in Battye and Roberts (eds.), 1995 Clause structure and language change. Oxford University Press]
Lightfoot, David W. 1999. The development of language: acquisition, change and evolution. Oxford: Blackwell
Newmeyer, Frederick J. 1998. Language form and language function. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Paul, Hermann. 1880. Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte. [1970] Niemeyer: Tübingen
Rask, Rasmus. 1818. Unders⊘gelse om det gamle Nordiske eller Islandske Sprogs Oprindelse. Copenhagen: Gyldendalske Boghandlings Forlag
Roberts, Ian G. 1993. ‘A formal account of grammaticalization in the history of Romance futures’, Folia Linguistica Historica 13: 219–58Google Scholar
Ross, John R. 1969. ‘Auxiliaries as main verbs’, in W. Todd (ed.), Studies in philosophical linguistics, series I. Evanston: Great Expectations
Sapir, Edward. 1921. Language. New York: Harcourt
Sapir, Edward. 1929. ‘The status of linguistics as a science’, Language 5: 207–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schleicher, August. 1848 Über die Bedeutung der Sprache für die Naturgeschichte des Menschen. Weimar: Hermann Böhlau
Schleicher, August. 1861–2. Compendium der vergleichenden Grammatik der indogermanischen Sprachen. Weimar: Hermann Böhlau
Schleicher, August. 1863. Die darwinische Theorie und die Spachwissenschaft. Weimar: Hermann Böhlau
Vennemann, Theo. 1975. ‘An explanation of drift’, in C. N. Li (ed.), Word order and word order change. Austin: University of Texas Press, 269–305
Warner, Anthony R. 1995. ‘Predicting the progressive passive: parametric change within a lexicalist framework’, Language 71.3: 533–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, Anthony R. 1997. ‘The structure of parametric change, and V movement in the history of English’, in Ans van Kemenade and Nigel Vincent (eds.), Parameters of morphosyntactic change. Cambridge University Press, 380–93
Watkins, Calvert. 1976. ‘Toward Proto-Indo-European syntax: problems and pseudoproblems’, in S. Steever, C. Walker and S. Mufwene (eds.), Diachronic syntax. Chicago Linguistic Society, 305–26

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×