Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-tn8tq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-06-30T10:43:16.207Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

B - Opening sequencing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

James E. Katz
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Mark Aakhus
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Get access

Summary

It is an easily noticeable fact about two-party conversations that their speaker sequencing is alternating. That is to say, the sequencing of speakers in two-party conversation can be described by the formula “ababab,” where “a” and “b” are the parties to the conversation.

The “abab” formula is a specification, for two-party conversation, of a basic rule for conversation: one party at a time. The strength of this rule can be seen in the members' practice that, in a multi-party setting (more precisely, where there are four or more participants), if more than one person is talking, it can be claimed not that the rule has been violated, but that more than one conversation is going on. Thus, Bales can write (1950, p. 461; emphasis added):

The conversation generally proceeded so that one person talked at time, and all members in the particular group were attending the same conversation. In this sense, these groups might be said to have a “single focus,” that is, they did not involve a number of conversations proceeding at the same time.

When combined with an analytic conception of an utterance, the “abab” specification has a variety of other interesting consequences, such as allowing us to see how persons can come to say “X is silent,” when no person in the setting is talking – as in Bergler's (1938) title, “On the Resistance Situation: The Patient Is Silent.

Type
Chapter
Information
Perpetual Contact
Mobile Communication, Private Talk, Public Performance
, pp. 326 - 385
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Albert, E. (1964).“‘Rhetoric’, ‘Logic’, and ‘Poetics’ in Burundi: Cultural Patterning of Speech Behavior.” In J. J. Gumperz and D. Hymes (eds.), The Ethnography of Communication. Washington, DC: American Anthropologist (Special Issue), 66(6), part 2, pp. 35–54CrossRef
Bales, R. F. (1950). Interaction Process Analysis: A Method for the Study of Small Groups. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley
Berelson, B. R., Lazarsfeld, P. F., and McPhee, W. N. (1954). Voting: A Study of Opinion Formation in a Presidential Campaign. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Bergler, E. (1938). “On the Resistance Situation: The Patient Is Silent.”Psychoanalytic Review 25: 170–186Google Scholar
Bolinger, D. (1958). Interrogative Structures of American English. University of Alabama Press
Ervin-Tripp, S. (1969). “Sociolinguistic Rules of Address.” In L. Berkowitz (ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New York and London: Academic Press, 93–107
Goffman, E. (1953). “Communication Conduct in an Island Community.” Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Sociology, University of Chicago
Goffman, E. (1961). Encounters: Two Studies in the Sociology of Interaction. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill
Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in Public Places: Notes on the Social Organization of Gathering. New York: Free Press
Harrah, D. (1963). Communication: A Logical Model. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Hymes, D. (1964a). Language in Culture and Society. New York: Harper & Row
Hymes, D. (1964b). “Introduction: Toward Ethnographies of Communication.” In J. J. Gumperz and D. Hymes (eds.), The Ethnography of Communication. Washington, DC: American Anthropologist (Special Issue), 66(6), part 2, pp. 1–34
Landis, M. H., and Burtt, H. E. (1924). “A Study of Conversations.”Journal of Comparative Psychology 4: 81–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, D. K. (1969). Convention: A Philosophical Study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Morgan, B. (1967). “How's Your Aide's Telephone Technique?”Medical Economics, April 3: 219–223Google Scholar
Oakeshott, M. J. (1959). The Voice of Poetry in the Conversation of Mankind. An Essay. London: Bowes & Bowes
Perelman, C., and Olbrechts-Tyteca, L. (1969). The New Rhetoric: A Treatise on Argumentation. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press
Priestly, J. B. (1926). Talking. New York and London: Harper
Sacks, H. (1972a). “An Initial Investigation of the Usability of Conversational Materials for Doing Sociology.” In D. N. Sudnow (ed.), Studies in Social Interaction. New York: Free Press, 31–74
Sacks, H. (1972b). “On the Analyzability of Stories by Children.” In J. J. Gumperz and D. Hymes (eds.), Directions in Sociolinguistics: The Ethnography of Communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 325–345
Sacks, H. (1992). Lectures on Conversation, 2 vols. Ed. by G. Jefferson, with Introductions by E. A. Schegloff. Oxford: Blackwell
Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., and Jefferson, G. (1974). “A Simplest Systematics for the Organization of Turn-Taking for Conversation.”Language 50: 696–735CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1967). “The First Five Seconds: The Order of Conversational Openings.” Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, University of California, Berkeley
Schegloff, E. A. (1968). “Sequencing in Conversational Openings.”American Anthropologist 70: 1075–1095CrossRef
Schegloff, E. A. (1972). “Notes on a Conversational Practice: Formulating Place.” In D. N. Sudnow (ed.), Studies in Social Interaction. New York: Free Press, 75–119
Schegloff, E. A. (forthcoming) “Answering the Phone.” In G. H. Lerner (ed.), Conversation Analysis: Studies from the First Generation
Schegloff, E. A., and Sacks, H. (1973). “Opening up Closings.” Semiotica 8:289–327; reprinted in John Baugh and Joel Sherzer (eds.), Language in Use: Readings in Sociolinguistics. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1984
Steinzor, B. (1949). “The Development and Evaluation of a Measure of Social Interaction.”Human Relations 2: 319–347CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephen, F. F., and Mishler, E. Y. (1952). “The Distribution of Participation in Small Groups: An Exponential Approximation.”American Sociological Review 17: 598–608CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J., and Potter, R. (1962). “An Analytical Unit for the Study of Interaction.”Human Relations 15: 245–263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Westermarck, E. (1926). Ritual and Belief in Morocco. London: Macmillan

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×