Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-wxhwt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-15T12:10:28.506Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Sense and sensitivity: metacognition in Alzheimer's disease

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Chris Moulin
Affiliation:
University of Leeds, UK
Get access

Summary

Everyone complains of his memory,

and no one complains of his judgement.

François Duc de La Rochefoucauld, (1613–1680)

Consider two presentations of patients that are typical of people attending memory clinics for neuropsychological assessment. Patient A was referred by her family doctor, after her husband had insisted that she had a professional opinion on her memory problems. She presented with a marked memory impairment, but enjoyed the testing session and joked about her memory not being as good as it used to be, but she was confident that she was scoring well in the formal assessments of memory. She could give no examples of her memory difficulties, and denied it was having any impact on her life. And yet, just the day before she had been upset that she could not remember her grandchildren's names, or recognise her sister. Patient B self-referred to see a community-based screening team and harangued his family doctor for a referral for formal memory assessment. He did not joke about his problems, but complained constantly that his memory was failing him. He gave detailed vignettes of recent memory failures, such as failing to lock up the house, but his scores on a range of memory assessments were well within the normal range. Detailed testing, history, and medical examination suggested that Patient A had Alzheimer's disease, and that Patient B was one of the group of clients called the “worried well.”

Type
Chapter
Information
Applied Metacognition , pp. 197 - 223
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Almkvist, O., Fratiglioni, L., Aguero-Torres, H., Viitanen, M., and Backmän, L. (1999). Cognitive support at episodic encoding and retrieval: similar patterns of utilization in community-based samples of Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 816–830CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arriagada, P. V., Marzloff, K., and Hyman, B. T. (1992). Distribution of Alzheimer-type pathologic changes in nondemented elderly individuals matches the pattern in Alzheimer's disease. Neurology, 42, 1681–1688CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bäckman, L., and Lipinska, B. (1993). Monitoring of general knowledge: evidence for preservation in early Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 31, 335–345CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Baddeley, A. D. (1997). Human memory: theory and practice. Hove, UK: Psychology Press
Becker, J. T. (1987). A two-component model of memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease. In R. J. Wurtman, S. H. Corkin, and J. H. Growdon (eds.), Alzheimer's disease: advances in basic research and therapies, pp. 343–348. Cambridge, MA: Center for Brain Sciences and Metabolism Charitable Trust
Bruce, P. R., Coyne, A., and Botwinick, J. (1982). Adult age differences in metamemory. Journal of Gerontology, 37, 354–357CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connor, L. T., Dunlosky, J., and Hertzog, C. (1997). Age-related differences in absolute but not relative metamemory accuracy. Psychology and Aging, 12, 50–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corkin, S. (1981). Acetylcholine, aging and Alzheimer's disease: implications for treatment. Trends in Neuroscience, 4, 287–290CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Correa, D. D., Graves, R. E., and Costa, L. (1996). Awareness of memory deficit in Alzheimer's disease patients and memory impaired older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 3, 215–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coyne, A. C. (1985). Adult age, presentation time, and memory performance. Experimental Aging Research, 11, 147–149CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cummings, J. L., and Benson, D. F. (1992). Dementia: a clinical approach. Boston, MA: Butterworth's
Della Sala, S., Logie, R. H., and Spinnler, H. (1992). Is primary memory deficit of Alzheimer patients due to a “central executive” impairment?Journal of Neurolinguistics, 7, 325–346CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunlosky, J., and Connor, L. T. (1997). Age differences in the allocation of study time account for age differences in memory performance. Memory and Cognition, 25, 691–700CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunlosky, J., and Nelson, T. O. (1992). Importance of the kind of cue for judgments of learning (JOL) and the delayed JOL effect. Memory and Cognition, 20, 374–380CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of the patient for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. D. W., Baddeley, A. D., and Hodges, J. R. (1996). Analysis of the episodic memory deficit in early Alzheimer's disease: evidence from the doors and people test. Neuropsychologia, 34, 537–551CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herlitz, A., and Viitanen, M. (1991). Semantic organization and verbal episodic memory in patients with mild and moderate Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 13, 559-574CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertzog, C., and Dixon, R. A. (1994). Metacognitive development in adulthood and old age. In J. Metcalfe and A. P. Shimamura (eds.), Metacognition: knowing about knowing, pp. 227–251. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Jameson, K. A., Narens, L., Goldfarb, K., and Nelson, T. O. (1990). The influence of near-threshold priming on metamemory and recall. Acta Psychologica, 73, 55–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joynt, R. J., and Shoulson, I. (1985). Dementia. In K. M. Heilman and E. Valenstein (eds.), Clinical neuropsychology. New York: Oxford University Press
Kopelman, M. D. (1985). Rates of forgetting in Alzheimer-type dementia and Korsakoff's syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 15, 527–541Google Scholar
Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one's own knowledge during study: a cue utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 349–370CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonesio, R. J., and Nelson, T. O. (1990). Do different metamemory judgements tap the same underlying aspects of memory?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 464–470Google Scholar
Lipinska, B., and Bäckman, L. (1996). Feeling of knowing in fact retrieval: further evidence for preservation in early Alzheimer's disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 350–358CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lopez, O. L., Becker, J. T., Somsak, D., Dew, M. D., and DeKosky, S. T. (1994). Awareness of cognitive deficits and anosognosia in probable Alzheimer's disease. European Journal of Neurology, 34, 277–282CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovelace, E. A. (1984). Metamemory: monitoring future recallability during study. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 756–766Google Scholar
Mandler, G., Goodman, G. O., and Wilkesgibbs, D. L. (1982). The word frequency paradox in recognition. Memory and Cognition, 10, 33–42CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mazzoni, G., and Nelson, T. O. (1995). Judgments of learning are affected by the kind of encoding in ways that cannot be attributed to the level of recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1263–1274Google ScholarPubMed
McDonald-Miszczak, L., Hunter, M. A., and Hultsch, D. F. (1994). Adult age differences in predicting memory performance: the effects of normative information and task experience. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48, 95–118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGlynn, S. M., and Kaszniak, A. W. (1991). When metacognition fails: impaired awareness of deficit in Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 3, 183–189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moss, M. B., and Albert, M. S. (1988). Alzheimer's disease and other dementing disorders. In M. S. Albert and M. B. Moss (eds.), Geriatric neuropsychology, pp. 293–303. New York: The Guildford Press
Moulin, C. J. A. (1999). Does a metacognitive deficit contribute to the memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease? Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Bristol University
Moulin, C. J. A., Perfect, T. J., and Fitch, F. (in press). Judgments of learning, study time and item repetition in Alzheimer's disease. In P. Chambres, M. Izaute, and P.-J. Marescaux (eds.), Metacognition: process, function, and use. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press
Moulin, C. J. A., Perfect, T. J., and Jones, R. W. (2000a). Evidence for intact memory monitoring in Alzheimer's disease: metamemory sensitivity at encoding. Neuropsychologia, 38, 1242–1250CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moulin, C. J. A., Perfect, T. J., and Jones, R. W. (2000b). Global predictions of memory in Alzheimer's disease: evidence for preserved metamemory monitoring. Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition, 7, 230–244CrossRef
Moulin, C. J. A., Perfect, T. J., and Jones, R. W. (2000c). The effects of repetition on allocation of study time and judgements of learning in Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychologia, 38, 748–756CrossRef
Multhaup, K. S., and Balota, D. A. (1997). Generation effects and source memory in healthy older adults and in adults with dementia of the Alzheimer type. Neuropsychology, 11, 382–391CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, T. O. (1993). Judgments of learning and the allocation of study time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 269–273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, T. O., and Leonesio, R. J. (1988). Allocation of self-paced study time and the labor in vain effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 676–686Google ScholarPubMed
Nelson, T. O., and Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: a theoretical framework and some new findings. In G. H. Bower (ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Volume 26, pp. 125–173. San Diego, CA: Academic PressCrossRef
Pappas, B. A., Sunderland, T., Weingartner, H. M., Vitiello, B., Martinson, H., and Putnam, K. (1992). Alzheimer's disease and feeling of knowing for knowledge and episodic memory. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 47, 159–164CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perfect, T. J., and Hollins, T. S. (1999). Feeling of knowing judgments do not predict subsequent recognition performance for eyewitness memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 5, 250–264Google Scholar
Reisberg, B., Gordon, B., McCarthy, M., and Ferris, S. H. (1985). Clinical symptoms accompanying progressive cognitive decline in Alzheimer's disease. In V. L. Melnick and N. N. Dubler (eds.), Alzheimer's dementia, pp. 19–39. Clifton, NJ: Humana PressCrossRef
Rubin, D. C., and Friendly, M. (1986). Predicting which words get recalled: measures of free recall, availability, goodness, emotionality, and pronouncability for 925 nouns. Memory and Cognition, 14, 79–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schacter, D. L., McLachlan, D. R., Moscovitch, M., and Tulving, E. (1986). Monitoring of recall performance by memory disorders patients. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 8, 130Google Scholar
Schneck, M. K., Reisberg, B., and Ferris, S. H. (1982). An overview of the current concepts of Alzheimer's disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 165–173Google ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, B. L., and Metcalfe, J. (1994). Methodological problems and pitfalls in the study of human metacognition. In J. Metcalfe and A. P. Shimamura (eds.), Metacognition: knowing about knowing, pp. 93–113. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Shimamura, A. P. (1994). The neuropsychology of metacognition. In J. Metcalfe and A. P. Shimamura (eds.), Metacognition: knowing about knowing, pp. 253–276. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Shimamura, A. P., and Squire, L. R. (1986). Memory and metamemory: a study of feeling-of-knowing phenomenon in amnesic patients. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 452–460Google ScholarPubMed
Storandt, M., Botwinick, J., and Danziger, W. L. (1986). Longitudinal changes: patients with mild SDAT and matched healthy controls. In L. W. Poon (ed.), Clinical memory assessment of older adults, pp. 277–284. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Stuss, D. T. (1991). Disturbance of self awareness after frontal system damage. In G. P. Prigatano and D. L. Schacter (eds.), Awareness of deficits after brain injury: clinical and theoretical issues, pp. 63–83. New York: Oxford University Press
Woodward, J. L., Dunlosky, J., and Salthouse, T. A. (1999). Task decomposition analysis of inter-trial free recall performance on the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning test in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 21, 666–676CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×