Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-wxhwt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-11T16:25:19.532Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 July 2009

Marisa Bortolussi
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
Peter Dixon
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Psychonarratology
Foundations for the Empirical Study of Literary Response
, pp. 275 - 294
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, D. The hitchhikers guide to the galaxy. London: Pan Books; 1979
Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. A.Bayesian analysis of attribution processes. Psychological Bulletin. 1975; 82:21–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, N. H. Foundations of information integration theory. New York: Academic Press; 1981
Anderson, R. C. and Pichert, J.Recall of previously unrecallable information following a shift in perspective. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1978; 17:1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andringa, E.; Tan, E.; van Horssen, P.; and Jacobs, A. Effects of focalization in film narration. in: Totosy de Zepetnek, T. and Sywenky, I., editors. The systemic and empirical approach to literature and culture as theory and application. Edmonton: University of Alberta; 1997; pp. 203–15
Argueta, R. One day of life, Vintage International Edition, 1991, translated from original, Un Día en la Vida, San Salvador: UCA Editores; 1980
Aumonier, S. “Miss Bracegirdle does her duty.” in: Priestley, J. B., editor. Great British short stories. London: Reader's Digest Association; 1974; pp. 26–39
Baddeley, A. Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon; 1986
Bal, Mieke. The narrating and the focalizing: A theory of the agents in narrative. Style. 1983; 17(2):234–69Google Scholar
Bal, Mieke. Narratology: Introduction to the theory of narrative. Boheemen, C. van, translator. Toronto: University of Toronto Press; 1985
Bally, C.Le style indirect libre en français moderne. Germanisch-Romanisch Monatsschrift. 1912; 4(549–56):597–606Google Scholar
Banfield, A.Narrative style and the grammar of direct and indirect speech. Foundations of Language. 1973; 10:1–39Google Scholar
Banfield, A.Reflective and non-reflective consciousness in the language of fiction. Poetics Today. 1981; 2(2):61–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banfield, A. Where epistemology, style, and grammar meet literary history. in: Lucy, J. A., editor. Reflexive language: Reported speech and metapragmatics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1993;(3):339–64
Barthes, R.Introduction à l'analyse structurale des récets. Communications. 1966: 8:1–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barthes, R. Introduction à l'analyse structurale des récits. Barthes, R.; Kayser, W.; Booth, W. C., and Hamon, P., editors. Poétique du récit. Paris: Edition du Seuil; 1977; pp. 7–57
Bartlett, F. C. Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1932
Bates, E. The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press; 1979
Bauer, W., et al. Text und Rezeption: Wirkungsanalyse Zeitgenössischer Lyrik am Beispiel des Gedichtes “Fadensonne” von Paul Celan. Frankfurt: Athenäum; 1972
Beach, R. A teacher's introduction to reader-response theories. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English; 1993
Belli, G. The inhabited woman. March, K., translator. Willimantic, CT: Warner Books Edition; 1995
Black, J. B. and Wilensky, R.An evaluation of story grammars. Cognitive Science. 1979; 3:213–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, J. B.; Turner, T. J. and Bower, G. H.Point of view in narrative comprehension, memory, and production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1979; 18:187–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bleich, D. Subjective criticism. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press; 1978
Booth, W. The rhetoric of fiction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1961
Bordwell, D. Narration in the fiction film. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press; 1985
Borges, J. L. “Emma Zunz.” in: Borges, J. L. Labyrinths. New York: Modern Library; 1983; pp. 132–7
Bortolussi, M. and Dixon, P.The effects of formal training on literary reception. Poetics Today. 1996; 23:471–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bortolussi, M. Science and the study of literature. Rusch, G., editor. Spiel: Siegener Periodicum Zur Internationalen Empirischen Literaturwissenschaft. 1998; 16(1/2):67–70
Bortolussi, M. A psychonarratological revision of the theory of focalization. Language and Style. in press
Bower, G. H.Experiments on story understanding and recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1976; 28:511–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H.; Black, J. B.; and Turner, T. J.Scripts in memory for text. Cognitive Psychology. 1979; 11:177–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremond, Claude. La logique des possibles narratifs. Communications. 1966; 8:60–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremond, Claude. Logique du récit. Paris: Seuil, 1973
Bremond, ClaudeThe logic of narrative possibilities. New Literary History. 1980; 11:398–411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremond, Claude. A critique of the motif. Todorov, T., editor. French literary theory today. Carter, R., translator. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1982; pp. 124–46
Brodzki, B.She was unable not to think: Borges' “Emma Zunz” and the female subject. MLA. 1985; 100(2):330–47Google Scholar
Bronzwaer, W.Mieke Bal's concept of focalization: A critical note. Poetics Today. 1981; 2(2):193–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, N. R.Estimation strategies and the judgment of event frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1995; 21:1539–53Google Scholar
Bruce, B.A social interaction model of reading. Discourse Processes. 1981; 4:273–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, D. J.; Tversky, B.; and Franklin, N.Internal and external spatial frameworks for representing described scenes. Journal of Memory and Language. 1992; 31:74–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burch, N. To the distant observer. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press; 1979
Card, O. S. Ender's game. New York: Tom Doherty Associates; 1977
Carpenter, P. A. and Just, M. A., Integrative processes in comprehension. in: LaBerge, D. and Samuels, J., editors. Basic processes in reading: Perception and comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977a
Carpenter, P. A. and Just, M. A. Reading comprehension as eyes see it. in: Just, M. A. and Carpenter, P. A., editors. Cognitive processes in comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977b
Carr, E. Klee Wyck. Toronto: Irwin Publishing; 1941
Carter, A. Nights at the circus. London: Vintage; 1994
Carter, A. Black Venus. London: Vintage; 1996
Cather, W. “Paul's Case.” in: Cather, Willa. Five stories. New York: Vintage Books; 1959; pp. 149–74
Cela, J. C. The Family of Pascual Duarte. Kerrigan, A., translator. Boston: Little, Brown and Co.; 1964
Chafe, W. L. Discourse structure and human knowledge. in: Freedle, W. O. and Carroll, J. B., editors. Language comprehension and the acquisition of knowledge. Washington, DC: Winston & Sons; 1972
Chafe, W. L. Some things that narratives tell us about the mind. in: Britton, B. K. and Pellegrini, A. D., editors. Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1990; pp. 79–98
Chang, F.Active memory processes in visual sentence comprehension: Clause effects and pronomial reference. Memory and Cognition. 1980; 8:58–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatman, S.On the formalist–structuralist theory of character. Journal of Literary Semantics. 1972; 1:57–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatman, S. Story and discourse: Narrative structure in fiction and film. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1978
Chatman, S.Characters and narrators: Filter, center, slant, and interest-focus. Poetics Today. 1986; 7(2):189–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatman, S. Coming to terms. Ithaca: Cornell University Press; 1990
Chatman, S.How loose can narrators get? (And how vulnerable can narratees be?). Narrative. 1995; 3(3):3Google Scholar
Cherryh, C. J. Cyteen: The betrayal. New York: Popular Library; 1988
Cherryh, C. J. Fortress of owls. New York: Harpe Prism; 1999
Chomsky, N. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton; 1957
Chomsky, N.Review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Language. 1959; 35:26–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1965
Clark, H. H.The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1973; 12:335–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1996
Clark, H. H. and Carlson, T. B.Hearers and speech acts. Language. 1982; 58:332–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Chase, W. G.On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology. 1972; 3:472–517CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Gerrig, R. J.On the pretense theory of irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1984; 113:121–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, H. H. and Gerrig, R. J.Quotations as demonstrations. Language. 1990; 66:764–805CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Schaefer, E. F.Contributing to discourse. Cognitive Science. 1989; 13:259–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.; Cohen, J.;Smith, J. E. K.; and Keppel, G.Discussion of Wike and Church's comments. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1976; 15:257–66Google Scholar
Cohen, J.The Earth is round (p <.05). American Psychologist. 1994; 49:997–1003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, D. Transparent minds: Narrative modes for presenting consciousness in fiction. 1978: Princeton: Princeton University Press
Corcoran, M. G.Language, character, and gender in the direct discourse of Dubliners. Style. 1991; 25(3):439–53Google Scholar
Coste, D. Narrative as communication. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota; 1989
Coulmas, F. Direct and Indirect Speech. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyère; 1986
Craig, C. “In the hills.” in: Mordecai, P. and Wilson, B., editors. Her true-true name: An anthology of women's writing from the Caribbean. Oxford: Heinemann; 1990; pp. 56–9
Craik, F. I. M. and Lockhart, R. S.Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior. 1972; 11:671–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M. and Watkins, M. J.The role of rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1973; 12:599–607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culler, J. Defining narrative units. in: Fowler, R., editor. Style and structure in literature: Essays in the new stylistics. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1975a
Culler, J. Structuralist poetics: Structuralism, linguistics, and the study of literature. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul; 1975b
Cupchik, G. C. Identification as a basic problem for aesthetic reception. in: Tötösy de Zepetnek, S. and Sywenky, I., editors. The systemic and empirical approach to literature and culture as theory and application. Edmonton: University of Alberta Press; 1997; pp. 11–22
Cupchik, G. C.; Oatley, K.; and Vorderer, L.Emotional effects of reading excerpts from short stories by James Joyce. Poetics. 1998; 25:363–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danov, D.Dialogic structures in Crime and Punishment. Russian, Croatian and Serbian, Czech and Slovak, Polish Literature. 1986; 19(3):291–314Google Scholar
Deighton, L. Berlin game. London: Grafton Books; 1984
de Man, P. Blindness and insight: Essays in the rhetoric of comparative criticism. Minneapolis: University of Minneapolis Press; 1983
Dillon, G. Language processing and the reading of literature. Bloomington: Indiana University Press; 1978
Dingwall, W. The species-specificity of speech. in: Dato, D. P., editor. Developmental psycholinguistics: Theory and applications. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press; 1975
Dixon, P.Why scientists value p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 1998; 5:390–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P. and Bortolussi, M.Literary communication: Effects of reader-narrator co-operation. Poetics. 1996a; 23(6):405–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P. The narrator in the text and the narrator in the reader. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature. 1996b; Nakoda Lodge, Alberta, Canada
Dixon, P. I metodi della psiconarratologia. in: Nemesio, A., editor. L'esperienza del testo. Rome: Meltemi Editori; 1999c; pp. 126–43
Dixon, P. Narratorial relevance and character goals. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature. 2000; Toronto
Dixon, P.Text is not communication: A challenge to a common assumption. Discourse Processes. 2001a; 31(1):1–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P. Prolegomena for a science of psychonarratology. in: W. van Peer and S. Chatman, editors. New perspectives on narrative perspective. Albany, NY: State University of New York; 2001b; pp. 275–88
Dixon, P. and O'Reilly, T.Scientific versus statistical inference. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1999; 53:133–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P.; Bortolussi, M.; Twilley, L. C.; and Leung, A.Literary processing and interpretation: Towards empirical foundations. Poetics. 1993; 22:5–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doležel, L. Possible worlds and literary fictions. in: Allén, S., editor. Possible worlds in humanities, arts and sciences. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter; 1989; pp. 221–42
Dorfman, M. H.Evaluating the interpretive community: Evidence from expert and novice readers. Poetics. 1996; 23:453–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U.The cognitive perspective of ‘naturalist’ linguistic models. Cognitive Linguistics. 1990; 1:75–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duchan, J. F.; Bruder, G. A.; and Hewitt, L. E. Deixis in Narrative. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1995
Ebert, T. L. The romance of patriarchy: Ideology, subjectivity, and postmodern feminist cultural theory. Cultural Critique. 1988; 10
Eco, U. The role of the reader: Explorations in the semiotics of texts. Bloomington: Indiana University Press; 1979
Edmiston, W. F.Focalization and the first-person narrator: A revision of the theory. Poetics Today. 1989; 10(4):729–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, A. W. F. Likelihood. Baltimore: John Hopkins Univeristy; 1992
Einstein, G. O.; McDaniel, M. A.; Owen, P. D.; and Coté, N. C.Encoding and recall of texts: The importance of material appropriate processing. Journal of Memory and Language. 1990; 29:566–582CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellison, R. Invisible man. New York: Random House; 1952
Emberson, J.Reported speech in medieval German narrative. Paregon: Bulletin of the Australian and New Zealand Association for Medieval and Renaissance Studies. 1986; 4:103–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmott, C. Narrative comprehension: A discourse perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1997
Ericsson, K. A. and Kintsch, W.Long term working memory. Psychological Review. 1995; 102:211–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erlich, S. Point of view: A linguistic analysis of literary style. London & New York: Routledge; 1990
Faulkner, W. Honor. Collected stories of William Faulkner. New York: Random House; 1950; pp. 551–64
Faulkner, W. Honor. A rose for Emily. Selected short stories of William Faulkner. New York: The Modern Library; 1961; pp. 49–61
Fetterley, J. The resisting reader: A feminist aproach to American fiction. Bloomington: Indiana University Press; 1977
Findley, T. Not Wanted on the Voyage. Toronto: Penguin Books; 1984
Fisch, H.Character as linguistic sign. New Literary History. 1990; 21:593–606CrossRef
Fish, S.Literature in the reader: Affective stylistics. New Literary History. 1970; 2:123–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fish, S. Is there a text in this class: The authority of interpretive communities. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1980
Fish, S.Being interdisciplinary is so very hard to do. Profession. 1989; 89:15–22Google Scholar
Fishelov, D.Types of characters, characteristics of types. Style. 1990; 3:422–39Google Scholar
Fishelov, D. Metaphors of genre: The role of analogies in genre theory. State College, PA: Pennsylvania State University; 1993
Fisher, R. A. Statistical methods for research workers. Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd; 1925
Fisher, R. A.Statistical methods and scientific induction. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B. 1955; 17:69–78Google Scholar
Flaubert, G. Madame Bovary: A story of provincial life. Russell, A., translator. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books; 1950
Fletcher, C. R.Strategies for the allocation of short-term memory during comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 1986; 25:43–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, C. R. and Bloom, C. P.Causal reasoning in the comprehension of simple narrative texts. Journal of Memory and Language. 1988; 27:235–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fludernik, M. The fictions of language and the languages of fiction: The linguistic representation of speech and consciousness. London: Routledge; 1993
Fludernik, M. Towards a “natural” narratology. London: Routledge; 1996
Forster, E. M. Aspects of the novel. London: Edward Arnold; 1974
Fort, B. Manon's suppressed voice: The uses of reported speech. Romanic Review. 1985; 76(2):172–91
Fowler, A. Kinds of literature: An introduction to the theory of genres and modes. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press; 1982
Franklin, N. and Tversky, B.Searching imagined environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1990; 119:63–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, N.; Tversky, B.; and Coon, V.Switching points of view in spatial mental models. Memory & Cognition. 1992; 20:507–18CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frazier, L. and Rayner, K.Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology. 1982; 14:178–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frow, J.Spectacle binding: On character. Poetics Today. 1986; 7(2):227–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García, C. Dreaming in Cuban, New York: Ballantine Books; 1992
Genette, G. Figures III. Paris: Editions du Seuil; 1973
Genette, G. Narrative discourse. Oxford: Basil Blackwell; 1980
Genette, G. Narrative discourse: An essay in method. McDowell, R. S. and Velie, A., translators. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1983
Gergen, K. J.Textual considerations in the scientific construction of human character. Style. 1990; 24(3):17–31Google Scholar
Gerrig, R. J. Experiencing narrative worlds. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1993
Gerrig, R. J. and Allbritton, D. W.The construction of literary character: A view from cognitive psychology. Style. 1990; 24:380–91Google Scholar
Gerrig, R. J.; Brennan, S. E.; and Ohaeri, J. O.What can we conclude from speakers behaving badly?Discourse Processes. 2000; 29:173–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerrig, R. J.; Ohaeri, J. O; and Brennan, S. E.Illusory transparency revisited. Discourse processes. 2000; 29:137–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R.Comprehending figurative referential descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 1990; 16:56–66Google ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R. Intentions in the experience of meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1999
Gibson, W. All tomorrow's parties. New York: Ace Books; 1999
Glanzer, M.; Fischer, B.; and Dorfman, D.Short-term storage in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1984; 23:467–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenn, C. G.The rules of episodic structure and of story length in children's recall of simple stories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1978; 17:229–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M.What memory is for. Behavioural and Brain Sciences. 1997; 20:1–55Google ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, A. M. and Robertson, D. A.Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes. 1999; 28:1–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M.; Robertson, D. A.; and Members of the Honors Seminar in Cognitive Psychology. Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000; 43:379–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M.; Meyer, M.; and Lindem, K.Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 1987; 26:69–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, S. N. and Royall, R.Evidence and scientific research. American Journal of Public Health. 1988; 78:1568–74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graesser, A. C.; Bowers, C.; Olde, B.; and Pomeroy, V.Who said what? Source memory for narrator and character agents in literary short stories. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1999; 91:284–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C.; Bowers, C. A.; Olde, B.; White, K.; and Person, N. K.Who knows what? Propagation of knowledge among agents in a literary storyworld. Poetics. 1999; 26:143–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C.; Singer, M.; and Trabasso, T.Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review. 1994; 101:371–95CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graves, B. and Frederiksen, C. H.Literary expertise in the description of a fictional narrative. Poetics. 1991; 20:1–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greimas, A. J.Narrative grammars: Units and levels. MLN, 1971; 86:793-806CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. Logic and conversation. in: Cole, P. and Morgan, J. L., editors. Syntax and semantics: Speech acts. New York: Academic Press; 1975; pp. 41–58
Gross, J. B.A telling side of narration: Direct discourse and French women writers. The French Review. 1993; 66(3):401–11Google Scholar
Gross, S.Cognitive readings; or, the disappearance of Literature in the Mind. Poetics Today. 1997; 18(2):271–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hakala, C.Accessibility of spatial information in a situation model. Discourse Processes. 1999; 27:261–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. and Hasan, R. Cohesion in English. New York: Longman; 1976
Hamon, P. Pour un statut sémiologique du personnage. Littérature, 1972; 6:86–110CrossRef
Hamon, P. Pour un statut sémiologique du personnage. in: Barthes, R.; Kayser, W.; Booth, W. C.; and Hamon, P., editors. Poétique du récit. Paris: Editions du Seuil; 1977; pp. 115–80
Hartley, J.Invisible fictions: Television audiences, paedocracy, pleasure. Textual Practice. 1987; 1(2):121–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, W. J. Character and the novel. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1965
Hauptmeier, H.; Meutsch, D.; and Viehoff, P.Empirical research on understanding literature. Poetics Today. 1989; 10(3):563–604CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, S. E. and Clark, H. H.What's new? Acquiring new information in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1974; 13:512–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R. and Flower, L. S.Writing research and the writer. American Psychologist. 1986; 41:1106–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R.; Schriver, K. A.; Spilka, R; and Blaustein, A. If it's clear to me it must be clear to them. Paper presented at the Conference on College Composition and Communication; New Orleans, LA; 1986
Hemingway, E. The snows of Kilimanjaro. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons; 1927
Hirsch, E. D. The aims of interpretation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1976
Hochman, B. Character in literature. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1985
Holland, N. 5 readers reading. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1975
Holland, N. Recovering “The Purloined Letter”: Reading as a personal transaction. in: Saleiman, S. R. and Crossman, I., editors. The reader in the text: Essays on audience and interpretation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1980; pp. 350–70
Holland, N. Fantasy and defense in Faulkner's “A rose for Emily.” in: Staton, S., editor. Literary theories in praxis. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press; 1987; pp. 294–307
Holub, R. C. Reception theory: A critical introduction. London: Routledge; 1989
Irving, W. “The adventure of the German student.” in: Litz, A. W., editor. Major American short stories, 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1994; pp. 38–43
Iser, W. The implied reader: Patterns of communication in prose fiction from Bunyan to Beckett. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1974
Iser, W. The act of reading: A theory of aesthetic response. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1978
Iser, W. Interaction between text and reader. in: Suleiman, S. R. and Crossman, I., editors. The reader in the text: Essays on audience and interpretation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1980; pp. 10–19
Jahn, M.Frames, preferences, and the reading of third-person narratives: Towards a cognitive narratology. Poetics Today. 1997; 18(4):441–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauss, H. R.Literary history as a challenge to literary theory. New Literary History. 1970; 2(1):7–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauss, H. R.Levels of identification of hero and audience. New Literary History. 1974; 5(2):283–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1983
Johnson, N. S. and Mandler, J. M.A tale of two structures: Underlying and surface forms in stories. Poetics. 1980; 9:51–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, E. E. and Davis, K. From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. in: Berkowitz, L., editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1965; pp. 219–66
Jost, F.Narration(s): En decà et au-delà. Communications. 1983; 38:192–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, J. Dubliners. New York: Penguin Books; 1991
Just, M. A. and Carpenter, P. A. The psychology of reading and language comprehension. Boston: Allyn and Bacon; 1987
Kamau, K. A. Flickering shadows. New York: Henry Holt and Company; 1996
Kaschak, M. P. and Glenberg, A. M.Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000; 43:508–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, H. H.Attribution in social psychology: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 1967; 15:192–238
Kenny, D. A.A general model of consensus and accuracy in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review. 1991; 98:155–63CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keysar, B.The illusory transparency of intention: Linguistic perspective taking text. Cognitive Psychology. 1994; 26:165–208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kincaid, J. Marbles. in: Mordecai, P. and Wilson, B., editors. Her true-true name: An anthology of women's writing from the Caribbean. Oxford: Heinemann; 1990; pp. 112–16
Kintsch, W. The representation of meaning in memory. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1974
Kintsch, W. and Greene, E.The role of culture-specific schemata in the comprehension and recall of stories. Discourse Processes. 1978; 1:1–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W.;Kozminsky, E.; Streby, W. J.; McKoon, F.; and Keenan, J. M.Comprehension and recall of text as a function of content variables. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1975; 14:196–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knapp, J. V.Family systems as psychotherapy, literary character, and literature: An introduction. Style. 1997; 31(2):223–54Google Scholar
Knapp, J. V.Introduction: Self-preservation and self-transformation: Interdisciplinary approaches to literary character. Style. 1990; 24(3):349–64Google Scholar
Krieger, M. Arts on the level: The fall of the elite object. Knoxville: University of Tennessee Press; 1981
Kushigian, J. La economía de las palabras: Disipador del miedo inefable en Borges y Silvina Bullrich. Inti: Revista de Literatura Hispánica. 1989 Spring–1989 Fall; 29–30:207–214
Labov, W. Language in the Inner City: Studies in the Black English Vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania; 1972; pp. 354–96
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: Basic Books; 1999
Lamarque, P. Narrative and Invention: The Limits of Fictionality. in: Nash, C., editor. Narrative in culture: The uses of storytelling in the sciences, philosophy, and literature. London: University of Warwick Centre for Research in Philosophy and Literature; 1990; pp. 131–53
Lanser, S. The narrative act: Point of view in fiction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1981
Lanser, S.Sexing the narrative: Propriety, desire, and the engendering of narratology. Narrative. 1995; 3(1):85–94Google Scholar
Lardner, R. The love nest and other stories. New York: C. Scribner's Sons; 1926
Larsen, S. F. and Seilman, U.Personal remindings while reading literature. Text. 1988; 8:411–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, S. F. and Viehoff, R. Introduction: “Empirical” – What does it mean in IGEL studies? in: Rusch, G., editor. Emprical approaches to literature: Procedings of the fourth conference of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature – IGEL; Budapest. Siegen: Lumis Publications; 1995:21–27
Larsen, S. F.; László, J.; and Seilman, U. Across time and place: Cultural-historical knowledge and personal experience in appreciation of literature. in: Ibsch, E.; Schram, D.; and Steen, G., editors. Empirical studies of literature: Proceedings of the second IGEL Conference, Amsterdam, 1989. Amsterdam: Rodopi; 1991
le Carré, J. The naive and sentimental lover. London: Pan Books; 1972
le Carré, J. Smiley's people. London: Penguin Books; 1992
le Carré, J. Our game. Toronto: Penguin Books; 1995
le Carré, J. The tailor of Panama. Toronto: Penguin; 1997
Le Guin, U. K. “The wife's story.” in: The Compass Rose. New York: Bantam Books; 1983
Le Guin, U. K. Always coming home. Toronto: Bantam Books; 1985
Le Guin, U. K. Tehanu: The last book of Earthsea. New York: Atheneum; 1990
Lehnert, W. G. Plot units: A narrative summarization strategy. in: Lehnert, W. G. and Ringle, M. H., editors. Strategies for natural language processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1982; pp. 375–412
Lenneberg, E. The biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley; 1967
Levelt, W. J. M. Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1989
Levelt, W. J. M. Perspective taking and ellipsis in spatial descriptions. in: Bloom, M. A.; Peterson, N. L.; and Garret, M. F. Language and space. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1996; pp. 77–108
Levine, W. H. and Klin, C. M.Tracking of spatial components of situation models. Memory & Cognition. 2001; 29:327–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Limon, J. The place of fiction in the time of science: A disciplinary history of American writing. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990
Linde, C. and Labov, W.Spatial netwoks as a site for the study of language and thought. Language. 1975; 51:924–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, G. R.A picture is worth a thousand p values: On the irrelevance of hypothesis testing in the microcomputer age. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 1993; 25:250–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, D. L. and Ley, L.Implicit causality and discourse focus: The interaction of text and reader characteristics in pronoun resolution. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000; 42:545–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, I. H.And there is only my imagination where our history should be: An interview with Cristina García. Michigan Quarterly Review. 1994; 33:3Google Scholar
Mackie, J. L. The cement of the universe: A study of causation. Oxford: Clarendon; 1980
Magliano, J. P.; Dijkstra, K.; and Zwann, R. A.Generating predictive inferences while viewing a movie. Discourse Processes. 1996; 22:199-224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Makaryk, I. R., editor and compiler. Encyclopedia of contemporary literary theory: Approaches, scholars, terms. Toronto: University of Toronto Press; 1993
Mandler, J. M.A code in the node: The use of a story schema in retrieval. Discourse Processes. 1978; 1:14–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M. Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1984
Mandler, J. M. and Goodman, M. S.On the psychological reality of story structure. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1982; 21:507–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.Characterization in narrative: Some theoretical prolegomena. Neophilologus. 1983; 67:1–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.The doer and the deed: Action as basis for characterization in narrative. Poetics Today. 1986; 7(2):205–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.Structuralist approaches to character in narrative: The state of the art. Semiotica. 1989; 75(1/2):1–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.Individuals in narrative worlds: An ontological perspective. Poetics Today. 1990a; 11(4):843–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.The what, the when, and the how of being a character in literary narrative. Style. 1990b; 24(3):453–68Google Scholar
Margolin, U. Fictional individuals and their counterparts. Andrew, J., editor. Studies in Slavic Literature and Poetics. 1992; 1743–56
Margolin, U.Changing individuals in narrative: Science, philosophy, literature. Semiotica. 1995; 107(12):5–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.Characters in literary narrative: Representation and signification. Semiotica. 1998; 106(3/4):373–92Google Scholar
McCutchen, D. and Perfetti, C. A.The visual tongue-twister effect: Phonological activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1982; 2:672–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDaniel, M. A.; Hines, R. J.; Waddil, P. J.; and Einstein, G. O.What makes folk tales unique: Content familiarity, causal structure, scripts, or superstructures?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1994; 20:169–84Google ScholarPubMed
McHale, B.Free indirect discourse: A survey of recent accounts. PTL: A Journal for Descriptive Poetics and Theory of Literature. 1978; 3:249–87Google Scholar
McHale, B.Unspeakable sentences, unnatural acts: Linguistics and poetics revisited. Poetics Today. 1983; 4(1):17–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKoon, G. and Ratcliff, R.Inference during reading. Psychological Review. 1992; 99:440–66CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McRobbie, A. Feminism and youth culture. Cambridge, MA: Hyman; 1990
Medin, D. L. and Schaffer, M. M.Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review. 1978; 85:207–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, B. J. F. The organization of prose and its effect on memory. Amsterdam: North Holland; 1975
Miall, D. S. and Kuiken, D.Aspects of literary response: A new questionnaire. Research in the Teaching of English. 1995; 29:37–58Google Scholar
Moore, R. E. Performance form and the voices of characters in five versions of the Wasco Coyote cycle. in: Lucy, J. A., editor. Reflexive language: Reported speech and metapragmatics. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1993
Morrison, T. Beloved. New York: Plume, Penguin Putnam; 1998
Morrow, D. G.; Bower, G. H.; and Greenspan, S. L.Updating situation models during narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 1989; 28:292–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, D. G.; Greenspan, S. L.; and Bower, G. H., Accessibility and situation models in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 1987(26):165–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mukarovsky, J. Aesthetic function, norm and value as social facts. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press; 1970
Munro, A. The office. in: Munro, A., editor. Collected short stories. New York: Random House; 1996; pp. 59–74
Murillo, L. A. The Cyclical Night: Irony in James Joyce and Jorge Luis Borges. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1968
Myers, J. L. and O'Brien, E. J.Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes. 1998; 26:131–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nash, C. Narrative in culture. London: Routledge; 1994
Nelles, W.Getting focalization into focus. Poetics Today. 1990; 11(2):365–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. and Simon, H. A. Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1972
Neyman, J. and Pearson, E. S. On the use and interpretation of certain test criteria for purposes of statistical inference. Biometrika. 1928; 20A:263–94
Nosofsky, R. M.Tests of an exemplar model for relating perceptual classification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1991; 17:3–27Google ScholarPubMed
Oatley, K.A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative. Poetics. 1994; 23:53–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, E. J. and Albrecht, J. E.Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1992; 18:777–84Google ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, E. J.; Lorch, R. F. Jr.; and Myers, J. L.Memory-based text processing. Discourse Processes. 1998; 26:2–3Google Scholar
Olson, G. M.; Mack, R. L., and Duffy, S. A.Cognitive aspects of genre. Poetics. 1981; 10:283–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oltean, S.Functions of free indirect discourse: The case of a novel. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique. 1986; 31:153–64Google Scholar
Oltean, S.A survey of the pragmatic and referential functions of free indirect discourse. Poetics Today. 1993; 14(4):691–714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omanson, R. C.An analysis of narratives: Identifying central, supportive, and distracting content. Discourse Processing. 1982; 5:195–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, P. Points of origin: On focalization in narrative. Canadian Review of Comparative Literature. 1992; Sept:331–50
O'Neill, P. Fictions of discourse: Reading narrative theory. Toronto: University of Toronto Press; 1994
Ortega, R. P.Intento de interpretación psicoanalítica de un cuento de Jorge Luis Borges. Eco. 1971; 23(138–139):587–99Google Scholar
Pavel, T. G. Fictional worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1986
Phelan, J. Reading people, reading plots: Character, progression, and the interpretation of narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1989
Pinker, S. The language instinct. New York: Morrow; 1994
Piwowarczyk, M. A.The narratee and the situation of enunciation: A reconsideration of Prince's theory. Genre. 1976; 9:161–77Google Scholar
Polanyi, L. Literary complexity in everyday storytelling. in: Tannen, D., editor. Spoken and written language: Exploring orality and literacy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1982
Pollard-Gott, L.Attribution theory and the novel. Poetics. 1993; 21:499–524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porter, K. A. Rope. in: Cahill, S., editor. Women and fiction: Short stories by and about women. New York: New American Library; 1975; pp. 78–84
Postman, L. and Senders, V. L.Incidental learning and generality of set. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1946; 36:153–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, J. and Wetherell, M. Discourse and social psychology. London: Sage; 1987
Prince, G. A grammar of stories. The Hague: Mouton; 1973
Prince, G. Dictionary of narratology. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1987
Prince, G. Narratology: The form and functioning of narrative. Amsterdam: Mouton; 1982
Prince, G. On narratology: Past, present, future. French Literary Studies. 1990; XVII
Propp, V. Morphologie du conte. Derrida, M.; Todorov, T.; and Kahn, C., translators. Paris: Editions du Seuil; 1965
Propp, V. Morphology of the folktale. Austin: University of Texas; 1968
Raap, D. N.; Gerrig, R. J.; and Prentice, D. A.Readers' trait-based models of characters in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 2001;45:737–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabinowitz, P. J. Other reader-oriented theories. in: Selden, R., editor. The Cambridge history of literary criticism: From formalism to poststructuralism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1995; 375–403
Ramazani, V. The free indirect mode: Flaubert and the poetics of irony. Charlottesville: Univeristy Press of Virginia; 1988
Reeder, G. D. and Brewer, M. B.A schematic model of dispositional attribution in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review. 1979; 86:61–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richard, J.-P. Littérature et sensation. Paris: Aux Editions du Seuil; 1954
Richards, E. and Singer, M.Representation of complex goal structures in narrative comprehension. Discourse Processes. 2001; 31:111–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, B.Beyond poststructuralism: Theory of character, the personae of modern drama, and the antinomies of critical theory. Modern Drama. 1997; 40:86–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ricoeur, P. Temps et récit. Paris: Seuil; 1983
Riffaterre, M.Describing poetic structures: Two approaches to Baudelaire's “Les Chats.”Yale French Studies. 1966; 36/37:200–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimmon-Kenan, S. Narrative fiction: Contemporary poetics. New York: Methuen; 1983
Rosch, E. and Mervis, C. B.Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology. 1975; 7:573–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, L. The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. in: Berkowitz, L., editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1977; pp. 173–220
Ross, L. D.; Amabile, T. M.; and Steinmetz, J. L.Social roles, social cognition, and biases in social-perception processes. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 1977; 35:485–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowling, J. K. Harry Potter and the goblet of fire. London: Raincoast Books; 2000
Rumelhart, D. E. Notes on a schema for stories. in: Bobrow, D. G. and Collins, A., editors. Representation and understanding: Studies in cognitive science. New York: Academic Press; 1975; pp. 211–36
Rusch, G. The notion of “empirical”: Knowing how. in: Rusch, G., editor. Empirical approaches to literature: Proceedings of the fourth biannual conference of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature – IGEL; Budapest. Siegen: Lumis Publications; 1995: pp. 103–8
Sanders, J. and Redeker, G. Perspective and the representation of speech and thought. in: Fauconnier, G., editor. Worlds and grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1996; pp. 290–317
Sanford, A. J. and Garrod, S. Understanding written language: Explorations in comprehension beyond the sentence. Chichester: Wiley; 1981
Sanford, A. J. and Garrod, S. C.The role of scenario mapping in text comprehension. Discourse Processes. 1998; 26:159–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. C. and Abelson, R. P. Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977
Schegloff, E. A.; Jefferson, G.; and Sacks, H.The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language. 1977; 53:361–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. J.Receptional problems with contemporary narrative texts and some of their reasons. Poetics. 1980; 9:119–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. J.Empirical studies in literature: Introductory remarks. Poetics. 1981; 10:317–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. J.The empirical science of literature ESL: A new paradigm. Poetics. 1983; 12:19–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. J. What can “empirical” mean in a constructivist context? 10 considerations. in: Rusch, G., editor. Empirical approaches to literature: Proceedings of the fourth biannual conference of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature – IGEL; Budapest. Siegen: Lumis Publications; 1995: 109–112
Schober, M. F.Spatial perspective-taking in conversation. Cognition. 1993; 47:1–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, M. S. and McClelland, J. L.A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review. 1989; 96:523–68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selden, R. The Cambridge history of literary criticism: From formalism to poststructuralism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1995
Shapiro, M.How narrators report speech. Language and Style. 1984; 17(1):67–78Google Scholar
Shaw, H. E.Thin description: A reply to Seymour Chatman. Narrative. 1995; 3(3):307–14Google Scholar
Shepard, L. Life during wartime. New York: Bantam Books; 1987
Shklovsky, V. Art as technique. in: Lemon, L. T. and Reis, M. J., editors. Russian formalist criticism: Four essays. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1965; pp. 3–24
Sidner, C.Focusing and discourse. Discourse Processes. 1983; 6:107–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair-de Zwart, H. Language acquisition and cognitive development. in: Moore, T., editor. Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press; 1973
Singer, M. and Ferreira, F.Inferring consequences in story comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1983; 22:437–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slatoff, W. With respect to readers: Dimensions of literary response. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1970
Sophocles. Oedipus the king and Antigone. Arnott, P. D., translator. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1969
Spilich, G. J.; Vesonder, G. T.; Chiesi, H. L.; and Voss, J. F.Text processing of domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1979; 18:275–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spolsky, E. Gaps in nature: Literary interpretation and the modular mind. Albany: State University of New York Press; 1993
Stampfl, B.Filtering Rimmon-Kenan, Chatman, Black, Freud, and James: Focalization and the divided self in “The beast in the jungle.”Style. 1992; 26(3):388–99Google Scholar
Stanzel, F. K. Teller-characters and reflector-characters in narrative theory. Poetics Today. 1981; 2(2):4–15CrossRef
Stein, N. L. and Glenn, C. G. An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. in: Le Freed, R., editor. New directions in discourse processing (Vol. 2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1979; pp. 53–120
Suleiman, S. and Crosman, I. The reader in the text: Essays on audience and interpretation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1980
Symonds, J. A., On the application of evolutionary principles to art and literature. in: Essays speculative and suggestive. New York: AMS Press; 1970; pp. 197–224
Tan, E. S. H.Film-induced affect as a witness emotion. Poetics. 1994; 23:7–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D.Oral and literate strategies in spoken and written narratives. Language. 1982; 58(1):1–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D. Conversational style. Analyzing talk among friends. Norwood: Ablex; 1984
Taylor, H. A. and Tversky, B.Perspective in spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language. 1996; 35:371–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndyke, P. W.Cognitive structure in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive Psychology. 1977; 9:51–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thury, E. M. and Friedlander, A. The impact of expertise: The role of experience in reading literary texts. in: Rusch, G., editor. Empirical approaches to literature: Proceedings of the fourth biannual conference of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature – IGEL; Budapest. Siegen: Lumis Publications; 1995; p. 22
Tolliver, J. Discourse analysis and the interpretation of literary narrative. Style. 1990; 24(2):266–83
Tomachevsky, B. Thématique. in: Todorov, T., editor and translator. Théorie de la littérature. Paris: Aux Editions du Seuil; 1965
Toolan, M. J. Narrative: A critical linguistic introduction. New York: Routledge; 1995
Trabasso, R. and Sperry, L. L.Causal relatedness and importance of story events. Journal of Memory and Language. 1985; 24:595–611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T. and Broek, P.Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language. 1985; 24:612–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. and Burnstein, E.Processing the information contained in another's behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1975; 11:439–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M. Reading minds: The study of English in the age of cognitive science. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1991
Tversky, B. Spatial perspective in descriptions. in: Bloom, P.; Barret, M.; Hadel, L.; and Peterson, M., editors. Language and Space. Boston: MIT Press; 1996; pp. 463–91
Uleman, J. S.; Hon, A.;Roman, R. J.; and Moskowitz, G. B.On-line evidence for spontaneous trait inferences at encoding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1996; 22:377–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Broek, P.; Risden, K.; Fletcher, C. R.; and Thurlow, R. A. “Landscape” view of reading: Fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a stable memory representation. in: Britton, B. K. and Graesser, A. C., editors. Models of understanding text. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1996; pp. 165–87
Dijk, T. A.Philosophy of action and theory of narrative. Poetics. 1976; 5Google Scholar
van Dijk, T. A. and Kintsch, W. Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press; 1983
Vipond, D. and Hunt, R. A. Literary processing and response as transactions: Evidence for the contribution of readers, texts, and situation. in: Meutsch, D. and Viehoff, R., editors. Comprehension of literary discourse: Interdisciplinary approaches. Berlin: de Gruyter; 1989; pp. 155–74
Vitoux, P.Le jeu de la focalisation. Poétique. 1982; 51:359–68Google Scholar
Vitoux, P.Notes sur la focalisation dans le roman autobiographique. Etudes Littéraires. 1984; 17(2):261–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, R.Who is the narrator?Poetics Today. 1997a; 18(4):495–513CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, R.Why we wept for Little Nell: Character and emotional involvment. Narrative. 1997b; 5(3):306–21Google Scholar
Warren, W. H.; Nicholas, D. W.; and Trabasso, T. Event chains and inferences in understanding narratives. in: Freedle, R. O., editor. Advances in discourse processes: New directions in discourse processing, Vol. 2. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1979
Weinberg, H. H.Irony and “style indirect libre” in Madame Bovary. Canadian Review of Comparative Literature. 1981; 8(1):1–9Google Scholar
Weitz, M.The role of theory in aesthetics. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism. 1956; 15(1):27–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weitz, M. The opening mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1977
Wellek, R. The concept of evolution in literary history. in: Wellek, R., editor. Concepts of criticism. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1973
Werth, P. N. How to build a world (in a lot less than six days, and using only what's in your head). in: Green, K., editor. New essays in deixis: Discourse, narrative, literature. Amsterdam: Rodopi; 1995; pp. 49–80
White, H. The value of narrativity in the representation of reality. in: Mitchell, W. J. T., editor. On narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1981; pp. 1–24
Whitehurst, G. Language development. in: Wolman, B., editor. Handbook of developmental psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1982
Wiebe, J. M. Recognizing subjective sentences: A computational analysis of narrative text. Buffalo, NY: SUNY, Department of Computer Science; 1990
Wieder, L. Telling the code. in: Turner, R., editor. Ethnomethodology. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin; 1974
Wike, E. L. and Church, J. D.Comments on Clark's “The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1976; 15:249–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilder, D.Perceiving persons as a group: Effects on attributions of causality and beliefs. Social Psychology. 1978; 1:13–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, S. G.; Rinck, M.; McNamara, T. P.; Bower, G. H.; and Morrow, D. G.Mental models and narrative comprehension: Some qualifications. Journal of Memory and Language. 1993; 32:141–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winograd, T. A framework for understanding discourse. in: Just, M. A. and Carp, P. A. Cognitive processes in comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977; pp. 63–88
Wolff, E.Der intendierte Leser: Uberlegungen und Beispiele zur Einfuhrung eines literaturwissenschaftlichen Begriffes. Poetica. 1971; 4(2):141–66Google Scholar
Zwann, R.Effect of genre expectations on text comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1994; 20:920–33Google Scholar
Zwann, R. A.; Langston, M. C.; and Graesser, A. C.The construction of situation models in narrative comprehension: An event-indexing model. Psychological Science. 1995; 6:292–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, D. The hitchhikers guide to the galaxy. London: Pan Books; 1979
Ajzen, I., and Fishbein, M. A.Bayesian analysis of attribution processes. Psychological Bulletin. 1975; 82:21–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, N. H. Foundations of information integration theory. New York: Academic Press; 1981
Anderson, R. C. and Pichert, J.Recall of previously unrecallable information following a shift in perspective. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1978; 17:1–12CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andringa, E.; Tan, E.; van Horssen, P.; and Jacobs, A. Effects of focalization in film narration. in: Totosy de Zepetnek, T. and Sywenky, I., editors. The systemic and empirical approach to literature and culture as theory and application. Edmonton: University of Alberta; 1997; pp. 203–15
Argueta, R. One day of life, Vintage International Edition, 1991, translated from original, Un Día en la Vida, San Salvador: UCA Editores; 1980
Aumonier, S. “Miss Bracegirdle does her duty.” in: Priestley, J. B., editor. Great British short stories. London: Reader's Digest Association; 1974; pp. 26–39
Baddeley, A. Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon; 1986
Bal, Mieke. The narrating and the focalizing: A theory of the agents in narrative. Style. 1983; 17(2):234–69Google Scholar
Bal, Mieke. Narratology: Introduction to the theory of narrative. Boheemen, C. van, translator. Toronto: University of Toronto Press; 1985
Bally, C.Le style indirect libre en français moderne. Germanisch-Romanisch Monatsschrift. 1912; 4(549–56):597–606Google Scholar
Banfield, A.Narrative style and the grammar of direct and indirect speech. Foundations of Language. 1973; 10:1–39Google Scholar
Banfield, A.Reflective and non-reflective consciousness in the language of fiction. Poetics Today. 1981; 2(2):61–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banfield, A. Where epistemology, style, and grammar meet literary history. in: Lucy, J. A., editor. Reflexive language: Reported speech and metapragmatics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1993;(3):339–64
Barthes, R.Introduction à l'analyse structurale des récets. Communications. 1966: 8:1–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barthes, R. Introduction à l'analyse structurale des récits. Barthes, R.; Kayser, W.; Booth, W. C., and Hamon, P., editors. Poétique du récit. Paris: Edition du Seuil; 1977; pp. 7–57
Bartlett, F. C. Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1932
Bates, E. The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press; 1979
Bauer, W., et al. Text und Rezeption: Wirkungsanalyse Zeitgenössischer Lyrik am Beispiel des Gedichtes “Fadensonne” von Paul Celan. Frankfurt: Athenäum; 1972
Beach, R. A teacher's introduction to reader-response theories. Urbana, IL: National Council of Teachers of English; 1993
Belli, G. The inhabited woman. March, K., translator. Willimantic, CT: Warner Books Edition; 1995
Black, J. B. and Wilensky, R.An evaluation of story grammars. Cognitive Science. 1979; 3:213–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, J. B.; Turner, T. J. and Bower, G. H.Point of view in narrative comprehension, memory, and production. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1979; 18:187–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bleich, D. Subjective criticism. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press; 1978
Booth, W. The rhetoric of fiction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1961
Bordwell, D. Narration in the fiction film. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press; 1985
Borges, J. L. “Emma Zunz.” in: Borges, J. L. Labyrinths. New York: Modern Library; 1983; pp. 132–7
Bortolussi, M. and Dixon, P.The effects of formal training on literary reception. Poetics Today. 1996; 23:471–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bortolussi, M. Science and the study of literature. Rusch, G., editor. Spiel: Siegener Periodicum Zur Internationalen Empirischen Literaturwissenschaft. 1998; 16(1/2):67–70
Bortolussi, M. A psychonarratological revision of the theory of focalization. Language and Style. in press
Bower, G. H.Experiments on story understanding and recall. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1976; 28:511–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G. H.; Black, J. B.; and Turner, T. J.Scripts in memory for text. Cognitive Psychology. 1979; 11:177–220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremond, Claude. La logique des possibles narratifs. Communications. 1966; 8:60–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremond, Claude. Logique du récit. Paris: Seuil, 1973
Bremond, ClaudeThe logic of narrative possibilities. New Literary History. 1980; 11:398–411CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bremond, Claude. A critique of the motif. Todorov, T., editor. French literary theory today. Carter, R., translator. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1982; pp. 124–46
Brodzki, B.She was unable not to think: Borges' “Emma Zunz” and the female subject. MLA. 1985; 100(2):330–47Google Scholar
Bronzwaer, W.Mieke Bal's concept of focalization: A critical note. Poetics Today. 1981; 2(2):193–201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, N. R.Estimation strategies and the judgment of event frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1995; 21:1539–53Google Scholar
Bruce, B.A social interaction model of reading. Discourse Processes. 1981; 4:273–311CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, D. J.; Tversky, B.; and Franklin, N.Internal and external spatial frameworks for representing described scenes. Journal of Memory and Language. 1992; 31:74–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burch, N. To the distant observer. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press; 1979
Card, O. S. Ender's game. New York: Tom Doherty Associates; 1977
Carpenter, P. A. and Just, M. A., Integrative processes in comprehension. in: LaBerge, D. and Samuels, J., editors. Basic processes in reading: Perception and comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977a
Carpenter, P. A. and Just, M. A. Reading comprehension as eyes see it. in: Just, M. A. and Carpenter, P. A., editors. Cognitive processes in comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977b
Carr, E. Klee Wyck. Toronto: Irwin Publishing; 1941
Carter, A. Nights at the circus. London: Vintage; 1994
Carter, A. Black Venus. London: Vintage; 1996
Cather, W. “Paul's Case.” in: Cather, Willa. Five stories. New York: Vintage Books; 1959; pp. 149–74
Cela, J. C. The Family of Pascual Duarte. Kerrigan, A., translator. Boston: Little, Brown and Co.; 1964
Chafe, W. L. Discourse structure and human knowledge. in: Freedle, W. O. and Carroll, J. B., editors. Language comprehension and the acquisition of knowledge. Washington, DC: Winston & Sons; 1972
Chafe, W. L. Some things that narratives tell us about the mind. in: Britton, B. K. and Pellegrini, A. D., editors. Narrative thought and narrative language. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1990; pp. 79–98
Chang, F.Active memory processes in visual sentence comprehension: Clause effects and pronomial reference. Memory and Cognition. 1980; 8:58–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatman, S.On the formalist–structuralist theory of character. Journal of Literary Semantics. 1972; 1:57–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatman, S. Story and discourse: Narrative structure in fiction and film. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1978
Chatman, S.Characters and narrators: Filter, center, slant, and interest-focus. Poetics Today. 1986; 7(2):189–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chatman, S. Coming to terms. Ithaca: Cornell University Press; 1990
Chatman, S.How loose can narrators get? (And how vulnerable can narratees be?). Narrative. 1995; 3(3):3Google Scholar
Cherryh, C. J. Cyteen: The betrayal. New York: Popular Library; 1988
Cherryh, C. J. Fortress of owls. New York: Harpe Prism; 1999
Chomsky, N. Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton; 1957
Chomsky, N.Review of Skinner's Verbal Behavior. Language. 1959; 35:26–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N. Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1965
Clark, H. H.The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1973; 12:335–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1996
Clark, H. H. and Carlson, T. B.Hearers and speech acts. Language. 1982; 58:332–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Chase, W. G.On the process of comparing sentences against pictures. Cognitive Psychology. 1972; 3:472–517CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Gerrig, R. J.On the pretense theory of irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1984; 113:121–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, H. H. and Gerrig, R. J.Quotations as demonstrations. Language. 1990; 66:764–805CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H. and Schaefer, E. F.Contributing to discourse. Cognitive Science. 1989; 13:259–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, H. H.; Cohen, J.;Smith, J. E. K.; and Keppel, G.Discussion of Wike and Church's comments. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1976; 15:257–66Google Scholar
Cohen, J.The Earth is round (p <.05). American Psychologist. 1994; 49:997–1003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohn, D. Transparent minds: Narrative modes for presenting consciousness in fiction. 1978: Princeton: Princeton University Press
Corcoran, M. G.Language, character, and gender in the direct discourse of Dubliners. Style. 1991; 25(3):439–53Google Scholar
Coste, D. Narrative as communication. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota; 1989
Coulmas, F. Direct and Indirect Speech. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyère; 1986
Craig, C. “In the hills.” in: Mordecai, P. and Wilson, B., editors. Her true-true name: An anthology of women's writing from the Caribbean. Oxford: Heinemann; 1990; pp. 56–9
Craik, F. I. M. and Lockhart, R. S.Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior. 1972; 11:671–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craik, F. I. M. and Watkins, M. J.The role of rehearsal in short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1973; 12:599–607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Culler, J. Defining narrative units. in: Fowler, R., editor. Style and structure in literature: Essays in the new stylistics. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1975a
Culler, J. Structuralist poetics: Structuralism, linguistics, and the study of literature. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul; 1975b
Cupchik, G. C. Identification as a basic problem for aesthetic reception. in: Tötösy de Zepetnek, S. and Sywenky, I., editors. The systemic and empirical approach to literature and culture as theory and application. Edmonton: University of Alberta Press; 1997; pp. 11–22
Cupchik, G. C.; Oatley, K.; and Vorderer, L.Emotional effects of reading excerpts from short stories by James Joyce. Poetics. 1998; 25:363–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danov, D.Dialogic structures in Crime and Punishment. Russian, Croatian and Serbian, Czech and Slovak, Polish Literature. 1986; 19(3):291–314Google Scholar
Deighton, L. Berlin game. London: Grafton Books; 1984
de Man, P. Blindness and insight: Essays in the rhetoric of comparative criticism. Minneapolis: University of Minneapolis Press; 1983
Dillon, G. Language processing and the reading of literature. Bloomington: Indiana University Press; 1978
Dingwall, W. The species-specificity of speech. in: Dato, D. P., editor. Developmental psycholinguistics: Theory and applications. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press; 1975
Dixon, P.Why scientists value p values. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 1998; 5:390–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P. and Bortolussi, M.Literary communication: Effects of reader-narrator co-operation. Poetics. 1996a; 23(6):405–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P. The narrator in the text and the narrator in the reader. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature. 1996b; Nakoda Lodge, Alberta, Canada
Dixon, P. I metodi della psiconarratologia. in: Nemesio, A., editor. L'esperienza del testo. Rome: Meltemi Editori; 1999c; pp. 126–43
Dixon, P. Narratorial relevance and character goals. Paper presented at the meeting of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature. 2000; Toronto
Dixon, P.Text is not communication: A challenge to a common assumption. Discourse Processes. 2001a; 31(1):1–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P. Prolegomena for a science of psychonarratology. in: W. van Peer and S. Chatman, editors. New perspectives on narrative perspective. Albany, NY: State University of New York; 2001b; pp. 275–88
Dixon, P. and O'Reilly, T.Scientific versus statistical inference. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1999; 53:133–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, P.; Bortolussi, M.; Twilley, L. C.; and Leung, A.Literary processing and interpretation: Towards empirical foundations. Poetics. 1993; 22:5–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doležel, L. Possible worlds and literary fictions. in: Allén, S., editor. Possible worlds in humanities, arts and sciences. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter; 1989; pp. 221–42
Dorfman, M. H.Evaluating the interpretive community: Evidence from expert and novice readers. Poetics. 1996; 23:453–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dressler, W. U.The cognitive perspective of ‘naturalist’ linguistic models. Cognitive Linguistics. 1990; 1:75–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duchan, J. F.; Bruder, G. A.; and Hewitt, L. E. Deixis in Narrative. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1995
Ebert, T. L. The romance of patriarchy: Ideology, subjectivity, and postmodern feminist cultural theory. Cultural Critique. 1988; 10
Eco, U. The role of the reader: Explorations in the semiotics of texts. Bloomington: Indiana University Press; 1979
Edmiston, W. F.Focalization and the first-person narrator: A revision of the theory. Poetics Today. 1989; 10(4):729–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, A. W. F. Likelihood. Baltimore: John Hopkins Univeristy; 1992
Einstein, G. O.; McDaniel, M. A.; Owen, P. D.; and Coté, N. C.Encoding and recall of texts: The importance of material appropriate processing. Journal of Memory and Language. 1990; 29:566–582CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellison, R. Invisible man. New York: Random House; 1952
Emberson, J.Reported speech in medieval German narrative. Paregon: Bulletin of the Australian and New Zealand Association for Medieval and Renaissance Studies. 1986; 4:103–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmott, C. Narrative comprehension: A discourse perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1997
Ericsson, K. A. and Kintsch, W.Long term working memory. Psychological Review. 1995; 102:211–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erlich, S. Point of view: A linguistic analysis of literary style. London & New York: Routledge; 1990
Faulkner, W. Honor. Collected stories of William Faulkner. New York: Random House; 1950; pp. 551–64
Faulkner, W. Honor. A rose for Emily. Selected short stories of William Faulkner. New York: The Modern Library; 1961; pp. 49–61
Fetterley, J. The resisting reader: A feminist aproach to American fiction. Bloomington: Indiana University Press; 1977
Findley, T. Not Wanted on the Voyage. Toronto: Penguin Books; 1984
Fisch, H.Character as linguistic sign. New Literary History. 1990; 21:593–606CrossRef
Fish, S.Literature in the reader: Affective stylistics. New Literary History. 1970; 2:123–62CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fish, S. Is there a text in this class: The authority of interpretive communities. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1980
Fish, S.Being interdisciplinary is so very hard to do. Profession. 1989; 89:15–22Google Scholar
Fishelov, D.Types of characters, characteristics of types. Style. 1990; 3:422–39Google Scholar
Fishelov, D. Metaphors of genre: The role of analogies in genre theory. State College, PA: Pennsylvania State University; 1993
Fisher, R. A. Statistical methods for research workers. Edinburgh: Oliver & Boyd; 1925
Fisher, R. A.Statistical methods and scientific induction. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, Series B. 1955; 17:69–78Google Scholar
Flaubert, G. Madame Bovary: A story of provincial life. Russell, A., translator. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books; 1950
Fletcher, C. R.Strategies for the allocation of short-term memory during comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 1986; 25:43–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, C. R. and Bloom, C. P.Causal reasoning in the comprehension of simple narrative texts. Journal of Memory and Language. 1988; 27:235–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fludernik, M. The fictions of language and the languages of fiction: The linguistic representation of speech and consciousness. London: Routledge; 1993
Fludernik, M. Towards a “natural” narratology. London: Routledge; 1996
Forster, E. M. Aspects of the novel. London: Edward Arnold; 1974
Fort, B. Manon's suppressed voice: The uses of reported speech. Romanic Review. 1985; 76(2):172–91
Fowler, A. Kinds of literature: An introduction to the theory of genres and modes. Boston, MA: Harvard University Press; 1982
Franklin, N. and Tversky, B.Searching imagined environments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 1990; 119:63–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Franklin, N.; Tversky, B.; and Coon, V.Switching points of view in spatial mental models. Memory & Cognition. 1992; 20:507–18CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frazier, L. and Rayner, K.Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology. 1982; 14:178–210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frow, J.Spectacle binding: On character. Poetics Today. 1986; 7(2):227–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
García, C. Dreaming in Cuban, New York: Ballantine Books; 1992
Genette, G. Figures III. Paris: Editions du Seuil; 1973
Genette, G. Narrative discourse. Oxford: Basil Blackwell; 1980
Genette, G. Narrative discourse: An essay in method. McDowell, R. S. and Velie, A., translators. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1983
Gergen, K. J.Textual considerations in the scientific construction of human character. Style. 1990; 24(3):17–31Google Scholar
Gerrig, R. J. Experiencing narrative worlds. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1993
Gerrig, R. J. and Allbritton, D. W.The construction of literary character: A view from cognitive psychology. Style. 1990; 24:380–91Google Scholar
Gerrig, R. J.; Brennan, S. E.; and Ohaeri, J. O.What can we conclude from speakers behaving badly?Discourse Processes. 2000; 29:173–8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerrig, R. J.; Ohaeri, J. O; and Brennan, S. E.Illusory transparency revisited. Discourse processes. 2000; 29:137–159CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R.Comprehending figurative referential descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition. 1990; 16:56–66Google ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R. Intentions in the experience of meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1999
Gibson, W. All tomorrow's parties. New York: Ace Books; 1999
Glanzer, M.; Fischer, B.; and Dorfman, D.Short-term storage in reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1984; 23:467–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenn, C. G.The rules of episodic structure and of story length in children's recall of simple stories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1978; 17:229–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M.What memory is for. Behavioural and Brain Sciences. 1997; 20:1–55Google ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, A. M. and Robertson, D. A.Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes. 1999; 28:1–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M.; Robertson, D. A.; and Members of the Honors Seminar in Cognitive Psychology. Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000; 43:379–401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M.; Meyer, M.; and Lindem, K.Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 1987; 26:69–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodman, S. N. and Royall, R.Evidence and scientific research. American Journal of Public Health. 1988; 78:1568–74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graesser, A. C.; Bowers, C.; Olde, B.; and Pomeroy, V.Who said what? Source memory for narrator and character agents in literary short stories. Journal of Educational Psychology. 1999; 91:284–300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C.; Bowers, C. A.; Olde, B.; White, K.; and Person, N. K.Who knows what? Propagation of knowledge among agents in a literary storyworld. Poetics. 1999; 26:143–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graesser, A. C.; Singer, M.; and Trabasso, T.Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review. 1994; 101:371–95CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graves, B. and Frederiksen, C. H.Literary expertise in the description of a fictional narrative. Poetics. 1991; 20:1–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greimas, A. J.Narrative grammars: Units and levels. MLN, 1971; 86:793-806CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. Logic and conversation. in: Cole, P. and Morgan, J. L., editors. Syntax and semantics: Speech acts. New York: Academic Press; 1975; pp. 41–58
Gross, J. B.A telling side of narration: Direct discourse and French women writers. The French Review. 1993; 66(3):401–11Google Scholar
Gross, S.Cognitive readings; or, the disappearance of Literature in the Mind. Poetics Today. 1997; 18(2):271–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hakala, C.Accessibility of spatial information in a situation model. Discourse Processes. 1999; 27:261–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, M. A. K. and Hasan, R. Cohesion in English. New York: Longman; 1976
Hamon, P. Pour un statut sémiologique du personnage. Littérature, 1972; 6:86–110CrossRef
Hamon, P. Pour un statut sémiologique du personnage. in: Barthes, R.; Kayser, W.; Booth, W. C.; and Hamon, P., editors. Poétique du récit. Paris: Editions du Seuil; 1977; pp. 115–80
Hartley, J.Invisible fictions: Television audiences, paedocracy, pleasure. Textual Practice. 1987; 1(2):121–138CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, W. J. Character and the novel. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1965
Hauptmeier, H.; Meutsch, D.; and Viehoff, P.Empirical research on understanding literature. Poetics Today. 1989; 10(3):563–604CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haviland, S. E. and Clark, H. H.What's new? Acquiring new information in comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1974; 13:512–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R. and Flower, L. S.Writing research and the writer. American Psychologist. 1986; 41:1106–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, J. R.; Schriver, K. A.; Spilka, R; and Blaustein, A. If it's clear to me it must be clear to them. Paper presented at the Conference on College Composition and Communication; New Orleans, LA; 1986
Hemingway, E. The snows of Kilimanjaro. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons; 1927
Hirsch, E. D. The aims of interpretation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1976
Hochman, B. Character in literature. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1985
Holland, N. 5 readers reading. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1975
Holland, N. Recovering “The Purloined Letter”: Reading as a personal transaction. in: Saleiman, S. R. and Crossman, I., editors. The reader in the text: Essays on audience and interpretation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1980; pp. 350–70
Holland, N. Fantasy and defense in Faulkner's “A rose for Emily.” in: Staton, S., editor. Literary theories in praxis. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press; 1987; pp. 294–307
Holub, R. C. Reception theory: A critical introduction. London: Routledge; 1989
Irving, W. “The adventure of the German student.” in: Litz, A. W., editor. Major American short stories, 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press; 1994; pp. 38–43
Iser, W. The implied reader: Patterns of communication in prose fiction from Bunyan to Beckett. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1974
Iser, W. The act of reading: A theory of aesthetic response. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press; 1978
Iser, W. Interaction between text and reader. in: Suleiman, S. R. and Crossman, I., editors. The reader in the text: Essays on audience and interpretation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1980; pp. 10–19
Jahn, M.Frames, preferences, and the reading of third-person narratives: Towards a cognitive narratology. Poetics Today. 1997; 18(4):441–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauss, H. R.Literary history as a challenge to literary theory. New Literary History. 1970; 2(1):7–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jauss, H. R.Levels of identification of hero and audience. New Literary History. 1974; 5(2):283–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson-Laird, P. N. Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1983
Johnson, N. S. and Mandler, J. M.A tale of two structures: Underlying and surface forms in stories. Poetics. 1980; 9:51–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, E. E. and Davis, K. From acts to dispositions: The attribution process in person perception. in: Berkowitz, L., editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1965; pp. 219–66
Jost, F.Narration(s): En decà et au-delà. Communications. 1983; 38:192–212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joyce, J. Dubliners. New York: Penguin Books; 1991
Just, M. A. and Carpenter, P. A. The psychology of reading and language comprehension. Boston: Allyn and Bacon; 1987
Kamau, K. A. Flickering shadows. New York: Henry Holt and Company; 1996
Kaschak, M. P. and Glenberg, A. M.Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000; 43:508–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelley, H. H.Attribution in social psychology: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. 1967; 15:192–238
Kenny, D. A.A general model of consensus and accuracy in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review. 1991; 98:155–63CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keysar, B.The illusory transparency of intention: Linguistic perspective taking text. Cognitive Psychology. 1994; 26:165–208CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kincaid, J. Marbles. in: Mordecai, P. and Wilson, B., editors. Her true-true name: An anthology of women's writing from the Caribbean. Oxford: Heinemann; 1990; pp. 112–16
Kintsch, W. The representation of meaning in memory. Hillsdale: Erlbaum; 1974
Kintsch, W. and Greene, E.The role of culture-specific schemata in the comprehension and recall of stories. Discourse Processes. 1978; 1:1–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kintsch, W.;Kozminsky, E.; Streby, W. J.; McKoon, F.; and Keenan, J. M.Comprehension and recall of text as a function of content variables. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1975; 14:196–214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knapp, J. V.Family systems as psychotherapy, literary character, and literature: An introduction. Style. 1997; 31(2):223–54Google Scholar
Knapp, J. V.Introduction: Self-preservation and self-transformation: Interdisciplinary approaches to literary character. Style. 1990; 24(3):349–64Google Scholar
Krieger, M. Arts on the level: The fall of the elite object. Knoxville: University of Tennessee Press; 1981
Kushigian, J. La economía de las palabras: Disipador del miedo inefable en Borges y Silvina Bullrich. Inti: Revista de Literatura Hispánica. 1989 Spring–1989 Fall; 29–30:207–214
Labov, W. Language in the Inner City: Studies in the Black English Vernacular. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania; 1972; pp. 354–96
Lakoff, G. and Johnson, M. Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York: Basic Books; 1999
Lamarque, P. Narrative and Invention: The Limits of Fictionality. in: Nash, C., editor. Narrative in culture: The uses of storytelling in the sciences, philosophy, and literature. London: University of Warwick Centre for Research in Philosophy and Literature; 1990; pp. 131–53
Lanser, S. The narrative act: Point of view in fiction. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1981
Lanser, S.Sexing the narrative: Propriety, desire, and the engendering of narratology. Narrative. 1995; 3(1):85–94Google Scholar
Lardner, R. The love nest and other stories. New York: C. Scribner's Sons; 1926
Larsen, S. F. and Seilman, U.Personal remindings while reading literature. Text. 1988; 8:411–29CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen, S. F. and Viehoff, R. Introduction: “Empirical” – What does it mean in IGEL studies? in: Rusch, G., editor. Emprical approaches to literature: Procedings of the fourth conference of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature – IGEL; Budapest. Siegen: Lumis Publications; 1995:21–27
Larsen, S. F.; László, J.; and Seilman, U. Across time and place: Cultural-historical knowledge and personal experience in appreciation of literature. in: Ibsch, E.; Schram, D.; and Steen, G., editors. Empirical studies of literature: Proceedings of the second IGEL Conference, Amsterdam, 1989. Amsterdam: Rodopi; 1991
le Carré, J. The naive and sentimental lover. London: Pan Books; 1972
le Carré, J. Smiley's people. London: Penguin Books; 1992
le Carré, J. Our game. Toronto: Penguin Books; 1995
le Carré, J. The tailor of Panama. Toronto: Penguin; 1997
Le Guin, U. K. “The wife's story.” in: The Compass Rose. New York: Bantam Books; 1983
Le Guin, U. K. Always coming home. Toronto: Bantam Books; 1985
Le Guin, U. K. Tehanu: The last book of Earthsea. New York: Atheneum; 1990
Lehnert, W. G. Plot units: A narrative summarization strategy. in: Lehnert, W. G. and Ringle, M. H., editors. Strategies for natural language processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1982; pp. 375–412
Lenneberg, E. The biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley; 1967
Levelt, W. J. M. Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1989
Levelt, W. J. M. Perspective taking and ellipsis in spatial descriptions. in: Bloom, M. A.; Peterson, N. L.; and Garret, M. F. Language and space. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 1996; pp. 77–108
Levine, W. H. and Klin, C. M.Tracking of spatial components of situation models. Memory & Cognition. 2001; 29:327–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Limon, J. The place of fiction in the time of science: A disciplinary history of American writing. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1990
Linde, C. and Labov, W.Spatial netwoks as a site for the study of language and thought. Language. 1975; 51:924–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loftus, G. R.A picture is worth a thousand p values: On the irrelevance of hypothesis testing in the microcomputer age. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers. 1993; 25:250–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, D. L. and Ley, L.Implicit causality and discourse focus: The interaction of text and reader characteristics in pronoun resolution. Journal of Memory and Language. 2000; 42:545–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López, I. H.And there is only my imagination where our history should be: An interview with Cristina García. Michigan Quarterly Review. 1994; 33:3Google Scholar
Mackie, J. L. The cement of the universe: A study of causation. Oxford: Clarendon; 1980
Magliano, J. P.; Dijkstra, K.; and Zwann, R. A.Generating predictive inferences while viewing a movie. Discourse Processes. 1996; 22:199-224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Makaryk, I. R., editor and compiler. Encyclopedia of contemporary literary theory: Approaches, scholars, terms. Toronto: University of Toronto Press; 1993
Mandler, J. M.A code in the node: The use of a story schema in retrieval. Discourse Processes. 1978; 1:14–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. M. Stories, scripts, and scenes: Aspects of schema theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1984
Mandler, J. M. and Goodman, M. S.On the psychological reality of story structure. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1982; 21:507–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.Characterization in narrative: Some theoretical prolegomena. Neophilologus. 1983; 67:1–14CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.The doer and the deed: Action as basis for characterization in narrative. Poetics Today. 1986; 7(2):205–25CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.Structuralist approaches to character in narrative: The state of the art. Semiotica. 1989; 75(1/2):1–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.Individuals in narrative worlds: An ontological perspective. Poetics Today. 1990a; 11(4):843–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.The what, the when, and the how of being a character in literary narrative. Style. 1990b; 24(3):453–68Google Scholar
Margolin, U. Fictional individuals and their counterparts. Andrew, J., editor. Studies in Slavic Literature and Poetics. 1992; 1743–56
Margolin, U.Changing individuals in narrative: Science, philosophy, literature. Semiotica. 1995; 107(12):5–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Margolin, U.Characters in literary narrative: Representation and signification. Semiotica. 1998; 106(3/4):373–92Google Scholar
McCutchen, D. and Perfetti, C. A.The visual tongue-twister effect: Phonological activation in silent reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1982; 2:672–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDaniel, M. A.; Hines, R. J.; Waddil, P. J.; and Einstein, G. O.What makes folk tales unique: Content familiarity, causal structure, scripts, or superstructures?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1994; 20:169–84Google ScholarPubMed
McHale, B.Free indirect discourse: A survey of recent accounts. PTL: A Journal for Descriptive Poetics and Theory of Literature. 1978; 3:249–87Google Scholar
McHale, B.Unspeakable sentences, unnatural acts: Linguistics and poetics revisited. Poetics Today. 1983; 4(1):17–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKoon, G. and Ratcliff, R.Inference during reading. Psychological Review. 1992; 99:440–66CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McRobbie, A. Feminism and youth culture. Cambridge, MA: Hyman; 1990
Medin, D. L. and Schaffer, M. M.Context theory of classification learning. Psychological Review. 1978; 85:207–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyer, B. J. F. The organization of prose and its effect on memory. Amsterdam: North Holland; 1975
Miall, D. S. and Kuiken, D.Aspects of literary response: A new questionnaire. Research in the Teaching of English. 1995; 29:37–58Google Scholar
Moore, R. E. Performance form and the voices of characters in five versions of the Wasco Coyote cycle. in: Lucy, J. A., editor. Reflexive language: Reported speech and metapragmatics. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1993
Morrison, T. Beloved. New York: Plume, Penguin Putnam; 1998
Morrow, D. G.; Bower, G. H.; and Greenspan, S. L.Updating situation models during narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 1989; 28:292–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, D. G.; Greenspan, S. L.; and Bower, G. H., Accessibility and situation models in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 1987(26):165–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mukarovsky, J. Aesthetic function, norm and value as social facts. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press; 1970
Munro, A. The office. in: Munro, A., editor. Collected short stories. New York: Random House; 1996; pp. 59–74
Murillo, L. A. The Cyclical Night: Irony in James Joyce and Jorge Luis Borges. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1968
Myers, J. L. and O'Brien, E. J.Accessing the discourse representation during reading. Discourse Processes. 1998; 26:131–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nash, C. Narrative in culture. London: Routledge; 1994
Nelles, W.Getting focalization into focus. Poetics Today. 1990; 11(2):365–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, A. and Simon, H. A. Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1972
Neyman, J. and Pearson, E. S. On the use and interpretation of certain test criteria for purposes of statistical inference. Biometrika. 1928; 20A:263–94
Nosofsky, R. M.Tests of an exemplar model for relating perceptual classification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance. 1991; 17:3–27Google ScholarPubMed
Oatley, K.A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative. Poetics. 1994; 23:53–74CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Brien, E. J. and Albrecht, J. E.Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1992; 18:777–84Google ScholarPubMed
O'Brien, E. J.; Lorch, R. F. Jr.; and Myers, J. L.Memory-based text processing. Discourse Processes. 1998; 26:2–3Google Scholar
Olson, G. M.; Mack, R. L., and Duffy, S. A.Cognitive aspects of genre. Poetics. 1981; 10:283–315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oltean, S.Functions of free indirect discourse: The case of a novel. Revue Roumaine de Linguistique. 1986; 31:153–64Google Scholar
Oltean, S.A survey of the pragmatic and referential functions of free indirect discourse. Poetics Today. 1993; 14(4):691–714CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Omanson, R. C.An analysis of narratives: Identifying central, supportive, and distracting content. Discourse Processing. 1982; 5:195–224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Neill, P. Points of origin: On focalization in narrative. Canadian Review of Comparative Literature. 1992; Sept:331–50
O'Neill, P. Fictions of discourse: Reading narrative theory. Toronto: University of Toronto Press; 1994
Ortega, R. P.Intento de interpretación psicoanalítica de un cuento de Jorge Luis Borges. Eco. 1971; 23(138–139):587–99Google Scholar
Pavel, T. G. Fictional worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; 1986
Phelan, J. Reading people, reading plots: Character, progression, and the interpretation of narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1989
Pinker, S. The language instinct. New York: Morrow; 1994
Piwowarczyk, M. A.The narratee and the situation of enunciation: A reconsideration of Prince's theory. Genre. 1976; 9:161–77Google Scholar
Polanyi, L. Literary complexity in everyday storytelling. in: Tannen, D., editor. Spoken and written language: Exploring orality and literacy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1982
Pollard-Gott, L.Attribution theory and the novel. Poetics. 1993; 21:499–524CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Porter, K. A. Rope. in: Cahill, S., editor. Women and fiction: Short stories by and about women. New York: New American Library; 1975; pp. 78–84
Postman, L. and Senders, V. L.Incidental learning and generality of set. Journal of Experimental Psychology. 1946; 36:153–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Potter, J. and Wetherell, M. Discourse and social psychology. London: Sage; 1987
Prince, G. A grammar of stories. The Hague: Mouton; 1973
Prince, G. Dictionary of narratology. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1987
Prince, G. Narratology: The form and functioning of narrative. Amsterdam: Mouton; 1982
Prince, G. On narratology: Past, present, future. French Literary Studies. 1990; XVII
Propp, V. Morphologie du conte. Derrida, M.; Todorov, T.; and Kahn, C., translators. Paris: Editions du Seuil; 1965
Propp, V. Morphology of the folktale. Austin: University of Texas; 1968
Raap, D. N.; Gerrig, R. J.; and Prentice, D. A.Readers' trait-based models of characters in narrative comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. 2001;45:737–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rabinowitz, P. J. Other reader-oriented theories. in: Selden, R., editor. The Cambridge history of literary criticism: From formalism to poststructuralism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 1995; 375–403
Ramazani, V. The free indirect mode: Flaubert and the poetics of irony. Charlottesville: Univeristy Press of Virginia; 1988
Reeder, G. D. and Brewer, M. B.A schematic model of dispositional attribution in interpersonal perception. Psychological Review. 1979; 86:61–79CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richard, J.-P. Littérature et sensation. Paris: Aux Editions du Seuil; 1954
Richards, E. and Singer, M.Representation of complex goal structures in narrative comprehension. Discourse Processes. 2001; 31:111–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richardson, B.Beyond poststructuralism: Theory of character, the personae of modern drama, and the antinomies of critical theory. Modern Drama. 1997; 40:86–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ricoeur, P. Temps et récit. Paris: Seuil; 1983
Riffaterre, M.Describing poetic structures: Two approaches to Baudelaire's “Les Chats.”Yale French Studies. 1966; 36/37:200–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimmon-Kenan, S. Narrative fiction: Contemporary poetics. New York: Methuen; 1983
Rosch, E. and Mervis, C. B.Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology. 1975; 7:573–605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, L. The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. in: Berkowitz, L., editor. Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1977; pp. 173–220
Ross, L. D.; Amabile, T. M.; and Steinmetz, J. L.Social roles, social cognition, and biases in social-perception processes. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology. 1977; 35:485–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowling, J. K. Harry Potter and the goblet of fire. London: Raincoast Books; 2000
Rumelhart, D. E. Notes on a schema for stories. in: Bobrow, D. G. and Collins, A., editors. Representation and understanding: Studies in cognitive science. New York: Academic Press; 1975; pp. 211–36
Rusch, G. The notion of “empirical”: Knowing how. in: Rusch, G., editor. Empirical approaches to literature: Proceedings of the fourth biannual conference of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature – IGEL; Budapest. Siegen: Lumis Publications; 1995: pp. 103–8
Sanders, J. and Redeker, G. Perspective and the representation of speech and thought. in: Fauconnier, G., editor. Worlds and grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1996; pp. 290–317
Sanford, A. J. and Garrod, S. Understanding written language: Explorations in comprehension beyond the sentence. Chichester: Wiley; 1981
Sanford, A. J. and Garrod, S. C.The role of scenario mapping in text comprehension. Discourse Processes. 1998; 26:159–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schank, R. C. and Abelson, R. P. Scripts, plans, goals and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977
Schegloff, E. A.; Jefferson, G.; and Sacks, H.The preference for self-correction in the organization of repair in conversation. Language. 1977; 53:361–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. J.Receptional problems with contemporary narrative texts and some of their reasons. Poetics. 1980; 9:119–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. J.Empirical studies in literature: Introductory remarks. Poetics. 1981; 10:317–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. J.The empirical science of literature ESL: A new paradigm. Poetics. 1983; 12:19–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, S. J. What can “empirical” mean in a constructivist context? 10 considerations. in: Rusch, G., editor. Empirical approaches to literature: Proceedings of the fourth biannual conference of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature – IGEL; Budapest. Siegen: Lumis Publications; 1995: 109–112
Schober, M. F.Spatial perspective-taking in conversation. Cognition. 1993; 47:1–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, M. S. and McClelland, J. L.A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review. 1989; 96:523–68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Selden, R. The Cambridge history of literary criticism: From formalism to poststructuralism. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 1995
Shapiro, M.How narrators report speech. Language and Style. 1984; 17(1):67–78Google Scholar
Shaw, H. E.Thin description: A reply to Seymour Chatman. Narrative. 1995; 3(3):307–14Google Scholar
Shepard, L. Life during wartime. New York: Bantam Books; 1987
Shklovsky, V. Art as technique. in: Lemon, L. T. and Reis, M. J., editors. Russian formalist criticism: Four essays. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press; 1965; pp. 3–24
Sidner, C.Focusing and discourse. Discourse Processes. 1983; 6:107–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair-de Zwart, H. Language acquisition and cognitive development. in: Moore, T., editor. Cognitive development and the acquisition of language. New York: Academic Press; 1973
Singer, M. and Ferreira, F.Inferring consequences in story comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1983; 22:437–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slatoff, W. With respect to readers: Dimensions of literary response. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press; 1970
Sophocles. Oedipus the king and Antigone. Arnott, P. D., translator. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1969
Spilich, G. J.; Vesonder, G. T.; Chiesi, H. L.; and Voss, J. F.Text processing of domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1979; 18:275–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spolsky, E. Gaps in nature: Literary interpretation and the modular mind. Albany: State University of New York Press; 1993
Stampfl, B.Filtering Rimmon-Kenan, Chatman, Black, Freud, and James: Focalization and the divided self in “The beast in the jungle.”Style. 1992; 26(3):388–99Google Scholar
Stanzel, F. K. Teller-characters and reflector-characters in narrative theory. Poetics Today. 1981; 2(2):4–15CrossRef
Stein, N. L. and Glenn, C. G. An analysis of story comprehension in elementary school children. in: Le Freed, R., editor. New directions in discourse processing (Vol. 2). Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1979; pp. 53–120
Suleiman, S. and Crosman, I. The reader in the text: Essays on audience and interpretation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1980
Symonds, J. A., On the application of evolutionary principles to art and literature. in: Essays speculative and suggestive. New York: AMS Press; 1970; pp. 197–224
Tan, E. S. H.Film-induced affect as a witness emotion. Poetics. 1994; 23:7–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D.Oral and literate strategies in spoken and written narratives. Language. 1982; 58(1):1–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannen, D. Conversational style. Analyzing talk among friends. Norwood: Ablex; 1984
Taylor, H. A. and Tversky, B.Perspective in spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language. 1996; 35:371–91CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thorndyke, P. W.Cognitive structure in comprehension and memory of narrative discourse. Cognitive Psychology. 1977; 9:51–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thury, E. M. and Friedlander, A. The impact of expertise: The role of experience in reading literary texts. in: Rusch, G., editor. Empirical approaches to literature: Proceedings of the fourth biannual conference of the International Society for the Empirical Study of Literature – IGEL; Budapest. Siegen: Lumis Publications; 1995; p. 22
Tolliver, J. Discourse analysis and the interpretation of literary narrative. Style. 1990; 24(2):266–83
Tomachevsky, B. Thématique. in: Todorov, T., editor and translator. Théorie de la littérature. Paris: Aux Editions du Seuil; 1965
Toolan, M. J. Narrative: A critical linguistic introduction. New York: Routledge; 1995
Trabasso, R. and Sperry, L. L.Causal relatedness and importance of story events. Journal of Memory and Language. 1985; 24:595–611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trabasso, T. and Broek, P.Causal thinking and the representation of narrative events. Journal of Memory and Language. 1985; 24:612–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trope, Y. and Burnstein, E.Processing the information contained in another's behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 1975; 11:439–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turner, M. Reading minds: The study of English in the age of cognitive science. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press; 1991
Tversky, B. Spatial perspective in descriptions. in: Bloom, P.; Barret, M.; Hadel, L.; and Peterson, M., editors. Language and Space. Boston: MIT Press; 1996; pp. 463–91
Uleman, J. S.; Hon, A.;Roman, R. J.; and Moskowitz, G. B.On-line evidence for spontaneous trait inferences at encoding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 1996; 22:377–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van den Broek, P.; Risden, K.; Fletcher, C. R.; and Thurlow, R. A. “Landscape” view of reading: Fluctuating patterns of activation and the construction of a stable memory representation. in: Britton, B. K. and Graesser, A. C., editors. Models of understanding text. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1996; pp. 165–87
Dijk, T. A.Philosophy of action and theory of narrative. Poetics. 1976; 5Google Scholar
van Dijk, T. A. and Kintsch, W. Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York: Academic Press; 1983
Vipond, D. and Hunt, R. A. Literary processing and response as transactions: Evidence for the contribution of readers, texts, and situation. in: Meutsch, D. and Viehoff, R., editors. Comprehension of literary discourse: Interdisciplinary approaches. Berlin: de Gruyter; 1989; pp. 155–74
Vitoux, P.Le jeu de la focalisation. Poétique. 1982; 51:359–68Google Scholar
Vitoux, P.Notes sur la focalisation dans le roman autobiographique. Etudes Littéraires. 1984; 17(2):261–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, R.Who is the narrator?Poetics Today. 1997a; 18(4):495–513CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walsh, R.Why we wept for Little Nell: Character and emotional involvment. Narrative. 1997b; 5(3):306–21Google Scholar
Warren, W. H.; Nicholas, D. W.; and Trabasso, T. Event chains and inferences in understanding narratives. in: Freedle, R. O., editor. Advances in discourse processes: New directions in discourse processing, Vol. 2. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1979
Weinberg, H. H.Irony and “style indirect libre” in Madame Bovary. Canadian Review of Comparative Literature. 1981; 8(1):1–9Google Scholar
Weitz, M.The role of theory in aesthetics. Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism. 1956; 15(1):27–35CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weitz, M. The opening mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1977
Wellek, R. The concept of evolution in literary history. in: Wellek, R., editor. Concepts of criticism. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press; 1973
Werth, P. N. How to build a world (in a lot less than six days, and using only what's in your head). in: Green, K., editor. New essays in deixis: Discourse, narrative, literature. Amsterdam: Rodopi; 1995; pp. 49–80
White, H. The value of narrativity in the representation of reality. in: Mitchell, W. J. T., editor. On narrative. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1981; pp. 1–24
Whitehurst, G. Language development. in: Wolman, B., editor. Handbook of developmental psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1982
Wiebe, J. M. Recognizing subjective sentences: A computational analysis of narrative text. Buffalo, NY: SUNY, Department of Computer Science; 1990
Wieder, L. Telling the code. in: Turner, R., editor. Ethnomethodology. Harmondsworth, UK: Penguin; 1974
Wike, E. L. and Church, J. D.Comments on Clark's “The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy.” Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior. 1976; 15:249–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilder, D.Perceiving persons as a group: Effects on attributions of causality and beliefs. Social Psychology. 1978; 1:13–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, S. G.; Rinck, M.; McNamara, T. P.; Bower, G. H.; and Morrow, D. G.Mental models and narrative comprehension: Some qualifications. Journal of Memory and Language. 1993; 32:141–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winograd, T. A framework for understanding discourse. in: Just, M. A. and Carp, P. A. Cognitive processes in comprehension. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1977; pp. 63–88
Wolff, E.Der intendierte Leser: Uberlegungen und Beispiele zur Einfuhrung eines literaturwissenschaftlichen Begriffes. Poetica. 1971; 4(2):141–66Google Scholar
Zwann, R.Effect of genre expectations on text comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. 1994; 20:920–33Google Scholar
Zwann, R. A.; Langston, M. C.; and Graesser, A. C.The construction of situation models in narrative comprehension: An event-indexing model. Psychological Science. 1995; 6:292–7CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Marisa Bortolussi, University of Alberta, Peter Dixon, University of Alberta
  • Book: Psychonarratology
  • Online publication: 28 July 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511500107.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Marisa Bortolussi, University of Alberta, Peter Dixon, University of Alberta
  • Book: Psychonarratology
  • Online publication: 28 July 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511500107.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Marisa Bortolussi, University of Alberta, Peter Dixon, University of Alberta
  • Book: Psychonarratology
  • Online publication: 28 July 2009
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511500107.010
Available formats
×