Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-4hvwz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-25T19:57:35.598Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

11 - Yes/No questions and rate of auxiliary learning for thirty-two children

from PART III - ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN AUXILIARY VERB LEARNING

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 September 2009

Brian J. Richards
Affiliation:
University of Reading
Get access

Summary

The Barnes, Gutfreund, Satterly and Wells study

Of the research outlined in Chapter 10, BGSW's use of the Bristol child language corpus (Wells, 1985) gives them the strongest claim to a representative sample of the population, and naturalistic and spontaneous speech undistorted by the presence of an observer (see Section 2.1). BGSW selected sixteen boys and sixteen girls with a broad range of family background and coded the speech addressed to them by adults in the recording when the child' s MLUS was closest to 1.5 morphemes (Tl). Because intervals between speech sampling were three months in the Bristol Study, exact matching for MLU was not possible and actual values for MLUS ranged from 1.0 to 2.21 (mean = 1.68). The mean age at this point was 2;0 (range l;6–2;9). To assess the children's progress, transcripts from a point nine months later (T2) were used. Children's gains were indicated by residual gain scores, the difference between predicted scores at T2 and actual scores at T2 (see Barnes et al., 1983; O'Connor, 1972). For the auxiliary there were three measures: range of auxiliary meanings, auxiliary frequency, and range of auxiliary forms. Meanings and forms are as defined in Wells (1979a), forms being major forms rather than subforms (see Section 2.2.2). Only the correlations with range of auxiliary meanings are reported.

Strengths and limitations of BGSW

  1. Size of population sample: BGSW's sample of thirty-two children is the largest of the published studies and only HG1 and HG2 approach this figure (N = 22). NGG's original sample of fifteen is reduced to two groups of six in the reanalysis (GNG). For FNB, SW, and YK, sample sizes are seven, nine and ten respectively.

  2. […]

Type
Chapter
Information
Language Development and Individual Differences
A Study of Auxiliary Verb Learning
, pp. 173 - 212
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×