Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6d856f89d9-72csx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T04:11:12.020Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Does biology play any role in sex differences in the mind?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Simon Baron-Cohen
Affiliation:
Professor of Developmental Psychopathology, University of Cambridge
Jude Browne
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Get access

Summary

There are interesting differences between the average male and female mind. Recognizing these could lead to mutual respect of difference. In using the word ‘average’ I am from the outset recognizing that such differences may have little to say about individuals. In addition, the differences are subtle, and are to do with the relative proportions of different drives in the typical male and female mind. The field of sex differences in psychology in the 1960s and '70s was so conflict ridden as to make an open-minded debate about any possible role of biology contributing to psychological sex differences impossible. Those who explored the role of biology – even whilst acknowledging the importance of culture – found themselves accused of defending an essentialism that perpetuated inequalities between the sexes, and of oppression. Not a climate in which scientists can ask questions about mechanisms in nature. Today, the pendulum has settled sensibly in the middle of the nature–nurture debate, and scientists who care deeply about ending inequality and oppression can at the same time also talk freely about biological differences between the male and female brain and mind.

My own view is that the field of sex differences in mind needs to proceed in a fashion that is sensitive to this history of conflict by cautiously looking at the evidence and being careful not to overstate what can be concluded. Once again, the evidence says nothing about individuals.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ahlgren, A. and Johnson, D. W. (1979) ‘Sex Differences in Cooperative and Competitive Attitudes from the 2nd through the 12th Grades’, Developmental Psychology 15: 45–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atran, S. (1994) ‘Core Domains versus Scientific Theories: Evidence from Systematics and Itzaj-Maya Folkbiology’, in Hirschfeld, L. A. and Gelman, S. A. (eds.), Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 316–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, A., Bolton, P. and Rutter, M. (1998) ‘A Full Genome Screen for Autism with Evidence for Linkage to a Region on Chromosome 7q’, Human Molecular Genetics 7: 571–8.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1988) ‘Social and Pragmatic Deficits in Autism: Cognitive or Affective?’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 18: 379–402.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995) Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind (Boston, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2000) ‘The Cognitive Neuroscience of Autism: Evolutionary Approaches’, in Gazzaniga, M. S. (ed.-in-chief), Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (eds.), The New Cognitive Neurosciences, 2nd edition (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 1249–57.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (2003) The Essential Difference: Men, Women, and the Extreme Male Brain (Boston: Basic Books).Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Baldwin, D. A. and Crowson, M. (1997) ‘Do Children with Autism Use the Speaker's Direction of Gaze Strategy to Crack the Code of Language?’, Child Development 68: 48–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. and Bolton, P. (1993) Autism: The Facts (Oxford: Oxford University Press).Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Bolton, P., Wheelwright, S., et al. (1998) ‘Does Autism Occur More Often in Families of Physicists, Engineers, and Mathematicians?’, Autism 2: 296–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. and Hammer, J. (1997) ‘Is Autism an Extreme Form of the “Male Brain”?’, in Rovee-Collier, C. and Lipsitt, L. P. (eds.), Advances in Infancy Research vol. 11 (Greenwich, CT: Ablex Publishing), 193–217.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C. and Robertson, M. (1997a) ‘Another Advanced Test of Theory of Mind: Evidence from Very High Functioning Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 38: 813–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., O'Riordan, M., Stone, V., Jones, R. and Plaistead, K. (1999a) ‘Recognition of Faux Pas by Normally Developing Children and Children with Asperger Syndrome or High-Functioning Autism’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 29: 407–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Richler, J., Bisarya, D., Gurunathan, N. and Wheelwright, S. (2003) ‘The Systemising Quotient (SQ): An Investigation of Adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism and Normal Sex Differences’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B 358: 361–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Bullmore, E. T., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C. and Williams, S. C. (2000) ‘The Amygdala Theory of Autism’, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 24: 355–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H. A., Wheelwright, S., et al. (1999) ‘Social Intelligence in the Normal and Autistic Brain: An fMRI study’, European Journal of Neuroscience 11: 1891–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. and Wheelwright, S. (1999) ‘ “Obsessions” in Children with Autism or Asperger Syndrome: A Content Analysis in Terms of Core Domains of Cognition’, British Journal of Psychiatry 175: 484–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. and Wheelwright, S. (2003) ‘The Friendship and Relationship Questionnaire’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 33: 509–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. and Jolliffe, T. (1997) ‘Is There a “Language of the Eyes”? Evidence from Normal Adults and Adults with Autism or Asperger Syndrome’, Visual Cognition 4: 311–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Lawson, J., Griffin, R. and Hill, J. (2002) ‘The Exact Mind: Empathising and Systemising in Autism Spectrum Conditions’, in Goswami, U. (ed.), Handbook of Cognitive Development (Oxford: Blackwell Publishers), 491–508.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Scahill, V., Spong, A. and Lawson, J. (2001a) ‘Are Intuitive Physics and Intuitive Psychology Independent? A Test with Children with Asperger Syndrome’, Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorder 5: 47–78.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Skinner, R., Martin, J. and Clubley, E. (2001b) ‘The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): Evidence from Asperger Syndrome/High-Functioning Autism, Males and Females, Scientists and Mathematicians’, [published correction appears in Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31: 603] Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31: 5–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stone, V. and Rutherford, M. (1999b) ‘A Mathematician, a Physicist, and a Computer Scientist with Asperger Syndrome: Performance on Folk Psychology and Folk Physics Test’, Neurocase 5: 475–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Stott, C., Bolton, P. and Goodyer, I. (1997b) ‘Is There a Link between Engineering and Autism?’, Autism 1: 101–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benbow, C. P. (1988) ‘Sex Differences in Mathematical Reasoning Ability in Intellectually Talented Preadolescents: Their Nature, Effects, and Possible Causes’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 11: 169–232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blair, R. J. (1995) ‘A Cognitive Developmental Approach to Morality: Investigating the Psychopath’, Cognition 57: 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charlesworth, W. R. and Dzur, C. (1987) ‘Gender Comparisons of Preschoolers’ Behavior and Resource Utilization in Group Problem Solving', Child Development 58: 191–200.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collins, D. W. and Kimura, D. (1997) ‘A Large Sex Difference on a Two-Dimensional Mental Rotation Task’, Behavioral Neuroscience 111: 845–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Batki, A. and Ahluwalia, J. (2000) ‘Sex Differences in Human Neonatal Social Perception’, Infant Behavior and Development 23: 113–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crick, N. R. and Grotpeter, J. K. (1995) ‘Relational Aggression, Gender, and Social-Psychological Adjustment’, Child Development 66: 710–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daly, M. and Wilson, M. (1988) Homicide (New York: Aldine de Gruyter).Google ScholarPubMed
Davis, M. H. (1994) Empathy: A Social Psychological Approach, ed. Harvey, J.. Brown and Benchmark Social Psychology Series (Boulder, CO: Westview Press).Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A. (1980) ‘Social Cognition and Children's Aggressive Behavior’, Child Development 51: 162–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eagly, A. H. (1987) Sex Differences in Social Behavior: A Social-Role Interpretation (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates).Google Scholar
Elliot, R. (1961) ‘Interrelationship among Measures of Field Dependence, Ability, and Personality Traits’, Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 63: 27–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, U. (ed.) (1991) Autism and Asperger Syndrome (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galea, L. A. M. and Kimura, D. (1993) ‘Sex Differences in Route-Learning’, Personality and Individual Differences 14: 53–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1996) ‘Sexual Selection and Sex Differences in Mathematical Abilities’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 19: 229–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1998) Male, Female: The Evolution of Human Sex Differences (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouchie, C. and Kimura, D. (1991) ‘The Relationship between Testosterone Levels and Cognitive Ability Patterns’, Psychoneuroendocrinology 16: 323–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J. A. (1978) ‘Gender Effects in Decoding Nonverbal Cues’, Psychological Bulletin 85: 845–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Happe, F. G. (1995) ‘The Role of Age and Verbal Ability in the Theory of Mind Task Performance of Subjects with Autism’, Child Development 66: 843–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, M. L. (1977) ‘Sex Differences in Empathy and Related Behaviors’, Psychological Bulletin 84: 712–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jennings, K. D. (1977) ‘People versus Object Orientation in Preschool Children: Do Sex Differences Really Occur?’, Journal of Genetic Psychology 131: 65–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jolliffe, T. and Baron-Cohen, S. (1997) ‘Are People with Autism or Asperger Syndrome Faster than Normal on the Embedded Figures Test?’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 38: 527–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, D. (1999) Sex and Cognition (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
Knight, G. P. and Chao, C.- C. (1989) ‘Gender Differences in the Cooperative, Competitive, and Individualistic Social Values of Children’, Motivation and Emotion 13: 125–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutchmaya, S. and Baron-Cohen, S. (2002) ‘Human Sex Differences in Social and Nonsocial Looking Preferences at 12 Months of Age’, Infant Behavior and Development 25: 319–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S. and Raggatt, P. (2002a) ‘Foetal Testosterone and Vocabulary Size in 18- and 24-Month-Old Infants’, Infant Behavior and Development 24: 418–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S. and Raggatt, P. (2002b) ‘Foetal Testosterone and Eye Contact in 12 Month-Old Infants’, Infant Behavior and Development 25: 327–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1998) The Two Sexes: Growing Up Apart, Coming Together (Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press).Google Scholar
Manning, J. T., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S. and Sanders, G. (2001) ‘The 2nd to 4th Digit Ratio and Autism’, Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 43: 160–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morton, J., Leslie, A. and Frith, U. (1995) ‘The Cognitive Basis of a Biological Disorder: Autism’, New Scientist 14: 434–8.Google Scholar
O'Riordan, M., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J. and Baron-Cohen, S. (2001) ‘Superior Visual Search in Autism’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 27: 719–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Power, T. G. (1985) ‘Mother– and Father–Infant Play: a Developmental Analysis’, Child Development 56: 1514–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiff, W. and Oldak, R. (1990) ‘Accuracy of Judging Time to Arrival: Effects of Modality, Trajectory, and Gender’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 16: 303–16.Google ScholarPubMed
Smith, P. M. (1985) Language, the Sexes, and Society (Oxford: Blackwell Publishers).Google Scholar
Strayer, F. F. (1980) ‘Child Ethology and the Study of Preschool Social Relations’, in Foot, H. C., Chapman, A. J. and Smith, J. R. (eds.), Friendship and Social Relations in Children (Chichester: John Wiley & Sons), 235–65.Google Scholar
Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., et al. (1998) ‘The Frequency and Distribution of Spontaneous Attention Shifts between Social and Nonsocial Stimuli in Autistic, Typically Developing, and Nonautistic Developmentally Delayed Infants’, Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 39: 747–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tannen, D. (1990) You Just Don't Understand: Women and Men in Conversation (New York: William Morrow & Company).Google Scholar
Task Force on DSM-IV (1994) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn (Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association).
Tordjman, S., Ferrari, P., Sulmont, V., Duyme, M. and Roubertoux, P. (1997) ‘Androgenic Activity in Autism’, American Journal of Psychiatry 154: 1626–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voyer, D., Voyer, S. and Bryden, M. (1995) ‘Magnitude of Sex Differences in Spatial Abilities: A Meta-analysis and Consideration of Critical Variables’, Psychological Bulletin 117: 250–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witkin, H. A., Lewis, H. B., Hertzman, M., Machover, K., Meissner, Bretnall P. and Wapner, S. (1954) Personality Through Perception (New York: Harper & Brothers).Google Scholar
Wittig, M. A., Allen, M. J. (1984) ‘Measurement of Adult Performance on Piaget's Water Horizontality Task’, Intelligence 8: 305–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×