Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-84b7d79bbc-4hvwz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-31T04:18:54.461Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - In search of autonomous-relational self-construal

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Sevda Bekman
Affiliation:
Bogaziçi University, Istanbul
Ayhan Aksu-Koç
Affiliation:
Bogaziçi University, Istanbul
Get access

Summary

I first met Çiğdem Kağıtçıbaşı in the late 1980s, when I became interested in cross-cultural approaches to social and organizational psychology and started to attend the congresses of the International Association for Cross-Cultural Psychology. I developed an increasing respect for the contributions that she was making, both to psychology in Turkey and to discussions of how best cross-cultural psychology might progress. A series of her former students came to work with me in the UK, and on visits to Turkey, I marveled at the way that she seemed to be a moving force behind so many positive initiatives.

More recently, I came to realize that the outcome of her research into families could provide a vital missing link within existing accounts of cross-cultural social psychology such as those provided by myself and Michael Bond (Smith and Bond 1993). Consequently, I was delighted when she was able to join us in generating a completely new version of our earlier book (Smith et al. 2006). I became convinced that her distinctive conceptualization of self had captured an element less well seen or articulated by others. This chapter documents my attempts to build bridges between her insight and the concepts and measures that have been devised by other contributors to the field.

Over the past few decades, psychologists have formulated increasingly sophisticated models of the ways in which persons regulate their behavior.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Banaji, M. R. and Prentice, D. J., “The self in social contexts,” Annual Review of Psychology, 45 (1994), 295–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A., “Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioural change,” Psychological Review, 84 (1977), 194–215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bandura, A., “Social cognitive theory in cultural context,” Applied Psychology: An International Review, 51 (2002), 269–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bresnahan, M. J., Levine, T. R., Shearman, S. M., Lee, S. Y., Park, C. Y., and Kiyomiya, T., “A multimethod multitrait validity assessment of self-construal in Japan, Korea and the United States,” Human Communication Research, 31 (2005), 33–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B. and Caporael, L. R., “Social identity motives in evolutionary perspective,” in Brown, R. J. and Capozza, D. (eds.), Social Identities: Motivational, Emotional and Cultural Influences (Hove, UK: Psychology Press, 2006), pp. 135–52.Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B. and Chen, Y. R., “Where (who) are collectives in collectivism: Toward conceptual clarification of individualism and collectivism,” Psychological Review, 114 (2007), 133–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer, M. B. and Gardner, W. L., “Who is this ‘we’? Levels of collective identity and self representations,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71 (1996), 83–93.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cross, S. E., Bacon, P. L., and Morris, M. L., “The relational-interdependent self-construal and relationships,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78 (2000), 791–808.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Earley, P. C., “Self or group? Cultural effects of training on self-efficacy and performance,” Administrative Science Quarterly, 39 (1994), 89–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgas, J., Berry, J. W., Vijver, F., Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., and Poortinga, Y. H., Families across Cultures: A 30-nation Psychological Study (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2006).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gudykunst, W. B., Matsumoto, Y., Ting-Toomey, S., Nishida, T., Kim, K., and Heyman, S., “The influence of cultural individualism-collectivism, self-construals and individual values on communication styles across cultures,” Human Communication Research, 22 (1996), 510–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harb, C. and Smith, P. B., “Self-construals across cultures: Beyond independence-interdependence,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 39 (2008), 178–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardin, E., “Convergent evidence for the multidimensionality of self-construal,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37 (2006), 516–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hardin, E., Leong, F. T., and Bhagwat, A. A., “Factor structure of the self-construal revisited,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35 (2004), 327–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstede, G., Culture's Consequences: International Differences in Work-related Values (Beverly Hills, CA: Sage, 1980).Google Scholar
Hofstede, G., Culture's Consequences: Comparing Values, Behaviors, Institutions and Organizations across Nations (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 2001).Google Scholar
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., “The autonomous-relational self: A new synthesis,” European Psychologist, 1 (1996), 180–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., “Autonomy and relatedness in cultural context: Implications for self and family,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 36 (2005), 403–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., Family and Human Development across Cultures: A View from the Other Side, Second edition (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2006).Google Scholar
Kashima, E. S. and Hardie, E. A., “The development and validation of the relational, individual and collective self-aspects,” Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 3 (2000), 19–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashima, Y. and Kashima, E., “Culture and language: The case of cultural dimensions and personal pronoun use,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29 (1998), 461–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kashima, Y., Yamaguchi, S., Kim, U., Choi, S. C., Gelfand, M. J., and Yuki, M., “Culture, gender and the self: A perspective from individualism-collectivism research,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69 (1995), 925–37.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurman, J., “Why is self-enhancement low in certain collectivist cultures? An investigation of two competing explanations,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34 (2003), 496–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, T. R., Bresnahan, M. J., Park, H. S., Lapinski, M. K., Lee, T. S., and Lee, D., “The (in)validity of self-construal scales revisited,” Human Communication Research, 29 (2003a), 291–308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levine, T. R., Bresnahan, M. J., Park, H. S., Lapinski, M. K., Wittenbaum, G., Shearman, S. M. et al., “Self-report measures of self-construals lack validity,” Human Communication Research, 29 (2003b), 210–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R. and Kitayama, S., “Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation,” Psychological Review, 98 (1991), 224–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markus, H. R. and Nurius, P., “Possible selves,” American Psychologist, 41 (1986), 954–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., and Kemmelmeier, M., “Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses,” Psychological Bulletin, 128 (2002), 3–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, S. H., “Beyond individualism and collectivism: New cultural dimensions of values,” in Kim, U., Triandis, H. C., Kağıtçıbaşı, Ç., Choi, S. C., and Yoon, G. (eds.), Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method and Applications (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage, 1994), pp. 85–119.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H., “Mapping and interpreting cultural differences around the world,” in Vinken, H., Soeters, J., and Ester, P. (eds.), Comparing Cultures: Dimensions of Culture in a Comparative Perspective (Leiden, The Netherlands: Brill, 2004), pp. 43–73.Google Scholar
Sedikides, C. and Brewer, M. B. (eds.), Individual Self, Relational Self, Collective Self (Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press, 2001).
Singelis, T. M., “The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construal,” Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20 (1994), 580–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singelis, T. M., Triandis, H. C., Bhawuk, D., and Gelfand, M., “Horizontal and vertical dimensions of individualism and collectivism: A theoretical and measurement refinement,” Cross-Cultural Research, 29 (1995), 240–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. B., “Acquiescent response bias as an aspect of cultural communication style,” Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 35 (2004), 50–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, P. B. and Bond, M. H., Social Psychology across Cultures (Hemel Hempstead: Prentice Hall, 1993).Google Scholar
Smith, P. B. and Fischer, R., “Acquiescence, extreme response bias and culture: A multi-level analysis,” in Vijver, F. J. R., Hemert, D. A., and Poortinga, Y. (eds.), Multilevel Analysis of Individuals and Cultures (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 2008), pp. 285–314.Google Scholar
Smith, P. B., Bond, M. H., and Ç. Kağıtçıbaşı, Understanding Social Psychology across Cultures: Living and Working in a Changing World (London: Sage, 2006).Google Scholar
Turner, J. C., “A self-categorisation theory,” in Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S., and Wetherell, M. S. (eds.), Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-categorization Theory (Oxford: Blackwell, 1987), pp. 42–67.Google Scholar
Yperen, N. W. and Buunk, B. P., “Equity theory and exchange and communal orientation from a cross-cultural perspective,” Journal of Social Psychology, 131 (1991), 5–21.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×