Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m8s7h Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-17T21:53:06.084Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

15 - Latent inhibition and other salience modulation effects: same neural substrates?

from Current topics in latent inhibition research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Robert Lubow
Affiliation:
Tel-Aviv University
Ina Weiner
Affiliation:
Tel-Aviv University
Get access

Summary

The complexity of the environment is such that even a snapshot in time from a single modality can exceed the processing capacity of the human brain (Tsotsos,1990). This means that selection of some incoming stimuli for more detailed analysis (while ignoring other stimuli) is essential for efficient cognitive processing. Without such filtering we would be inundated with competing sensory impressions. Thus notions of stimulus salience and relevance have assumed prominence in a number of domains, from visual attention (Fecteau & Munoz, 2006; Li, 2002; Morris, Friston & Dolan, 1997) to animal cognition (Mackintosh, 1975).

But what exactly is meant by the term stimulus salience? There is no straightforward answer to this question, as the apprehension of salience is likely to be determined by a variety of factors. First, the inherent features of the stimulus will contribute to its effective salience, for example its physical intensity (what we will term intrinsic salience). More physically intense stimuli will be learned about more easily. A stimulus's physical intensity cannot be modulated, but the perception of its intensity can be. Second, associative learning theories tell us that past experience with the stimulus can also influence the amount of attention that is paid to it (what we will term acquired salience). This affects what is known as its associability – how easily it may be learned about – prototypically demonstrated in latent inhibition (LI). Thus past experience can also modulate the effective salience of a stimulus.

Type
Chapter
Information
Latent Inhibition
Cognition, Neuroscience and Applications to Schizophrenia
, pp. 342 - 371
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, M. T., Padilla, Y., Myers, C. E., & Gluck, M. A. (2002). Selective hippocampal lesions disrupt a novel cue effect but fail to eliminate blocking in rabbit eyelid conditioning. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2, 318–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asin, K. E., Wirtshafter, D., & Kent, E. W. (1980). The effects of electrolytic median raphe lesions on two measures of latent inhibition. Behavioral and Neural Biology, 28, 409–418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakshi, V. P., & Kelley, A. E. (1991a). Dopaminergic regulation of feeding behavior: 1. Differential effects of haloperidol microinfusion into 3 striatal subregions. Psychobiology, 19, 223–232.Google Scholar
Bakshi, V. P., & Kelley, A. E. (1991b). Dopaminergic regulation of feeding behavior: 2. Differential effects of amphetamine microinfusion into 3 striatal subregions. Psychobiology, 19, 233–242.Google Scholar
Barklamb, K., & Cassaday, H. J. (2007). The salience of internal and external cues in cases of self-reported hunger and illness. Appetite, 49, 511–515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baruch, I., Hemsley, D. R., & Gray, J. A. (1988). Differential performance of acute and chronic schizophrenics in a latent inhibition task. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 176, 598–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bender, S., Muller, B., Oades, R. D., & Sartory, G. (2001). Conditioned blocking and schizophrenia: a replication and study of the role of symptoms, age, onset-age of psychosis and illness-duration. Schizophrenia Research, 49, 57–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bethus, I., Muscat, R., & Goodall, G. (2006). Dopamine manipulations limited to preexposure are sufficient to modulate latent inhibition. Behavioral Neuroscience, 120, 554–562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bleuler, E. (1911; translated 1950). Dementia Praecox or the Group of Schizophrenias. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Canales, J. J., & Iversen, S. D. (2000). Dynamic dopamine receptor interactions in the core and shell of nucleus accumbens differentially coordinate the expression of unconditioned motor behaviors. Synapse, 36, 297–306.3.0.CO;2-M>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carr, A. F. (1974). Latent inhibition and overshadowing in conditioned emotional response conditioning in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 86, 718–723.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassaday, H. J. (2009). Blocking, overshadowing and related concepts. In Stolerman, I. (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychopharmacology. New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Cassaday, H. J., Horsley, R. R., & Norman, C. (2005). Electrolytic lesions to nucleus accumbens core and shell have dissociable effects on conditioning to discrete and contextual cues in aversive and appetitive procedures respectively. Behavioural Brain Research, 160, 222–235.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Coutureau, E., Galani, R., Gosselin, O., Majchrzak, M., & Di Scala, G. (1999). Entorhinal but not hippocampal or subicular lesions disrupt latent inhibition in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 72, 143–157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crider, A., Solomon, P. R., & McMahon, M. A. (1982). Disruption of selective attention in the rat following chronic D-amphetamine administration: relationship to schizophrenic attention disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 17, 351–361.Google ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. A., Atwater, G. E., & Kilts, C. D. (1993). Effects of antipsychotic drugs on latent inhibition: sensitivity and specificity of an animal behavioral model of clinical drug action. Psychopharmacology, 112, 315–323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Escobar, M., Arcediano, F., & Miller, R. R. (2002a). Latent inhibition and contextual associations. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 123–136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Escobar, M., Arcediano, F., Platt, T. L., & Miller, R. R. (2004). Interference and time: a brief review and an integration. Reviews in the Neurosciences, 15, 415–438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Escobar, M., Oberling, P., & Miller, R. R. (2002b). Associative deficit accounts of disrupted latent inhibition and blocking in schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 26, 203–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evenden, J. L., & Carli, M. (1985). The effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens and caudate nucleus of rats on feeding in a novel environment. Behavioural Brain Research, 15, 63–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fecteau, J. H., & Munoz, D. P. (2006). Salience, relevance, and firing: a priority map for target selection. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 382–390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldon, J., Shofel, A., & Weiner, I. (1991). Latent inhibition is unaffected by direct dopamine agonists. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 38, 309–314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldon, J., & Weiner, I. (1989). Abolition of the acquisition but not the expression of latent inhibition by chlodiazepoxide in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 32, 123–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feldon, J., & Weiner, I. (1991). The latent inhibition model of schizophrenic attention disorder: haloperidol and sulpiride enhance rats' ability to ignore irrelevant stimuli. Biological Psychiatry, 29, 635–646.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Floresco, S. B., Ghods-Sharifi, S., Vexelman, C., & Magyar, O. (2006). Dissociable roles for the nucleus accumbens core and shell in regulating set shifting. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 2449–2457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallo, M., & Candido, A. (1995). Dorsal hippocampal lesions impair blocking but not latent inhibition of taste-aversion learning in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 413–425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garcia, J., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Sciences, 4, 123–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrud, P., Rawlins, J. N. P., Mackintosh, N. J., et al. (1984). Successful overshadowing and blocking in hippocampectomized rats. Behavioural Brain Research, 12, 39–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Good, M., & Macphail, E. M. (1994). Hippocampal lesions in pigeons (Columba livia) disrupt reinforced preexposure but not overshadowing or blocking. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 47B, 263–291.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A. (1982). The Neuropsychology of Anxiety: An Enquiry into the Functions of the Septo-hippocampal System. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J. N. P., Hemsley, D. R., & Smith, A. D. (1991). The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 14, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, J. A., Kumari, V., Lawrence, N., & Young, A. M. J. (1999). Functions of the dopaminergic innervation of the nucleus accumbens. Psychobiology, 27, 225–235.Google Scholar
Gray, J. A., Moran, P. M., Grigoryan, G., et al. (1997). Latent inhibition: The nucleus accumbens connection revisited. Behavioural Brain Research, 88, 27–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, N. S., Pickering, A. D., Hemsley, D. R., Dawling, S., & Gray, J. A. (1992). Abolition of latent inhibition by a single 5 mg dose of D-amphetamine in man. Psychopharmacology, 107, 425–430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, G. (1991). Perceptual and Associative Learning. Oxford Psychology Series No. 18. Oxford: Clarendon Press, pp. 29–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, G., & Channell, S. (1985). Differential effects of contextual change on latent inhibition and on the habituation of an orienting response. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Animal Behavior Processes, 11, 470–481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, G., & Pearce, J. M. (1979). Latent inhibition of a CS during CS–US pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 31–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harmer, C. J., & Phillips, G. D. (1999). Enhanced conditioned inhibition following repeated pre-treatment with d-amphetamine. Psychopharmacology, 142, 120–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemsley, D. R. (1977). What have cognitive deficits to do with schizophrenic symptoms?British Journal of Psychiatry, 130, 167–173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemsley, D. R. (1993). A simple (or simplistic) cognitive model for schizophrenia. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 31, 633–645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hitchcock, J. M., Lister, S., Fischer, T. R., & Wettstein, J. G. (1997). Disruption of latent inhibition in the rat by the 5-HT2 agonist DOI: effects of MDL 100,907, clozapine, risperidone and haloperidol. Behavioural Brain Research, 88, 43–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Holland, P. C., & Fox, G. D. (2003). Effects of hippocampal lesions in overshadowing and blocking procedures. Behavioral Neuroscience, 3, 650–656.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Honey, R. C., & Good, M. (1993). Selective hippocampal lesions abolish the contextual specificity of latent inhibition and conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 23–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horsley, R. R., Moran, P. M., & Cassaday, H. J. (2008). Systemic amphetamine and electrolytic lesions of the nucleus accumbens (shell versus core) have dissociable effects on appetitive overshadowing. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22, 172–181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ikemoto, I. (2007). Dopamine reward circuitry: two projection systems from the ventral midbrain to the nucleus accumbens-olfactory tubercle complex. Brain Research Reviews, 56, 27–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iordanova, M. D., Westbrook, R. F., & Killcross, A. S. (2006). Dopamine activity in the nucleus accumbens modulates blocking in fear conditioning. European Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 3265–3270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, J. H., & Wagner, A. R. (1980). One-trial overshadowing: evidence of distributive processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Animal Behavior Processes, 6, 188–205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, S. H., Gray, J. A., & Hemsley, D. R. (1992). Loss of the Kamin blocking effect in acute but not chronic schizophrenics. Biological Psychiatry, 32, 739–755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. H., Hemsley, D., Ball, S., & Serra, A. (1997). Disruption of the Kamin blocking effect in schizophrenia and in normal subjects following amphetamine. Behavioural Brain Research, 88, 103–114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jongen-Relo, A. L., Kaufmann, S., & Feldon, J. (2002). A differential involvement of the shell and core subterritories of the nucleus accumbens of rats in attentional processes. Neuroscience, 111, 95–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joseph, M. H., Peters, S. L., Moran, P. M., et al. (2000). Modulation of latent inhibition in the rat by altered dopamine transmission in the nucleus accumbens at the time of conditioning. Neuroscience, 101, 921–930.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamin, L. J. (1968). Attention-like processes in classical conditioning. In Jones, M. R. (Ed.), Miami Symposium on the Prediction of Behavior: Aversive Stimulation. Miami, OH: University of Miami Press.Google Scholar
Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: a framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 13–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kapur, S. (2004). How antipsychotics become anti-‘psychotic’ – from dopamine to salience to psychosis. Trends in the Pharmacological Sciences, 25, 402–406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, H., & Pearce, J. M. (1984). The strength of the orienting response during Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 90–109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaye, H., & Pearce, J. M. (1987). Hippocampal lesions attenuate latent inhibition of a CS and of a neutral stimulus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 39B, 107–125.Google Scholar
Killcross, A. S., Dickinson, A., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Amphetamine induced disruptions of latent inhibition are reinforcer mediated: implications for animal models of schizophrenic attentional dysfunction. Psychopharmacology, 115, 185–195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killcross, A. S., Stanhope, K. J., Dourish, C. T., & Piras, G. (1997). WAY100635 and latent inhibition in the rat: selective effects at preexposure. Behavioural Brain Research, 88, 51–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Konstandi, M., & Kafetzopoulos, E. (1993). Effects of striatal or accumbens lesions on the amphetamine-induced abolition of latent inhibitions. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 44, 751–754.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koob, G. F. (1992). Neuronal mechanisms of drug reinforcement. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 654, 171–191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraemer, P. J., & Roberts, W. A. (1984). The influence of flavor preexposure and test interval on conditioned taste aversions in the rat. Learning and Motivation, 15, 259–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraemer, P. J., & Spear, N. E. (1992). The effect of nonreinforced stimulus exposure on the strength of a conditioned taste aversion as a function of retention interval: do latent inhibition and extinction involve a shared process?Animal Learning and Behavior, 20, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kumari, V., Cotter, P. A., Mulligan, O. F., et al. (1999). Effects of D-amphetamine and haloperidol on latent inhibition in healthy male volunteers. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 13, 398–405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaCroix, L., Spinelli, S., Broersen, L. M., & Feldon, J. (2000). Blockade of latent inhibition following pharmacological increase or decrease of GABAA transmission. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 66, 893–901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelley, M. E. (2004). The role of associative history in models of associative learning: a selective review and a hybrid model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 57B, 193–243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, R., & Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (2000). Segregation of working memory functions within the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Experimental Brain Research, 133, 23–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, Z. (2002). A saliency map in primary visual cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 9–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorden, J. F., Rickert, E. J., & Berry, D. W. (1983). Forebrain monoamines and associative learning: I. Latent inhibition and conditioned inhibition. Behavioural Brain Research, 9, 181–199.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorden, J. F., Rickert, E. J., Dawson, R., & Pelleymounter, M. A. (1980). Forebrain norepinephrine and the selective processing of information. Brain Research, 190, 569–573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (1989). Latent Inhibition and Conditioned Attention Theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E. (2005). The construct validity of the animal-latent inhibition model of selective attention deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 139–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lubow, R. E., & Moore, A. U. (1959). Latent inhibition: the effect of non-reinforced preexposure to the conditional stimulus. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 52, 415–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luman, M., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The impact of reinforcement contingencies on AD/HD: a review and theoretical appraisal. Clinical Psychology Review, 25, 183–213.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. J. (1975). A theory of attention: variations in the associability of stimulus with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackintosh, N. J. (1976). Overshadowing and stimulus intensity. Animal Learning & Behavior, 4, 186–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackintosh, N. J., & Reese, B. (1979). One-trial overshadowing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 31B, 519–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaren, I. P. L., & Mackintosh, N. J. (2000). An elemental model of associative learning: I. Latent inhibition and perceptual learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28, 211–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Migo, E. M., Corbett, K., Graham, J., et al. (2006). A novel test of conditioned inhibition correlates with personality measures of schizotypy and reward sensitivity. Behavioural Brain Research, 168, 299–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, R. R., & Matzel, L. D. (1988). The comparator hypothesis: A response rule for the expression of associations. In Bower, G. H. (Ed.) The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, vol. 22. San Diego, CA: Academic Press, pp. 51–92.Google Scholar
Moran, P. M., Al-Uzri, M. M., Watson, J., & Reveley, M. A. (2003). Reduced Kamin blocking in non-paranoid schizophrenia: associations with schizotypy. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 37, 155–163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, P. M., Fischer, T., Hitchcock, J. M., & Moser, P. C. (1996). Effects of clozapine on latent inhibition in the rat. Behavioral Pharmacology, 7, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, P. M., & Moser, P. C. (1992). MDL 73,147EF a 5-HT3 antagonist, facilitates latent inhibition in the rat. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 42, 519–522.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moran, P. M., Owen, L., Crookes, A. E., Al-Uzri, M. M., & Reveley, M. A. (2008). Abnormal prediction error is associated with negative and depressive symptoms in schizophrenia. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 32, 116–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, J. S., Friston, K. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1997). Neural responses to salient visual stimuli. Proceedings of the Royal Society London Series B Biological Sciences, 264, 769–775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moser, P. C., Lister, S., Hitchcock, J., & Moran, P. M. (2000). The pharmacology of latent inhibition in the rat: a review of latent inhibition as an animal model of schizophrenia. Brain Research Reviews, 33, 247–282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myers, C. E., Gluck, M. A., & Granger, R. (1995). Dissociation of hippocampal and entorhinal function in associative learning – A computational approach. Psychobiology, 23, 116–138.Google Scholar
Nicholson, D. A., & Freeman, J. H. (2002). Medial dorsal thalamic lesions impair blocking and latent inhibition of the conditioned eyeblink response in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 116, 276–285.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Norman, C., & Cassaday, H. J. (2004). Disruption of latent inhibition to a contextual stimulus with systemic amphetamine. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82, 61–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oades, R. D., Rivet, J. M., Taghzouti, K., et al. (1987). Catecholamines and conditioned blocking: effects of ventral tegmental, septal and frontal 6-hydroxydopamine lesions in rats. Brain Research, 406, 136–146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oades, R. D., Zimmermann, B., & Eggers, C. (1996). Conditioned blocking in patients with paranoid, non-paranoid psychosis or obsessive compulsive disorder: associations with symptoms, personality and monoamine metabolism. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 30, 369–390.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oberling, P., Bristol, A. S., Matute, H., & Miller, R. R. (2000). Biological significance attenuates overshadowing, relative validity, and degraded contingency effects. Animal Learning & Behavior, 28, 172–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Donnell, P., Lavin, A., Enquist, L. W., Grace, A. A., & Card, J. P. (1994). Interconnected parallel circuits between rat nucleus accumbens and thalamus revealed by retrograde transynapic transport of pseudorabies virus. Journal of Neuroscience, 17, 2143–2167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohad, D., Lubow, R. E., Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1987). The effects of amphetamine on blocking. Psychobiology, 15, 137–143.Google Scholar
O'Tuathaigh, C. M. P., & Moran, P. M. (2002). Evidence for dopamine D1 receptor involvement in the stimulus selection task overshadowing in the rat. Psychopharmacology, 162, 225–231.Google ScholarPubMed
O'Tuathaigh, C. M. P., & Moran, P. M. (2004). The effect of sulpiride on amphetamine-induced disruption of overshadowing in the rat. Progress in Neuro-psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry, 28, 1249–1253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Tuathaigh, C. M. P., Salum, C., Young, A. M. J., et al. (2003). The effect of amphetamine disruption on Kamin blocking and overshadowing. Behavioral Pharmacology, 14, 315–322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearce, J. M., & Hall, G. (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peng, R. Y., Mansbach, R. S., Braff, D. L., & Geyer, M. A. (1990). A D2 dopamine receptor agonist disrupts sensorimotor gating in rats. Implications for dopaminergic abnormalities in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 3, 211–218.Google Scholar
Pothuizen, H. H. J., Jongen-Relo, A. L., Feldon, J., & Yee, B. K. (2006). Latent inhibition of conditioned taste aversion is not disrupted, but can be enhanced, by selective nucleus accumbens shell lesions in rats. Neuroscience, 137, 1119–1130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Purves, D., Bonardi, C., & Hall, G. (1995). Enhancement of latent inhibition in rats with electrolytic lesions of the hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 109, 366–370.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, S., Harley, C., & Revusky, S. (1993). Ibotenate lesions of the hippocampus enhance latent inhibition in conditioned taste aversion and increase resistance to extinction in conditioned taste preference. Behavioral Neuroscience, 107, 996–1004.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, R. A. (1969). Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin, 72, 77–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, R. A., & Wagner, A. R. (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcenment and nonreinforcement. In Black, A. H. & Prokasy, W. F. (Eds.), Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts, pp. 64–99.Google Scholar
Restivo, L., Passino, E., Middei, S., & Ammassari-Teule, M. (2002). The strain-specific involvement of nucleus accumbens in latent inhibition might depend on differences in processing configural- and cue-based information between C57BL/6 and DBA mice. Brain Research Bulletin, 57, 35–39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rickert, E. J., Bennett, T. L., Lane, P., & French, J. (1978). Hippocampectomy and the attenuation of blocking. Behavioral Biology, 22, 147–160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rickert, E. J., Lorden, J. F., Dawson, R., & Smyly, E. (1981). Limbic lesions and the blocking effect. Physiology & Behavior, 26, 601–606.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robbins, T. W., & Koob, G. F. (1980). Selective disruption of displacement behaviour by lesions of the mesolimbic dopamine system. Nature, 285, 409–412.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rose, J. E., & Woolsey, C. N. (1948). The orbitofrontal cortex and its connections with the mediodorsal nucleus in rabbit, sheep and cat. Research Publications – Association for Research in Nervous and Mental Disease, 27, 210–232.Google Scholar
Rossi, A. F., Bichot, N. P., Desimone, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2007). Top-down attentional deficits in macaques with lesions of lateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 11306–11314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., & Kalat, J. W. (1971). Specific hungers and poisoning as adaptive specializations of learning. Psychological Review, 78, 459–486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudy, J. W., & O'Reilly, R. C. (1999). Contextual fear conditioning, conjunctive representations, pattern completion, and the hippocampus. Behavioral Neuroscience, 113, 867–880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ruob, C., Elsner, J., Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1997). Amphetamine-induced disruption and haloperidol-induced potentiation of latent inhibition depend on the nature of the stimulus. Behavioural Brain Research, 88, 35–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russig, H., Kovacevic, A., Murphy, C. A., & Feldon, J. (2003). Haloperidol and clozapine antagonise amphetamine-induced disruption of latent inhibition of conditioned taste aversion. Psychopharmacology, 170, 263–270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russig, H., Murphy, C. A., & Feldon, J. (2002). Clozapine and haloperidol reinstate latent inhibition following its disruption during amphetamine withdrawal. Neuropsychopharmacology, 26, 765–777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiller, D., Zuckerman, L., & Weiner, I. (2006). Abnormally persistent latent inhibition induced by lesions to the nucleus accumbens core, basolateral amygdala and orbitofrontal cortex is reversed by clozapine but not by haloperidol. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40, 167–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scholten, M. R. M., Honk, J., Aleman, A., & Kahn, R. S. (2006). Behavioural inhibition system (BIS), behavioural activation system (BAS) and schizophrenia: relationship with psychopathology and physiology. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 40, 638–645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmajuk, N. A., Gray, J. A., & Larrauri, J. A. (2005). A pre-clinical study showing how dopaminergic drugs administered during pre-exposure can impair or facilitate latent inhibition. Psychopharmacology, 177, 272–279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmajuk, N., Lam, Y. W., & Christiansen, B. A. (1994). Latent inhibition of the rat eyeblink response: effect of hippocampal aspiration lesions. Physiology & Behavior, 55, 597–601.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmajuk, N. A., Spear, N. E., & Isaacson, R. C. (1983). Absence of overshadowing in rats with hippocampal lesions. Physiological Psychology, 11, 59–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schultz, W. (1997). Dopamine neurons and their role in reward mechanisms. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 191–197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schultz, W. (2006). Behavioural theories and the neurophysiology of reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 87–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serra, A. M., Jones, S. H., Toone, B., & Gray, J. A. (2001). Impaired associative learning in chronic schizophrenics and their first-degree relatives: a study of latent inhibition and the Kamin blocking effect. Schizophrenia Research, 48, 273–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shadach, E., Gaisler, I., Schiller, D., & Weiner, I. (2000). The latent inhibition model dissociates between clozapine, haloperidol and ritanserin. Neuropsychopharmacology, 23, 151–161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shapiro, K. L., Jacobs, W. J., & LoLordo, V. M. (1980). Stimulus-reinforcer interactions in Pavlovian conditioning of pigeons: implications for selective associations. Animal Learning & Behavior, 8, 586–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, P. R. (1977). Role of hippocampus in blocking and conditioned inhibition of rabbits' nictitating-membrane response. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 91, 407–417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, P. R., Crider, A., Winkelman, J. W., et al. (1981). Disrupted latent inhibition in the rat with chronic amphetamine or haloperidol-induced supersensitivity: relationship to schizophrenic attention disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 16, 519–537.Google ScholarPubMed
Solomon, P. R., & Staton, D. M. (1982). Differential effects of micro-injections of d-amphetamine into the nucleus accumbens or the caudate putamen on the rat's ability to ignore an irrelevant stimulus. Biological Psychiatry, 17, 743–756.Google ScholarPubMed
Stuss, D. T. (2006). Frontal lobes and attention: processes and networks, fractionation and integration. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 12, 261–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swan, J. A., & Pearce, J. M. (1988). The orienting response as an index of stimulus associability in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology – Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 292–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tai, C.-T., Cassaday, H. J., Feldon, J., & Rawlins, J. N. P. (1995). Both electrolytic and excitotoxic lesions of nucleus accumbens disrupt latent inhibition of learning in rats. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 64, 36–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trimble, K. M., Bell, R., & King, D. J. (2002). Effects of the selective dopamine D1 antagonists NNC 01–0112 and SCH 39166 on latent inhibition in the rat. Physiology & Behavior, 77, 115–123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsaltas, E., Preston, G. C., Rawlins, J. N. P., Winocur, G., & Gray, J. A. (1984). Dorsal bundle lesions do not affect latent inhibition of conditioned suppression. Psychopharmacology, 84, 549–555.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsotsos, J. K. (1990). Analyzing vision at the complexity level. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13, 423–445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bos, R., & Cools, A. R. (2003). Switching to cue-directed behavior: specific for ventral striatal dopamine but not ventral pallidum/substantia innominata GABA as revealed by a swimming test procedure in rats. Neuroscience, 118, 141–1149.Google Scholar
Meel, C. S., Oosterlaan, J., Heslenfeld, D. J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). Telling good from bad news: ADHD differentially affects processing of positive and negative feedback during guessing. Neuropsychologia, 43, 1946–1954.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., Wang, G. -J., & Goldstein, R. Z. (2002). Role of dopamine, the frontal cortex and memory circuits in drug addiction: insight from imaging studies. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 78, 610–624.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waelti, P., Dickinson, A., & Schultz, W. (2001). Dopamine responses comply with basic assumptions of formal learning theory. Nature, 412, 43–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, A. R. (1981). SOP: A model of automatic memory processing in animal behaviour. In Spear, N. E. & Miller, R. R. (Eds.), Information Processing in Animals: Memory Mechanisms. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 5–47.Google Scholar
Weiner, I. (1990). Neural substrates of latent inhibition: the switching model. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 442–461.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I. (2003). The “two-headed” latent inhibition model of schizophrenia: modeling positive and negative symptoms and their treatment. Psychopharmacology, 169, 257–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., Bernasconi, E., Broersen, L. M., & Feldon, J. (1997). Amphetamine-induced disruption of latent inhibition depends on the nature of the stimulus. Behavioral Pharmacology, 8, 442–457.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1987). Facilitation of latent inhibition by haloperidol in rats. Psychopharmacology, 91, 248–253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., & Feldon, J. (1997). The switching model of latent inhibition: an update of neural substrates. Behavioural Brain Research, 88, 11–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., Feldon, J., & Katz, Y. (1987). Facilitation of the expression but not the acquisition of latent inhibition by haloperidol in rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 26, 241–246.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, I., Gal, G., & Feldon, J. (1999). Disrupted and undisruptable latent inhibition following shell and core lesions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 877, 723–727.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., Gal, G., Rawlins, J. N. P., & Feldon, J. (1996). Differential involvement of the shell and core subterritories of the nucleus in latent inhibition and amphetamine-induced activity. Behavioural Brain Research, 81, 123–133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiner, I., Lubow, R. E., & Feldon, J. (1981). Chronic amphetamine and latent inhibition. Behavioural Brain Research, 2, 285–286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, I., Lubow, R. E., & Feldon, J. (1984). Abolition of the expression but not the acquisition of latent inhibition by chronic amphetamine in rats. Psychopharmacology, 83, 194–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weiner, I., Lubow, R. E., & Feldon, J. (1988). Disruption of latent inhibition by acute administration of low doses of amphetamine. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 30, 871–878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westbrook, R. F., Good, A. J., & Kiernan, M. J. (1997). Microinjection of morphine into the nucleus accumbens impairs contextual learning in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 111, 996–1013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, J. H., Wellman, N. A., Geaney, D. P., et al. (1996). Antipsychotic drug effects in a model of schizophrenic attentional disorder: a randomized controlled trial of the effects of haloperidol on latent inhibition in healthy people. Biological Psychiatry, 40, 1135–1143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, J. H., Wellman, N. A., Geaney, D. P., et al. (1997). Haloperidol enhances latent inhibition in visual tasks in healthy people. Psychopharmacology, 133, 262–268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Winocur, G., Rawlins, J. N. P., & Gray, J. A. (1987). The hippocampus and conditioning to contextual cues. Behavioral Neuroscience, 101, 617–625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, R. A., & Rompre, P. -P. (1989). Brain dopamine and reward. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 191–225.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wise, R. A. (2004). Dopamine, learning and motivation. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 483–494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. M. J., Ahier, R. G., Upton, R. L., Joseph, M. H., & Gray, J. A. (1998). Increased extracellular dopamine in the nucleus accumbens of the rat during associative learning of neutral stimuli. Neuroscience, 83, 1175–1183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. M. J., Moran, P. M., & Joseph, M. H. (2005). The role of dopamine in conditioning and latent inhibition: what, where, when and how?Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews, 29, 963–976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×