Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-dvmhs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-12T19:20:08.031Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Expression Entails Anticipation

Toward a Self-Regulatory Model of Bodily Feedback Effects

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Gün R. Semin
Affiliation:
Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Amsterdam
Eliot R. Smith
Affiliation:
Indiana University, Bloomington
Jens Förster
Affiliation:
International University, Bremen, Germany, and University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Ronald S. Friedman
Affiliation:
University of Albany, State University of New York, NY, USA
Get access

Summary

Expressive behavior represents more than merely a reaction to stimuli in the external world. Obviously, individuals nod when they are in agreement and embrace when they like one another; however, because the link between information processing and expressive behavior is overlearned, over the course of a lifetime, even innate expressive behavior comes to reciprocally influence the way that individuals process information. Accordingly, research on the impact of bodily feedback has repeatedly demonstrated that unobtrusively induced expression patterns can unintentionally influence cognition, emotion, and behavior.

In this chapter, we will argue that models accounting for such effects largely overlap with models predicting affective influences on cognition and behavior because they focus on the affective meaning of the bodily patterns. However, some bodily cues can activate systems of approach or avoidance, triggering strategic inclinations that operate independent of affect. We will introduce a self-regulatory model to explain such effects. Let us first summarize some of the pertinent findings.

FACIAL AND BODILY FEEDBACK EFFECTS: SUMMARY OF RESEARCH FINDINGS

Effects of bodily expression patterns have been found on feelings, evaluative judgments, memory, and behavior. For example, several experiments have shown that unobtrusively induced expression patterns influence participants' reported feelings (e.g., Zajonc, Murphy, & Inglehart, 1989). Prominently, Stepper and Strack (1993) found that participants feel pride more intensely if they experience positive feedback while maintaining a positive, upright body position than if they receive the same feedback while in a negative, slouched position.

Type
Chapter
Information
Embodied Grounding
Social, Cognitive, Affective, and Neuroscientific Approaches
, pp. 289 - 308
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anderson, M. C., & Neely, J. H. (1996). Interference and inhibition in memory retrieval. In Bjork, E. L. & Bjork, R. A. (Eds.), Memory (pp. 237–313). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., Niedenthal, P. M., Barbey, A. K., & Ruppert, J. A. (2003). Social embodiment. In Ross, B. H. (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, Vol. 43 (pp. 43–92). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Basso, M. R., Schefft, B. K., Ris, M. D., & Dember, W. N. (1996). Mood and global-local visual processing. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 2, 249–255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumann, N., Kuhl, J., & Kazén, M. (2005). Left-hemispheric activation and self-infiltration: Testing a neuropsychological model of internalization. Motivation and Emotion, 29, 135–163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bisiach, E., Geminiani, G., Berti, A., & Rusconi, M. L. (1990). Perceptual and premotor factors of unilateral neglect. Neurology, 40, 1278–1281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36, 129–148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1980). Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on a tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 18, 491–498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burke, A., Heuer, F., & Reisberg, D. (1992). Remembering emotional events. Memory & Cognition, 20, 277–290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, W. R., Johnson, M. C., & Borkovec, T. D. (1986). Worry: An electrocortical analysis. Advances in Behavioral Research and Therapy, 8, 193–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Crites, S. L. (1996). Social neuroscience: Principles of psychophysiological arousal and response. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds). Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (pp. 72–101). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Priester, J. R., & Berntson, G. G. (1993). Rudimentary determinants of attitudes II: Arm flexion and extension have differential effects on attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 5–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality: Emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 741–751.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Centerbar, D. B., & Clore, G. L. (2006). Do approach-avoidance actions create attitudes?Psychological Science, 17, 22–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clore, G. L., Schwarz, N., & Conway, M. (1994). Affective causes and consequences of social information processing. In Wyer, R. S. & Srull, T. (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition, 2nd ed. (pp. 323–417). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2003). The state and trait nature of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion. In Hugdahl, K. and Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The asymmetrical brain (pp. 565–615). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Crowe, E., & Higgins, E. T. (1997). Regulatory focus and strategic inclinations: Promotion and prevention in decision making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 69, 117–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1998). Anxiety and attentional focusing: Trait, state and hemispheric influences. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 745–761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In Niedenthal, P. M. and Kitayama, S. (Eds.), Heart's eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 167–196). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Easterbrook, J. A. (1959). The effect of emotion on cue utilization and the organization of behavior. Psychological Review, 66, 183–201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekstrom, R. B., French, J. W., Harman, H. H., & Dermen, D. (1976). Manual for kit of factor-referenced cognitive tests. Princeton, NJ: Educational Testing Service.Google Scholar
Fiedler, K. (1988). Emotional mood, cognitive style, and behavior regulation. In Fiedler, K. & Forgas, J. (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and social behavior (pp. 100–119). Toronto: Hogrefe International.Google Scholar
Förster, J. (1995). Der Einfluß von Ausdrucksverhalten auf das menschliche Gedächtnis: Theoretische Überlegungen und Experimente zu Motor-Kongruenzeffekten [The influence of expression behavior on human memory: Theoretical reflections and experiments on motor-congruency effects]. Bonn: Holos.Google Scholar
Förster, J. (1998). The influence of motor perception on likeability judgments of attractive and unattractive portraits. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 45, 167–182.Google Scholar
Förster, J. (2003). The influence of approach and avoidance motor actions on food intake. European Journal of Social Psychology, 33, 339–350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Förster, J. (2004). How body feedback influences consumers' evaluation of products. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 14, 416–426.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Förster, J., Friedman, R. S., Özelsel, A., & Denzler, M. (2006). The influence of approach and avoidance cues on the scope of perceptual and conceptual attention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 133–146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Förster, J., Grant, H., Idson, L. C., & Higgins, E. T. (2001). Success/failure feedback, expectancies, and approach/avoidance motivation: How regulatory focus moderates classic relations. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 253–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Förster, J., & Higgins, E. T. (2005). How global versus local perception fits regulatory focus. Psychological Science, 16, 631–636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Bianco, A. T. (2003). Speed/accuracy decisions in task performance: Built-in trade-off or separate strategic concerns?Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 90, 148–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Förster, J., Higgins, E. T., & Idson, L. C. (1998). Approach and avoidance strength as a function of regulatory focus: Revisiting the “goal looms larger” effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1115–1131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Förster, J., & Stepper, S. (2000). Compatibility between approach/avoidance stimulation and valenced information determines residual attention during the process of encoding. European Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 853–871.3.0.CO;2-1>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Förster, J., & Strack, F. (1996). Influence of overt head movements on memory for valenced words: A case of conceptual-motor compatibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 421–430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 3, 218–226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2000). The effects of approach and avoidance motor actions on the elements of creative insight. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 477–492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2001). The effects of promotion and prevention cues on creativity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 1001–1013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2002). The influence of approach and avoidance motor actions on creative cognition. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 41–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2003). The influence of approach and avoidance cues on analytical problem solving. Unpublished manuscript, Universität Würzburg.
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2005). Effects of motivational cues on perceptual asymmetry: Implications for creativity and analytical problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 263–275.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, R. S., & Förster, J. (2007). Activation and measurement of motivational states. In Elliott, A. J. (Ed.), Handbook of approach and avoidance motivation. New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Gasper, K. (2004). Do you see what I see? Affect and visual information processing. Cognition and Emotion, 18, 405–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gasper, K., & Clore, G. (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information. Psychological Science, 13, 34–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heller, W., Koven, N. S., & Miller, G. A. (2003). Regional brain activity in anxiety and depression, cognition/emotion interaction, and emotion regulation. In Hugdahl, K. & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The asymmetrical brain (pp. 533–564). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Heller, W., & Nitschke, J. B. (1998). The puzzle of regional brain activity in depression and anxiety: The importance of subtypes and comorbidity. Cognition and Emotion, 12, 421–447.Google Scholar
Heller, W., Nitschke, J. B., Etienne, M. A., & Miller, G. A. (1997). Patterns of regional brain activity differentiate types of anxiety. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 376–385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T. (1997). Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52, 1280–1300.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T. (2000). Beyond pleasure and pain. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Motivational science: Social and personality perspectives (pp. 231–255). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T., Shah, J., & Friedman, R. (1997). Emotional responses to goal attainment: Strength of regulatory focus as moderator. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 515–525.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirt, E. R., McDonald, H. E., & Melton, R. J. (1996). Processing goals and the affect-performance link: Mood as main effect or mood as input? In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self- regulation (pp. 303–328). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (2000). Positive affect and decision making. In Lewis, M. & Haviland-Jones, J. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions, 2nd ed. (pp. 417–435). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M., & Daubman, K. A. (1984). The influence of affect on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47, 1206–1217.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1122–1131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M. S., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of word associations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1413–1426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, J., Heller, W., Banich, M. T., & Burton, L. A. (1983a). Are variations among right-handed individuals in perceptual asymmetries caused by characteristic arousal differences between hemispheres?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 329–359.Google Scholar
Levy, J., Heller, W., Banich, M. T., & Burton, L. A. (1983b). Asymmetry of perception in free viewing of chimeric faces. Brain & Cognition, 2, 404–419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Luu, P., Tucker, D. M., & Derryberry, D. (1998). Anxiety and the motivational basis of working memory. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22, 577–594.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikulincer, M., Kedem, P., & Paz, D. (1990a). Anxiety and categorization – 1. The structure and boundaries of mental categories. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 805–814.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikulincer, M., Paz, D., & Kedem, P. (1990b). Anxiety and categorization – 2. Hierarchical levels of mental categories. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 815–821.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, N. E. (1944). Experimental studies of conflict. In Hunt, J. M. (Ed.), Personality and the behavior disorders (pp. 431–465). Oxford: Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Milner, A. D., Brechmann, M., & Pagliarini, L. (1992). To halve and to halve not: An analysis of line bisection in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 30, 515–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, B. E. (2003). Two-hand line-bisection task outcomes correlate with several measures of hemisphericity. Brain and Cognition, 51, 305–316.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Navon, D. (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cognitive Psychology, 9, 353–383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neumann, R., Förster, J., & Strack, F. (2003). Motor compatibility: The bidirectional link between behaviour and evaluation. In Musch, J. & Klauer, K. C. (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp. 371–391). NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Nitschke, J. B., Heller, W., Palmieri, P. A., & Miller, G. A. (1999). Contrasting patterns of brain activity in anxious apprehension and anxious arousal. Psychophysiology, 36, 628–637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohlsson, S. (1984). Restructuring revisited: II. An information processing theory of restructuring and insight. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 25, 117–129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reeves, F. B., & Bergum, B. O. (1972). Perceptual narrowing as a function of peripheral cue relevance. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 35, 719–724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roseman, I. J. (1984). Cognitive determinants of emotions: A structural theory. In Shaver, P. (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology, Vol. 5 (pp. 11–36). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Schooler, J. W., & Melcher, J. (1995). The ineffability of insight. In Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 97–134). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, E. T. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior, Vol. 2 (pp. 527–561). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (2007). Feelings and phenomenal experiences. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. (Eds.), Social psychology. Handbook of basic principles, 2nd ed. (pp. 385–407). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Shiv, B., & Fedirikhin, A. (1999). Heart and mind in conflict: The interplay between affect and cognition in consumer decision making. Journal of Consumer Research, 26, 278–292.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, S. M. (1995). Fixation, incubation, and insight in memory, problem solving, and creativity. In Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 135–155). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Schumacher, J. S. (1993). Constraining effects of examples in a creative generation task. Memory & Cognition, 21, 837–845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stepper, S., & Strack, F. (1993). Proprioceptive determinants of emotional and nonemotional feelings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 211–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strack, E. (1992). The different routes to social judgments: Experiential versus informational strategies. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), The construction of social judgment (pp. 249–275). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L. L., & Stepper, S. (1988). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tucker, D. M., & Williamson, P. A. (1984). Asymmetric neural control systems in human self-regulation. Psychological Review, 91, 185–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, S. K., & Tucker, D. M. (1982). Anxiety and perceptual structure: Individual differences in neuropsychological function. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 91, 210–220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, G. L., & Petty, R. E. (1980). The effects of overt head movements on persuasion: Compatibility and incompatibility of responses. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 1, 219–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weltman, G., Smith, J. E., & Edstrom, G. H. (1971). Perceptual narrowing during pressure-chamber exposure. Human Factors, 13, 99–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witkin, H. A., Oltman, P. K., Raskin, E., & Karp, S. A. (1971). A manual for the embedded figures test. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B., & Markus, H. (1984). Affect and cognition: The hard interface. In Izard, C., Kagan, J., & Zajonc, R. B. (Eds.), Emotions, cognition, and behavior (pp. 73–102). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B., Murphy, S. T., & Inglehart, M. (1989). Feeling and facial efference: Implications of the vascular theory of emotion. Psychological Review, 96, 395–416.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Expression Entails Anticipation
    • By Jens Förster, International University, Bremen, Germany, and University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Ronald S. Friedman, University of Albany, State University of New York, NY, USA
  • Gün R. Semin, Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Amsterdam, Eliot R. Smith, Indiana University, Bloomington
  • Book: Embodied Grounding
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805837.013
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Expression Entails Anticipation
    • By Jens Förster, International University, Bremen, Germany, and University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Ronald S. Friedman, University of Albany, State University of New York, NY, USA
  • Gün R. Semin, Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Amsterdam, Eliot R. Smith, Indiana University, Bloomington
  • Book: Embodied Grounding
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805837.013
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Expression Entails Anticipation
    • By Jens Förster, International University, Bremen, Germany, and University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands, Ronald S. Friedman, University of Albany, State University of New York, NY, USA
  • Gün R. Semin, Koninklijke Nederlandse Akademie van Wetenschappen, Amsterdam, Eliot R. Smith, Indiana University, Bloomington
  • Book: Embodied Grounding
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805837.013
Available formats
×