Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-68945f75b7-6rp8b Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-02T19:24:05.849Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2005

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbott, V., Black, J., & Smith, E. (1985). The representation of scripts in memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 179–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adler, A. (1931). What life should mean to you. Oxford: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Adolph, K. (1997). Learning in the development of infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62.CrossRef
Adolph, K. (2000). Specificity of learning: Why infants fall over a vertiable cliff. Psychological Science, 11, 290–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G., & Damasio, A. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 2683–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aggleton, J., & Mishkin, M. (1986). The amygdala: Sensory gateway to the emotions. In , R. Plutchik & , H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (pp. 281–9). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Aglioti, S., Goodale, M., & DeSouza, J. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Agre, P., & Chapman, D. (1987). Pengi: An implementation of a theory of activity. Proceedings of AAAI-87. Menlo Park: AAAI.
Ahsen, A. (1995). Self-report questionnaires: New directions for imagery research. Journal of Mental Imagery, 19, 107–22.Google Scholar
Akshoomoff, N., Courchesne, E., & Townsend, J. (1997). Attention coordination and anticipatory control. International Review of Neurobiology, 411, 575–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M., & DiRusso, A. (1999). The function of gesture in learning to count: More than keeping track. Cognitive Development, 14, 37–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M., Kita, S., & Young, A. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 593–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allison, T., Puce, R., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception for visual cases of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 267–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, E. (1995). Seeing is believing: Visual categories in the Russian lexicon. In , E. Contini-Morava & , B. Goldberg (Eds.), Meaning as explanation (pp. 363–77). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Anifeld, M. (1996). Only tongue protrusion modeling is matched by neonates. Developmental Review, 16, 149–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arditi, A., Holtzman, J., & Kosslyn, S. (1988). Mental imagery and seeing experiences in congenital blindness. Neuropsychologia, 26, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, M. (1946). On the mechanism of suggestion and hypnosis. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 107–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, M., & Gasson, S. (1954). Feelings and emotions as dynamic factors in personality integration. In , M. Arnold & , S. Gasson (Eds.), The human person (pp. 294–313). New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Asci, F. (2003). The effect of physical fitness training on trait anxiety and physical self-concept of female university students. Psychology of Sports and Exercise, 4, 255–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Attneave, F., & Olson, R. (1967). Discriminability of stimuli varying in physical and retinal orientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 149–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ax, A. (1953). The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 433–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayer, A. (1936). Language, truth, and logic. London: Gollancz.Google Scholar
Ayers, T., & Jonides, J. (1979). Differing suffix effects for the same physical suffix. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 5, 315–21.Google Scholar
Baars, B. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Babcock, M., & Freyd, J. (1988). Perception of dynamic information in static handwritten forms. American Journal of Psychology, 101, 111–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bach-y-Rita (1996). Sustitucion sensorielle et qualia. In , J. Proust (Ed.), Perception et intermodalite (pp. 81–100). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In , G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and memory: Vol. 8 (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Lieberman, K. (1980). Spatial working memory. In , R. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 521–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. (1988). Intermodal learning in infancy: Learning on the basis of two kinds of invariant relational in audible and visible events. Child Development, 59, 197–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, L. (1995). Intermodal origins of self-perception. In , P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy (pp. 349–73). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. (2000). Increasing specificity in development of intermodal perception. In , D. Muir & , A. Slater (Eds.), Infant development: The essential readings (pp. 119–37). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L., & Watson, S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis for self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bai, D., & Bertenthal, B. (1992). Locomotor structure and the development of spatial search skills. Child Development, 63, 215–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, D. (1998). Getting a grip: A computational model of the acquisition of verb semantics for hand actions. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, International Computer Science Institute, University of California, Berkeley.
Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, R. (1987a). Object permanence in 3.5 and 4.5 month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1987b). Young infants' responding about the physical and spatial properties of a hidden object. Cognitive Development, 2, 179–200.
Baillargeon, R. (1993). The object concept revisited: New direction in the investigation of infant's physical knowledge. In , C. Granud (Ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 265–313). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1994). Object permanence in young infants: Further evidence. Child Development, 62, 1227–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1995). Physical reasoning in infancy. In , M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Physical reasoning in infancy (pp. 187–204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (2000). How do infants learn about the physical world. In , D. Muir & , A. Slater (Eds.), Infant development: The essential readings (pp. 195–212). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants' physical reasoning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 89–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young infants: Further evidence. Child Development, 114, 1227–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., DeVos, J., & Graber, M. (1989). Location memory in 8-month-old infants in a non-search AB task: Further evidence. Cognitive Development, 4, 345–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., & Graber, M. (1988). Evidence of location memory in 8-month-old infants in a non-search AB task. Developmental Psychology, 24, 502–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Needham, A., DeVos, J. (1992). The development of young infants' intuitions about support. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 69–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, L. (2000). Bodies and persons. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D., & Baird, J. (2001). Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 171–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, D., Hayhoe, M., Pook, P., & Rao, R. (1997). Deictic codes and the embodiment of cognition. Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 20, 723–67.Google ScholarPubMed
Bargh, J., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11, 211–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. (1985). Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 11, 629–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. (1987). The instability of graded structure in concepts. In , U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 101–40). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1989). Intra-concept similarity and its implications for inter-concept similarity. In , S. Vosniadou & , A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 76–121). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1991). Deriving categories to achieve goals. In , G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 27 (pp. 1–64). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1995). Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: Manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. In , A. Collins, , S. Gathercole, , M. Conway, & , P. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1999a). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1999b). Language comprehension: Archival memory or preparation for situated action. Discourse Processes, 28, 61–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. (2002). Being there conceptually: Simulating categories in preparation for situated action. In , N. Stein & , P. Bauer (Eds.), Representation, memory, and development: Essays in honor of Jean Mandler (pp. 1–15). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language & Cognitive Processes, 18, 513–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L., & Medin, D. (1986). Concepts: Fixed definitions or dynamic context-dependent representations?Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 6, 187–202.Google Scholar
Bassili, J. (1978). Facial motion in the perception of faces and emotional expression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 373–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Basso, K. (1990). Western Apache language and culture: Essays in linguistic anthropology. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. (1996). What do infants recall of their lives? Memory for specific events by one- to two-year-olds. American Psychologist, 51, 29–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, P. (1997). Development of memory in early childhood. In , N. Cowan (Ed.), The development of memory in childhood (pp. 83–111). Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bavac-Cikoja, D., & Turvey, M. (1995). Does perceived size depend on perceived distance? An argument for extended haptic perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 216–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J., Black, A., Lemery, C., & Mullet, J. (1986). “I show how you feel”: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beach, K. (1988). The role of external mnemonic system in acquiring an occupation. In , M. Gruneberg & , P. Morris (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 342–6). Oxford: Wiley.Google Scholar
Beardsworth, T., & Buckner, T. (1981). The ability to recognize oneself from a video recording of one's movement without seeing one's body. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 19–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, A., & Ward, T. (1991). Children's use of shape in extending novel labels to animate objects: Identity versus postural change. Cognitive Development, 6, 3–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, F. (2001). The meanings of war & peace. College Station: Texas A & M Press.Google Scholar
Beer, R. (1997). The dynamics of adaptive behavior. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 20, 257–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, R. (2003). The dynamics of active categorical perception in an evolved model agent. Adaptive Behavior, 11, 209–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beitel, D., Gibbs, R., & Sanders, P. (2001). The embodied approach to the polysemy of the spatial preposition “on.” In , H. Cuyckens (Ed.), Polsemy in cognitive linguistics (pp. 241–60). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bergan, B., & Chang, N. (in press). Simulation-based language understanding in embodied construction grammar. In , J.-O. Ostman & , M. Fried (Eds.), Construction grammar(s): Cognitive and cross-linguistic dimensions. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Berko, J., Burke, L., Craven, J., & Sarlo, N. (1992). The importance of motor activity in sensorimotor development: A perspective for children with physical handicaps. Human Development, 35, 226–40.Google Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (2000). Causes and consequences of feelings. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, L., & Troccoli, B. (1990). Feelings, direction of attention, and expressed evaluations of others. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 305–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermond, B., Nieuwenhuyse, B., Fasolti, S., & Schuerman, J. (1991). Spinal cord lesions, peripheral feedback, and intensities of emotional feelings. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 201–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermudez, J., Marcel, A., & Eilan, N. (Eds.) (1995). The body and the self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. (1993). Infants' perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic and knowledge-based constraints. In , C. Granrud (Ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 175–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B., & Campos, J. (1987). New directions in the study of early experience. Child Development, 58, 560–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertenthal, B., Campos, J., & Barrett, K. (1984). Self-produced locomotion: An organizer of emotional, cognitive, and social development in infancy. In , R. Ende & , R. Harmon (Eds.), Continuties and discontinuities in development (pp. 174–210). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B., Campos, J., & Kermoian, R. (1994). An epigenetic perspective on the development of self-produced locomotion and its consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 140–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertenthal, B., & Pinto, J. (1993). Complementary processes in the perception and production of human movements. In , L. Smith and , E. Thelen (Eds.), A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications (pp. 209–39). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Berthier, N., DeBlois, S., Poirer, C., Novack, M., & Clifton, R. (2000). Where's the ball? Two- and three-year olds reasons about unseen events. Developmental Psychology, 36, 394–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthoz, A. (2000). The brain's sense of movement. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bigelow, A. (1992). Locomotion and search behavior in blind infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 179–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S-J., Wolpert, D., & Firth, C. (2000). Why can't you tickle yourself?NeuroReport, 11, R11–R16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloch, H. (1990). Structure and function of early sensorymotor coordinations. In , H. Bloch & , B. Bertenthal (Eds.), Sensorymotor organization and development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 163–78). New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1993). Language development from two to three. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, P., & German, T. (2001). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77, B25–B31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogartz, R., & Shinsky, J. (1998). On the perception of partially-occluded objects in 6-month-olds. Cognitive Development, 13, 141–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boone, T., & Cunningham, J. (1998). Children's decoding of emotion in expressive body movements: The development of cue attunement. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1007–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers' conception of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13, 185–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boschker, M., Bakker, F., & Michaels, C. (2002). Effect of mental imagery in realizing affordances. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 775–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, J., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature, 391, 756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G., Black, J., & Turner, T. (1979). Scripts in memory for texts. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 177–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, S., & Stark, L. (1997). Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflects the contents of the visual scene. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 27–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility 6 between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain & Cognition, 44, 124–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brecht, M., Singer, W., & Engel, A. (1998). Correlation analysis of corticotectal interactions in the cat visual system. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79, 2394–2407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgeman, B. (1983). Mechanisms of space constancy. In , A. Hein & , M. Jeannerod (Eds.), Spatially-oriented behavior (pp. 263–79). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B. (2000). Interaction between vision for perception and vision for behavior. In , Y. Rossetti & , A. Revonsuo (Eds.), Interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing (pp. 17–40). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Kirch, M., & Sperling, A. (1981). Segregation of cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G., & Nagle, M. (1979). Relations between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 692–700.Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Peery, S., & Anand, S. (1997). Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 456–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridges, K., & Goldberg, D. (1992). Somatization in primary health care: Prevalence and determinants. In , B. Cooper & , R. Eastwood (Eds.), Primary health care and psychiatric epidemiology (pp. 341–50). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Brighton, V., Segal, A., Werther, P., & Steiner, J. (1977). Facial expression and hedonic response to taste stimuli. Journal of Dental Research, 56, B161.Google Scholar
Brooks, L. (1968). Spatial and verbal components of the act of recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22, 349–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. (1991). Intelligence without representations. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 139–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. (2002). Flesh and machine: How robots will change us. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Browder, J., & Gallagher, J. (1948). Dorsal cordotomy for painful phantom limb. Annals of Surgery, 128, 456–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (1992). Articulatory phonology: An overview. Phonetica, 49, 155–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (1995). Dynamics and articulatory phonology. In , R. Port & Gelder, T. (Eds.), Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition (pp. 175–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, J., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18, 32–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brownlow, S., Dixon, A., Egbert, C., & Radcliffe, R. (1997). Perception of movement and dancer characteristics for point-light displays of dance. Psychological Record, 47, 411–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brugger, P., Regard, M., & Shiffrar, M. (2000). Hand movement observation in a person born without hands: Is body schema innate? Meeting of the Swiss Neurological Society, London, England, Sept. 2000.
Brugman, C., & Lakoff, G. (1988). Cognitive typology and lexical networks. In , S. Small, , G. Gorrell, & , M. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Lexical ambiguity resolution (pp. 477–508). Palo Alto, CA: Morgan Kaufman.Google Scholar
Brugman, C., & McCaulay, M. (1986). Interacting semantic systems: Mixtec expressions of location. Berkeley Linguistic Society, 12, 315–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J., Goodnow, J., & Austin, G. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bruno, N. (2001). When does action resist visual illusions?Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 379–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, D., & Wright, G. (1999). How body asymmetries determine accessibility in spatial function. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 487–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullitt-Jones, M. (1999). Hunger: A memoir of desire. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Burgess, C. (2000). Theory and operational definitions in computational memory models. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 482–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, C., & Lund, K. (2000). The dynamics of meaning in memory. In , E. Dietrich & , A. Markman (Eds.), Cognitive dynamics (pp. 117–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Burton, G. (1992). Nonvisual judgments of the crossability of path gap. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 698–713.Google Scholar
Butcher, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In , D. McNeil (Ed.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action (pp. 167–91). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Butler, S., Berthier, N., & Clifton, R. (2002). Two-year-olds' search strategies and visual tracking in a hidden displacement task. Developmental Psychology, 38, 581–90.
Butterworth, B., & Beattie, G. (1978). Gesture and silence as indicators of planning in speech. In , R. Campbell & , P. Smith (Eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language (pp. 347–60). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Butterworth, B., & Hadar, U. (1989). Gesture, speech, and computational stages: A reply to McNeil. Psychological Review, 96, 168–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G. (1993). Dynamic approaches to infant perception and action: Old and new theories about the origins of knowledge. In , L. Smith & , E. Thelen (Eds.), A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications (pp. 171–87). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J., Klein, D., Bernston, G., & Hatsfield, E. (1993). The psychophysiology of emotion. In , M. Lewis & , J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 119–42). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Camras, L., Lambrecht, L., & Michel, G. (1996). Infant “surprise” expressions as coordinative motor structures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 183–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, R. (1997). Experienced cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carpenter, P., & Eisenberg, P. (1978). Mental rotation and the frame of reference in blind and sighted individuals. Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 117–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, W. (1874). Principles of mental physiology, with their applications to the training and discipline of the mind and the study of its morbid conditions. New York: Appelton.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Carriedo, N., Alonso, M., & Fernandez, A. (1997). The role of verb tense and verb aspect in the foregrounding of information during reading. Memory & Cognition, 25, 438–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrera-Levillain, P., & Fernandez-Dols, J-M. (1994). Neutral faces in context: Their emotional meaning and their function. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 281–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll-Phelen, B., & Hampson, P. (1996). Multiple components of the perception of musical sequences: A cognitive neuroscience analysis and some implications for auditory imagery. Music Perception, 13, 517–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, L., & Bauer, P. (1999). When the event is more than the sum of its parts: 9-month-olds' long-term ordered recall. Memory, 7, 147–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cary, M., & Carlson, R. (1999). External support and the development of problem-solving routines. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1053–70.Google Scholar
Case, R. (1992). The mind's staircase. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cassell, J., Bickmore, T., Campbell, L., Vihjammsson, H., & Yan, H. (2001). More than just a pretty face: Conversational protocols and the affordances of embodiment. Knowledge-Based Systems, 14, 55–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castiello, U. (1996). Grasping a fruit: Selection for action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 582–603.Google Scholar
Cataldi, S. (1996). Emotion, depth, and flesh. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Chalmers, D. (1996). The conscious mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chambers, D., & Reisberg, D. (1992). What an image depicts depends on what an image means. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 145–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Z., Sanchez, R., & Campbell, T. (1997). From beyond to within their grasp: Analogical problem solving in 10- and 13-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 790–801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Churchland, P. (1984). Matter and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. (1985). Reduction, qualia, and the direct introspection of brain states. Journal of Philosophy, 82, 8–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P., Ramachandran, V., & Sejinowski, T. (1994). A critique of pure vision. In , C. Koch & , J. Davis (Eds.), Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain (pp. 23–60). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (1998). Straight: An image schema and its metaphorical extensions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9, 107–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancey, W. (1997). Situated cognition: On human knowledge and computer representations. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1996). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). The dynamic challenge. Cognitive Science, 21, 461–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2003). Natural-born cyborgs: Minds, technologies, and the future of human intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. (1996). Using language. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, E. (2001). Meaning and the specification of motion in music. Musicae Scientiae, 5, 213–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, S., & French, R. (1978). Can congenital amputees achieve academically?American Corrective Therapy Journal, 32, 7–11.Google ScholarPubMed
Clearfield, M. (2000). The role of locomotor experience in the development of navigational memory. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation.
Clement, C. (1987). Applying general principles to novel problems as a function of learning history: Abstraction from examples vs. studying general statements. Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 585.Google Scholar
Coccia, M., Bartolini, M., Luzzi, S., Provinciali, L., & Ralph, M. (2004). Semantic memory is an amodal, dynamic system: Evidence from the interaction of naming and object use in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 515–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, L., & Oakes, L. (1993). How infants perceive a simple causal event. Developmental Psychology, 29, 421–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colcombe, S., & Wyer, R. (2001). The role of prototypes in the mental representation of temporally related events. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 67–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, J. (1995). Pride and the daily marathon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cole, J. (1997). On “being faceless”: Selfhood and facial embodiment. Journal of Consciousness, 4, 467–84.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Hood, L., & McDermott, R. (1997). Concepts of ecological validity: Their differing implications for comparative cognition. In , M. Cole & , Y. Engestroem (Eds.), Mind, culture, and activity (pp. 48–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Collie, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Deferred imitation by 6- and 9-month-old infants: More evidence for declarative memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 83–90.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, L., & Shepard, R. (1982). Mental imagery and their transformations. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Connell, J. (1989). A colony architecture for an artificial machine. AI Tech Report 1152, MIT AI Labs, August.
Corballis, M. (1994). Neuropsychology of perceptual functions. In , D. Zaidel (Ed.), Neuropsychology handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed.) (pp. 83–104). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Corriss, D., & Kose, G. (1998). Action and imagination in the formation of images. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 979–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1997). Dissecting the computational architecture of social inference mechanisms. Characterizing human psychological adaptations. Ciba Foundation symposium, No. 208 (pp. 132–61). New York: Wiley.
Craighero, L. (1996). Grasping a fruit: Selection for action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 582–603.Google Scholar
Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umilta, C. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1673–92.Google ScholarPubMed
Creem, S., & Proffitt, D. (1998). Two memories for geographical slant: Separation and interdependence of action and awareness. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 22–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1996). Why neuroscience may be able to explain consciousness. Scientific American, 273, 84–85.Google Scholar
Crott, W., & Cruse, A. (2004). Cognitive Linguistics. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Perennial.Google Scholar
Csordas, T. (Ed.) (1994). Embodiment and experience. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cutting, J., Proffitt, D., & Kozlowski, L. (1978). A biomechanical invariant for gait perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 357–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A system-level proposal for the neural substrate of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: G.P. Putnam & Sons.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace & Co.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. New York: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Damasio, A., & Damasio, H. (1994). Cortical systems for retrieval of concrete knowledge: The convergence zone framework. In , C. Koch & , J. Davis (Eds.), Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain (pp. 61–74). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human action. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 172–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., Grezess, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Grassi, F., & Fazio, F. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions: Influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain, 120, 1763–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Problanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioral Brain Research, 34, 35–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Bettinardi, V., Tadary, B., Woods, R., Mazziotta, J., & Fazio, F. (1994). Mapping motor representations with PET. Nature, 371, 600–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLoache, J., Uttal, D., & Rosengren, K. (2004). Scale errors offer evidence for a perception-action dissociation early in life. Science, 304, 1027–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. (1992). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Rivera, J. (1977). A structural theory of the emotions. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1984). The philosophical writings of Descartes: vol. 2. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3, 357–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Minton, Balch.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The theory of inquiry. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (1991). Neuro-psychological insights into the meaning of object concept development. In , S. Carey and , R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology & cognition (pp. 67–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., & Knippenberg, A. (1998). The relation between perception and behavior, or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 865–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., Bargh, J., & Miedema, J. (2001). Of mice and mackerels: Attention and automatic social behavior. In , H. Bless & , J. Forgas (Eds.), Subjective experience in social cognition and behavior (pp. 37–51). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
DiPelligrinoi, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
diSessa, A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition & Instruction, 10, 105–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodd, B. (1979). Lipreading in infancy: Attention to speech in and out of synchrony. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 478–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doffner, G. (1999). The connectionist route to embodiment and dynamicism. In , A. Riegler, , M. Peschl, & , A. von Stein (Eds.), Understanding representation in the cognitive sciences: Does representational need reality? (pp. 23–32). New York: Kluwer..Google Scholar
Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Dorman, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Raphael, L. (1977). Stop-consonant recognition: Release bursts and formant transitions as functionally equivalent, context-dependent cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 22, 109–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, M. (1970). Natural symbols. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Downing, L. (2000). Negation, text worlds, and discourse: The pragmatics of fiction. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Duclos, S., Laird, J., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stern, L., & Lighten, O. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions and postures on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 100–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagle, R. (1985). Deprivation of early sensorimotor experience and cognition in the severely involved cerebral palsy child. Journal of Autism, and Developmental Disorders, 15, 269–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: Evidence from psychophysiological studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 335–59.Google ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies: clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics, and marriage. New York: Norton.
Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions?Psychological Review, 99, 550–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell's mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 268–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. (Eds.) (1994). The nature of emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R., & Friesen, W. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishing among emotions. Science, 221, 1208–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N., & Hennelly, R. (1980). A bilingual word-length effect: Implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 43–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (1995). Questioning consciousness. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2002). Efferent brain processes and the enactive approach to consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 40–52.Google Scholar
Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2000). Micro-affordances: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 457–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition, and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Kosslyn, S., & Bellugi, U. (1993). Visual imagery and visual-spatial language: Enhanced imagery abilities in deaf and hearing ASL signers. Cognition, 46, 139–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelkamp, J. (1998). Memory for actions. Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. (1984). Motor programme information as a separable unit. Psychological Research, 46, 283–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, W. (1973). The process of ‘taking-into-account’ in visual perception. Perception, 2, 267–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M., Hammond, K., Levine, D., & Calvanio, R. (1988). Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 439–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M., & McClelland, J. (1991). A computational model of semantic memory impairment: Modality specificity and emergent category-specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 339–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farnell, B. (1995). Do you see what I mean? Plains Indian sign talk and the embodiment of action. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, J., & Narayanan, S. (2004). Embodiment in a neural theory of language. Brain & Language, 89, 385–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernandez-Dols, J-M., & Carrera, P. (1994). Neutral faces in context: Their emotional meaning and their function. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 287–99.Google Scholar
Fernandez-Dols, J-M., Carrera, P., Casado, C. (2002). The meaning of expression: Views from art and other sources. In , L. Anolli, , R. Ciceri, & , G. Riva (Eds.), Say not to say: New perspectives on miscommunication (pp. 117–34). Amsterdam: IOS Press.Google Scholar
Fernandez-Dols, J-M., & Ruiz-Belda, M. (1995). Are smiles signs of happiness? Gold medal winners at the Olympic games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1113–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrara, K. (1994). Therapeutic ways with words. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
Fery, J.-C. (2003). Differentiating visual and kinesthetic imagery in mental practiceCanadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, ⅹⅹ–ⅹⅹ.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fillmore, C. (1982). Frame semantics. In Linguistic Society of Korea (Ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm (pp. 111–38). Hansin, Seoul.
Finke, R. (1989). Principles of mental imagery. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Finke, R., & Freyd, J. (1985). Transformations of visual memory induced by implied motions of pattern elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 780–94.Google ScholarPubMed
Finke, R., Freyd, J., & Shyi, G. (1986). Implied velocity and acceleration induce transformations of visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 175–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, S. (1990). The evolution of psychological concepts about the body. In , T. Cash & , T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance, and change (pp. 3–20). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, P., Carello, C., Schmidt, R., & Corey, D. (1994). Haptic and visual perception of an affordance for upright posture. Ecological Psychology, 6, 265–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, O. (2002). The problem of the soul: Two visions of mind and how to reconcile them. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Flanagan, R., & Beltzer, M. (2000). Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor prediction in the size-weight illusion. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 737–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flanagan, R., King, S., Wolpert, D., & Johansson, R. (2001). Sensorimotor prediction and memory in object manipulation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 87–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, C., van den Broek, P., & Arthur, E. (1996). A model of narrative comprehension and recall. In , B. Britton & , A. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp. 142–64). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fletcher, R. (1994). Levels of representation in memory for discourse. In , M. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 589–608). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Flores d'Arcais, G., & Schreuder, R. (1982). Semantic activation during object naming. Psychological Research, 49, 153–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fogel, A., & Hannan, T. (1985). Manual actions of nine- to fifteen-week-old human infants during face-to-face interactions with their mothers. Child Development, 56, 1271–9.Google Scholar
Foster, D. (1983). Visual discrimination, categorical identification, and categorical rating in brief displays of curved lines: Implications for discrete encoding processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 785–806.Google ScholarPubMed
Foster, J., & Strack, F. (1996). Influence of overt head movement on memory for valenced words: A case of conceptual-motor compatibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 421–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, C. (1986). An event approach to the study of speech perception from a direct-realist perspective. Journal of Phonetics, 14, 3–28.Google Scholar
Fowler, C. (1987). Perceivers as realists, talkers too: Commentary on papers by Strange, Diehl et al., and Rakerd and Verbrugge. Journal of Memory & Language, 26, 574–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, C. (1994). Auditory “objects” – The role of motor activity in auditory perception and speech perception. In , K. Pribram (Ed.), Origins: Brain and self-organization (pp. 593–603). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fowler, C., & Rosenblum, D. (1991). The perception of phonetic gestures. In , I. Mattingly & , M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Modularity and the motor theory of speech perception (pp. 33–59). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Franz, V., Gegenfurtner, K., Buelthoff, H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11, 20–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, N., Lloyd, S., & Sinha, C. (1980). Infant search tasks reveal early concepts of containment and canonical usage of objects. Cognition, 8, 243–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, W. (1991). The physiology of perception. Scientific American, 264, 78–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, W. (2001). How brains make up their minds. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Freyd, J. (1987). Dynamic mental representation. Psychological Review, 94, 429–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freyd, J., & Finke, R. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126–32.Google Scholar
Freyd, J., & Finke, R. (1985). A velocity effect for representational momentum. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 443–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freyd, J., & Johnson, J. (1987). Probing the time course of representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126–32.Google Scholar
Freyd, J., & Jones, K. (1994). Representational momentum for a spiral path. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 968–76.Google ScholarPubMed
Freyd, J., Kelly, M., & DeKay, M. (1990). Representational momentum in memory for pitch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1107–17.Google ScholarPubMed
Freyd, J., & Pantzer, T. (1995). Static patterns moving in the mind. In , S. Smith, , T. Ward, & , R. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 181–204). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Freyd, J., Pantzer, T., & Cheng, J. (1988). Representing statics as forces in equilibrium. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 395–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friberg, A., & Sundberg, J. (1994). Does music performance allude to locomotion? A model of final ritardandi derived from measurements of stopping runners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, 1469–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friberg, A., & Sundberg, J. (1999). Does music performance allude to locomotion? A model of final ritardandi derived from measurements of stopping runners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, 1469–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friberg, A., Sundberg, J., & Fryden, L. (2000). Music for motion: Sound level envelopes of tones expressing human locomotion. Journal of New Musice Rsearch, 24, 199–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A. (1994). Human facial expression. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, R., & Foerster, J. (2000). The effect of approach and avoidance motor actions on the elements of creative insight. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 477–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C., Blakemore, S., & Wolport, D. (2000). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 357–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G., Silveri, M., Daniele, A., & Giustolisi, L. (1995). Neuroimagining correlates of category-specific semantic disorders: A critical survey. Memory, 3, 247–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. (1995). Body schema and intentionality. In , J. L. Bermudez & , A. Marcel (Eds.), The body and self (pp. 225–44). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2001). The practice of mind: Theory, simulation, or primary interaction?Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 83–103.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Marcel, A. (1999). The self in contextualized action. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 212–28.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Meltzoff, A. (1996). The emerging sense of self and others: Merleau-Ponty and recent developmental studies. Philosophical Psychology, 9, 211–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V. (2000). The inner sense of action: Agency and motor representationsJournal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 23–40.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Ferari, P., & Umilta, M. (2002). The mirror matching system: A shared manifold for intersubjectivity. Behaviorial and Brain Sciences, 25, 35–6.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirron neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 439–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theories of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1985). The mind's new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, R., Martinez, R., & Sandoval, Y. (1987). Obesity and body image: An evaluation of sensory and non-sensory components. Psychological Medicine, 17, 927–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrod, S., & Sanford, A. (1985). On the real-time character of interpretation during reading. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 43–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garry, M., & Polaschik, D. (2000). Imagination and memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 6–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (1993). Vagueness's puzzles, polysemy's vagueness. Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 223–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (1997). Diachronic prototype semantics: A contribution to historical lexicography. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1979). Meaning and order in Morocan society. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, R. (1991). Epigenetic foundations of knowledge structures: Initial and transcendent construction. In , S. Carey and , R. Gelman (Eds.), Epigenesis of mind: Essays in biology and cognition (pp. 293–322). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gelman, R., Durgin, F., & Kaufman, L. (1995). Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by motion alone. In , D. Sperber & , D. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition (pp. 150–84). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., Imai, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2002). As time goes by: Understanding time as spatial metaphor. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 537–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgopoulos, A., Lurito, J., Petrides, M., Schwartz, A., & Massey, J. (1989). Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science, 243, 234–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z.Csiba, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1999). Early socio-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model. In , P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 101–36). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. (1991). The saturated self. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gerlach, C., Law, I., & Paulson, O. (2002). When action turns into words: Activation of motor-based knowledge during categorization of manipulable objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1230–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geurts, K. (2002). Culture and the senses: Bodily ways of knowing in an African community. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1996). Why many concepts are metaphorical. Cognition, 61, 309–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R. (1999a). Intentions in the experience of meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1999b). Moving metaphor out of the head and into the cultural world. In , R. Gibbs & , G. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 145–66). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., Beitel, D., Harrington, M., & Sanders, P. (1994). Taking a stand on the meanings of “stand”: Bodily experience as motivation for polysemy. Journal of Semantics, 11, 231–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Berg, E. (2002). Mental imagery and embodied activity. Journal of Mental Imagery, 26, 1–30.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Franks, H. (2002). Embodied metaphor in women's narratives about their experiences with cancer. Health Communication, 14, 139–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R., Lima, P., & Francuzo, E. (2004). Metaphor in thought and language is grounded in embodied experience. Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 1189–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Matlock, T. (2000). Psycholinguistics and mental representations. Cognitive Linguistics, 10, 263–9.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & O'Brien, J. (1990). Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning. Cognition, 36, 35–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R., Strom, L., & Spivey-Knowlton, M. (1997). Conceptual metaphor in mental imagery for proverbs. Journal of Mental Imagery, 21, 83–110.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Tenney, Y. (1980). The concept of scripts in understanding stories. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 9, 275–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R., & Orden, G. (2003). Are emotional expressions intentional? A self-organizational approach. Consciousness & Emotion, 4, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 1–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. (1962). Observations on active touch. Psychological Review, 69, 477–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston.
Gibson, J. (1968). What gives rise to the perception of motion?Psychological Review, 57, 335–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gilbert, A., Fridlund, A., & Sabini, J. (1987). Hedonic and social determinants of facial displays to odors. Chemical Senses, 12, 355–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. (1997). What is memory for?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. (1999). Why mental models need to be embodied. In , G. Rickert & , C. Habel (Eds.), Mental models in discourse processing (pp. 77–90). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A., & Robertson, D. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A., Meyer, M., & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 69–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A., Schroeder, J., & Robertson, D. (1998). Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering. Memory & Cognition, 26, 651–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glucksberg, S. (2001). Understanding figurative language. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S. (2002). Emotion language: A new synthesis?Contemporary Psychology, 47, 764–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Brown, M., & McGlone, M. (1993). Conceptual metaphors are not automatically accessed during idiom comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 21, 711–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S., & Keysar, B. (1990). Understanding metaphorical comparisons: Beyond similarity. Psychological Review, 97, 3–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goff, L., & Roediger, H. (1998). Imagination inflation for action events: Repeated imaginings leads to illusory recollection. Memory & Cognition, 26, 20–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1976). Response cries. Language, 54, 787–815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldap, C. (1992). Morphology and semantics of Yucatec space relators. Zeitschrift fur Phonetik, Sprachwissenschaft und Kommunikationsforschung, 45, 612–25.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldfield, E. (1993). Dynamic systems in development: Action systems. In , L. Smith & , E. Thelen (Eds.), A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications (pp. 51–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goldie, P. (2000). Explaining expressions of emotion. Mind, 109, 25–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, S. (1995). Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review, 105, 251–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. (1970). A theory of action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Goodale, M., & Humphrey, G. (1998). The objects of action and perception. Cognition, 67, 181–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodale, M., & Murphy, K. (2000). Space and the brain: Different neural substrates for allocentric and egocentric forms of reference. In , T. Metzinger (ed.), Neural correlates of consciousness (pp. 189–202). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational organization: Interaction between speakers and hearers. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goossens, L., Pauwels, B., Rudzka-Ostyn, M., Simon-Venderberger, J., & Varpays, J. 1995. By word of mouth: Metaphor, metonymy, and linguistic action in a cognitive perspective. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorfein, D. (Ed.) (2001). On the consequences of meaning selection: Principles of resolving lexical ambiguity. Washington, DC: APA Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, F. (2002). On the epigenetic evolution of species-specific perception: The developmental manifold concepts. Cognitive Development, 17, 1287–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouin-Decarie, T. (1969). A study of the mental and emotional development of the thalidomide child. In , B. Foss (Ed.). Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 167–187). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Grady, J. (1997). Theories are buildings revisited. Cognitive Linguistics, 8, 267–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grady, J. (1999). A typology of motivation for conceptual metaphor: Correlation vs. resemblance. In , R. Gibbs & , G. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 79–100). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Graesser, A., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graesser, A., Woll, S., Kowalski, D., & Smith, D. (1980). Memory for typical and atypical actions in scripted activities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 503–15.Google Scholar
Grafton, S., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuroimage, 6, 231–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granott, N., & Paziale, J. (Eds.) (2002). Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. (2001). Understanding microworlds. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 879–911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. (1978). Universals of human language: Vol. 3. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Greene, R., & Samuel, A. (1986). Recency and suffix effects in serial recall of musical stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 12, 517–24.Google ScholarPubMed
Gregory, R., & Wallace, J. (1963). Recovery from early blindness: A case study. Monograph No. 2. Experimental Psychology Society.
Grezes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 1–19.3.0.CO;2-V>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Science, 27, 377–396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadamard, J. (1945). The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hadar, U. (1989). Two types of gesture and their role in speech production. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 8, 221–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadar, U., Wenkert-Olenik, D., Krauss, R., & Soroker, N. (1998). Gesture and the pro-cessing of speech: Neuropsychological evidence. Brain & Language, 62, 107–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haith, M. (1997). The development of future thinking as essential for the emergence of skill in planning. In S. Friedman & Scholnick (Eds.), The developmental psychology of planning: Why, how, and when do we plan? (pp. 25–42). Mahwah, NJ Erlbaum.
Halff, H., Ortony, A., & Anderson, R. (1976). A context-sensitive representation of word meaning. Menory & Cognition, 4, 378–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, C., Bernoties, L., & Schmidt, D. (1995). Interference effects of mental imagery on a motor task. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 181–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, E., & Cairns, H. (1961). Plato: The collected dialogues. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hanna, E., & Meltzoff, A. (1993). Peer imitation by toddlers in laboratory and daycare contexts: Implications for social learning and memory. Developmental Psychology, 29, 701–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanrahan, C., Tetreau, B., & Sarrazin, C. (1995). Use of imagery while performing dance movement. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 413–30.Google Scholar
Hardy, L., & Callow, N. (1999). Efficacy of external and internal visual imagery perspectives for the enhancement of performances on tasks in which form is important. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 95–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harker, J., & Wierzbicka, A. (Eds.) (2001). Emotion in crosslinguistic perspective. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Harman, K., Humphrey, G., & Goodale, M. (1999). Active manual control of object views facilitate object recognition. Current Biology, 9, 1315–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatano, G., & Osawa, K. (1983). Digit memory of grand experts in abacus-derived mental calculation. Cognition, 5, 47–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapsom, R. (1992). Primitive emotional contagion. In , M. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Vol. 14. Emotion and social behavior (pp. 151–77). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hatsopoulos, N., & Warren, W. (1991). Resonance tuning in rhythmic arm movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 28, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healey, A. (1982). Short-term memory for order information. In , G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 16 (pp. 191–238). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hecht, H., Vogt, S., & Prinz, W. (2001). Motor learning enhances perceptual judgment: A case for action-perception transfer. Psychological Research, 65, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Heine, B. (1989). Adpositions in African languages. Linguistique Africaine, 2, 77–127.Google Scholar
Heine, B. (1997). Cognitive foundations of grammar. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heine, B., Ulrike, C., & Hunnemeyer, F. (1991). Grammaticalization: A conceptual framework. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Heller, J. (1961). Catch-22. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Hemingway, E. (1960). The collected poems of Ernest Hemingway. San Francisco: Pirated Edition.Google Scholar
Henderson, W., & Smyth, G. (1948). Phantom limbs. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, Psychiatry, 11, 88–117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heptulla-Chatterjee, S., Freyd, J., & Shiffrar, M. (1996). Configural processing in the perception of apparent biological motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 916–29.Google Scholar
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children. Nature, 370, 57–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertenstein, M. (2002). Touch: Its communicative function in infancy. Human Development, 45, 70–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewes, G. (1983). The invention of phonemically-based language. In Grolier, E. (Ed.), Glossogenetics: The origins and evolution of language (pp. 143–62). Paris: Harwood Publishers.Google Scholar
Heywood, C., & Kentridge, R. (2000). Affective blindsight?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 125–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohmann, G. (1966). Some effects of spinal cord lesions on experienced emotional feelings. Psychophysiology, 3, 143–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hommel, B. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 764–75.Google Scholar
Hommel, B. (1996). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 546–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornstein, S., & Mulligan, N. (2001). Memory of action events: The role of objects in memory of self- and other-performed tasks. American Journal of Psychology, 114, 199–217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, D. (2003). Sensual relations: Engaging the senses in culture and social theory. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubbard, T. (1990). Cognitive representation of linear motion: Possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement. Memory & Cognition, 18, 299–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, T. (1995). Cognitive representations of motion: Evidence for friction and gravity analogues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 241–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, T. (1996). Representational momentum, centripetal force, and curvilinear impetus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1049–62.Google ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, T. (1999). How consequences of physical properties influence mental representation: The environmental invariants hypothesis. In , P. Killeen & , W. Uttal (Eds.), Fechner Day 99: The end of 20th-century psychophysics, Proceedings of the 15th annual meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 274–9). Tempe, AZ: International Society for Psychophysics.Google Scholar
Hubbard, T., & Bharacha, J. (1988). Judged displaced in apparent vertical and horizontal motion. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 211–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1974). Vision in a monkey without striate cortex: A case study. Perception, 3, 241–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hupka, R., Zaleski, Z., Otto, J., Reidl, L., Tarabrina, N. (1996). Anger, envy, fear, and jealousy as felt in the body: A five-nation study. Cross-Cultural Research, 30, 243–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Husserl, E. (1977). Phenomenological psychology. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Husserl, E. (1980). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. Boston: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutchinson, W., Davis, K., Lozano, A, Tasker, R., & Dostrovsky, J. (1999). Pain-related neurons in the human cingulated cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 403–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R., & Mazziotta, J. (1998). Brain-behavior relationships: Evidence from practice effects or spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Journal of Neurophysiology, 76, 321–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Intos-Peterson, M., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, B. (1987). Sensory-perceptual qualities of images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 15, 188–99.Google Scholar
Ito, M. (1993). Movement and thought: Identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum. Trends in Neuroscience, 16, 448–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ito, M. (1999). Imagined movement and response programming. Journal of Mental Imagery, 23, 71–84.Google Scholar
Iverson, J., & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth, and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11–12, 19–40.Google Scholar
Ivry, R., & Fiez, J. (2000). Cerebellum contributions to cognition and imagery. In , M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neuroscience (pp. 999–1011). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R., & Aaron, D. (1991). Review of G. Lakoff & M. Turner, More than cool reason: A filed guide to poetic metaphor. Language, 67, 320–38.Google Scholar
Jackson, F. (1982). Epiphenomenal qualia. Philosophical Quarterly, 32, 127–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, F. (1986). What Mary didn't know. Journal of Philosophy, 83, 291–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, J. (1994). Chronic pain and the tension between the body as subject and object. In , T. Csordas (Ed.), Embodiment and experience (pp. 201–28). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jacobson, E. (1932). Electrophysiology of mental activities. American Journal of Psychology, 44, 677–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, K., Humphrey, K., & Goodale, M. (2001). Manipulating and recognizing virtual objects: Where the action is. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 111–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
James, W. (1892). Psychology: Briefer course. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1895). The knowing of things together. Psychological Review, 2, 105–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvella, R., & Collas, J. (1974). Memory for the intentions of sentences. Memory & Cognition, 2, 185–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behaviorial and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeannerod, M. (1995). Mental imagery in the motor cortex. Neuropsychologica, 33, 1419–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeannerod, M. (1999). To act or not to act: Perspectives in the representation of action. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 52, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1991). Knowing through the body. Philosophical Psychology, 4, 3–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. (1993). Conceptual metaphor and embodied structures of meaning. Philosophical Psychology, 6, 413–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. (2000). Thinking ahead: The case for motor imagery in prospective judgments of prehension. Cognition, 74, 33–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. (1995). Infants' detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaczmarek, K., & Bach-y-Rita, P. (1995). Tactile displays. In , W. Barfield & Furness, T. (Eds.), Virtual environments and advanced interface design (pp. 349–414). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kaiser, M., & Proffitt, D. (1987). Observers' sensitivity to dynamic anomalies in collision. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 275–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalnins, I., & Bruner, J. (1973). The coordination of visual observation and instrumental behavior in early infancy. Perception, 2, 307–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandel, S., Orliaguet, J.-P., & Viviani, P. (2000). Perceptual anticipation in handwriting: The role of implicit motor competence. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 706–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kant, I., (1787/1927). Immanuel Kant's Critique of pure reason : in commemoration of the centenary of its first publication. New York: MacMillan.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kaschak, M., & Glenberg, A. (2000). Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 508–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., & Fillmore, C. (1999). Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The “what's X doing Y?” construction. Language, 75, 1–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keefe, D., & McDaniel, M. (1993). The time course and durability of predictive inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 446–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keen, R. (2003). Representation of objects and events: Why do infants look so smart and toddlers look so dumb?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 79–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, M., & Freyd, J. (1987). Explorations of representational momentum. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 369–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelso, J. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 740–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, J., Gabia, P., & Nicholls, A. (1991). Tactile pictures. In , M. Heller & , W. Schift (Eds.), The psychology of touch (pp. 263–99). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kermoian, R., & Campos, J. (1988). Locomotor experience: A facilitator of spatial cognitive development. Child Development, 59, 908–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, N. (1983). The role of vision in “visual imagery” experiments: Evidence from the congenitally blind. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 265–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, D. (1973). The asymmetry of the human brain. Scientific American, 228, 70–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirsh, D. (1995). The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence, 73, 31–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatt, D. (1989). Review of selected models of speech perception. In , W. Marseln-Wilson (Ed.), Lexical representations and processes (pp. 169–226). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Klatzky, R. (1994). On the relation between motor imagery and visual imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 212–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, R., Lederman, S., & Metzger, V. (1985). Identifying objects by touch: An expert system. Perception and Psychophysics, 37, 299–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, R., Loomis, J., Lederman, S., & Wake, H. (1993). Haptic identification of objects and their depictions. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 170–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klatzky, R., Pellegrino, J., McCloskey, B., & Doherty, S. (1989). Can you squeeze a tomato?: The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgments. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 56–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinman, A. (1982). Neurasthenia and depression: A study of somatization and culture in China. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 6, 117–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., & Flach, R. (2001). Predicting the effects of action: Interaction of perception and action. Psychological Science, 12, 467–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., Seigerschmidt, E., Flach, R., & Prinz, W. (2002). Authorship effects in the production of handwriting strokes: Evidence for action simulation during action perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1027–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knuf, L., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). An analysis of ideomotor action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 779–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koivisto-Alanko, P. (1998). Mechanisms of semantic change in nouns of cognition: A general model. In , J. Coleman & , C. Kay (Eds.), Lexicology, semantics and lexicography (pp. 35–54). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Koleck, M., Bruchon-Schweitzer, M., Cousson-Gelie, F., Gillard, J., & Quintard, B. (2002). The body-image questionnaire: An extension. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 189–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolstad, V. (1991). Understanding of containment in 5.5 month-old infants. Poster presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle: Washington.Google Scholar
Kopp, C., & Shaperman, J. (1973). Cognitive development in the absence of object manipulation during infancy. Developmental Psychology, 9, 430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koslowski, L., & Cutting, J. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 575–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemisphere: A computational approach. Psychological Review, 94, 148–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. (1994). Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kosslyn, S., Cave, M., Provost, D., & Gierke, S. (1988). Sequential processes in image generation. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 319–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S., DiGirolamo, G., & Thompson, W. (1998). Mental rotation of object versus hands: Neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology, 35, 151–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S., Thompson, W., Wraga, M., & Alpert, N. (2001). Imagining rotation by endogenous versus exogenuous forces: Distinct neural mechanisms. Neuroreport, 12, 2519–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotchoubey, B. (2001). About ham and eggs – perception and action, ecology, and neuroscience: A reply to Michaels (2000). Ecological Psychology, 13, 123–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Activation in human MT/MST by static images with implied motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 48–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kourtzi, Z., & Shiffrar, M. (1997). One-shot view invariance in a moving world. Psychological Science, 8, 461–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kourtzi, Z., & Shiffrar, M. (1999). Dynamic representations of human body movement. Perception, 28, 49–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovecses, Z. (2000a). Metaphor and emotion: Language, culture and body in human feeling. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kovecses, Z. (2000b). Force and emotion. In , L. Albertazzi (Ed.), Meaning and cognition (pp. 145–68). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak?Current Direction in Psychological Science, 7, 54–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R., & Hadar, U. (1999). The role of speech-related arm/hand gestures in word retrieval. In , L. Messing & , R. Campbell (Eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign (pp. 93–116). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kraut, R., Fussell, S., & Siegel, J. (2003). Visual information as a conversational resource in collaborative physical tasks. Human-Computer Interaction, 18, 13–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraut, R., & Johnston, R. (1979). Social and emotional messages of smiling: An ethological approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1539–1553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krist, H., Fieberg, E., & Wilkening, F. (1993). Intuitive physics in action and judgment: The development of knowledge about projectile motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 952–966.Google Scholar
Kugler, P., & Turvey, M. (1987). Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P., & Miller, J. (1975). Speech perception by the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants. Science, 190, 69–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P., & Meltzoff, A. (1987). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuzouka, H., Oyama, S., Yamazaki, K., Suzuki, K., & Mitsuishi, M. (2000). GestureMan: A mobile robot that embodies a remote instructor's actions. Proceedings of the CSCW 2000 (pp. 155–62). New York: ACM.
Lachs, K., & Pisoni, D. (2004). Specification of cross-modal source information in isolated kinematic displays of speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116, 507–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laeng, B., & Teodorescu, D-S. (2002). Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenacts those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science, 26, 207–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What our categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1990). The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reasoning based on image-schemas?Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 39–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In , A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 202–251). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Nunez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lambek, M., & Strathern, A. (Eds.) (1998). Bodies and persons: Comparative perspectives from Africa and Melanesia. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambie, J., & Marcel, A. (2002). Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: A theoretical framework. Psychological Review, 109, 219–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landau, J., Libkuman, T., & Wildman, J. (2002). Mental simulation inflates performance estimates for physical abilities. Memory & Cognition, 30, 372–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. (1995). The emotion probe: Structure of motivation and attention. American Psychologist, 50, 372–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. (1991). Image, word, and symbol. Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Larsen, R., Kasimatis, M., & Frey, K. (1992). Facilitating the furrowed brow: An unobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis applied to unpleasant affect. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 321–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leander, K. (2002). Silencing in classroom interaction: Drawing and relating social space. Discourse Processes, 34, 193–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leder, D. (1990). The absent body. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lederman, S., & Klatzky, R. (1990). Haptic exploration and object representation. In , M. Goodale (Ed.), Vision and action: The control of grasping (pp. 98–109). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Lederman, S., & Klatzky, R. (2003). Feelng surfaces and objects remotely. In , R. Nelson (Ed.), The somatosensory system: Deciphering the brain's own body image (pp. 103–20). New York: C Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (1998). The emotional brain. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11, 173–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. (1988). The necessity of illusion: Perception and thought in infancy. In , L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought without language (pp. 185–210). Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. (1994). ToMM, ToBY, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In , L. Hirschfeld & , S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind (pp. 119–48). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, A., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality?Cognition, 25, 265–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, R., Ekman, P., Heider, K., & Friesen, W. (1992). Emotion and autonomic nervous system activity in the Minangkabau of West Sumatra. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 62, 972–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, D., & Simons, D. (1997). Failure to detect changes to attended objects in motion pictures. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 501–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levins, J., & Lewontin, R. (1985). The dialectical biologist. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. (1994). Vision, shape, and linguistic description: Tzeltal body-part terminology and object description. Linguistics, 32, 791–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew, A., & Butterworth, G. (1997). The development of hand-mouth coordination in 2- to 5-month-old infants: Similarities with reaching and grasping. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 59–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewkowicz, D. (1996). Perception of auditory -visual temporal synchrony in human infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1094–106.Google ScholarPubMed
Leyton, M. (1992). Symmetry, causality, mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Li, L., & Warren, W. (2002). Retinal flow is sufficient for steering during observation. Psychological Science, 13, 485–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libby, L. (2003). Imagining perspective and source monitoring in imagination inflation. Memory & Cognition, 31, 1072–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A. (1970). The grammars of speech and language. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 301–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A. (1991). Speech: A special code. In , A. Liberman (Ed.), Learning, development and conceptual change (pp. 121–145). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Liberman, A., Cooper, F., Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A., & Mattingly, I. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revisited. Cognition, 21, 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A., & Mattingly, I. (1989). A specialization for speech perception. Science, 243, 489–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A., & Whalen, D. (2000). On the relation of speech to language. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 187–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liljedahl, P. (2001). Embodied experience of velocity and acceleration: A narrative. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20, 439–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, S., Sinha, C., & Freeman, N. (1981). Spatial references systems and the canonicality effect in infant search. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logie, R. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Logie, R., & Marchetti, C. (1991). Visuo-spatial working memory: Visual, spatial, or central executive? In , R. Logie & , M. Denis (Eds.), Mental images in human cognition (pp. 105–15). Oxford: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Logie, R., & Pearson, D. (1997). The inner eye and the inner scribe of visuo-spatial working memory: Evidence for developmental fractionation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 241–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loring, D., Meador, K., Allison, J., & Wright, J. (2000). Relationship between motion and linguistic activation using fMRI. Neurology, 54, 981–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Low, S. (1994). Embodied metaphors: Nerves as lived experience. In , T. Csordas (Ed.), Embodiment and experience (pp. 139–62). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Luff, P., Heath, C., Kuzuoka, Hi., Hindmarsh, J., Yamazaki, K., & Oyama, S. (2003). Fractured ecologies: Creating environments for collaboration. Human-Computer Interaction, 18, 51–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maalej, Z. (2004). Figurative language in anger expressions in Tunisian Arabic: An extended view of embodiment. Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 51–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. (1975). Preliminaries to the study of single motor unit activity in speech musculature. Journal of Phonetics, 1, 55–71.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1998). The emergence of language from embodiment. In , B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 213–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Manaster, G., Cleland, C., & Brooks, J. (1978). Emotions as movement in relation to others. Journal of Individual Psychology, 34, 244–53.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and body: Psychology of emotion and stress. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. (1992). How to build a baby – 2. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. (1998). Babies think before they speak. Human Development, 41, 116–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thoughts. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, J., Bauer, P., & McDonough, L. (1991). Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 263–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manusov, V., & Rodriguez, M. (1989). Intentionally based nonverbal messages: A perceiver's perspective. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 15–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, A., & Deitrich, E. (2000). In defense of representation. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 135–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, D. (1999). Consciousness, mental imagery, and action. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 567–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marmor, G., & Zaback, L. (1976). Mental rotation by the blind: Does mental rotation depend on visual imagery?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 515–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Marrone, R. (1990). Body of knowledge: An introduction to body/mind psychology. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Marschark, M. (1994). Gesture and sign. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 209–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, A., Ungerleider, L., & Haxby, J. (2000). Category specificity and the brain: The sensory-motor model of semantic representation of objects. In , M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd edition) (pp. 1023–36). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Martin, A., Wiggs, C., Ungerleider, L., & Haxby, J. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, E. (1994). Flexible bodies: Tracking immunity in American culture from the days of polio to the age of AIDS. Boston: Beacon.Google Scholar
Massaro, D. (1987). Speech perception by ear and by eye: A paradigm for psychological inquiry. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Matisoff, J. (1978). Variational semantics in Tibeto-Burman: The organic approach to linguistic comparison. Occasional paper of the Wolfenden Society. Philadelphia: Institute for the Study of Human Issues.Google Scholar
Matlock, T. (2004). Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1389–1400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maturana, H. (1980). Biology of cognition. In , H. Maturana & , F. Varela (Eds.), Autopoiesis and cognition: The realization of the living (pp. 5–58). Boston: Reidel.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. (1983). What it is to see?Archivos de Biologica y Medicina Experimentales, 16, 255–69.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R., & Jacques, J. (2000). Gesture production during stuttered speech: Insights into the nature of gesture-speech integration. In , D. McNeil (Ed.), Language and gestures (pp. 199–214). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCloskey, M., & Kohl, D. (1983). Naive physics: The curvilinear impetus principle and its role in interactions with moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 9, 146–56.Google ScholarPubMed
McDonnell, P. (1988). Developmental responses to limb deficiencies and limb replacement. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 42, 120–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGlone, M., & Harding, J. (1998). Back (or forward) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1211–23.Google Scholar
McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeil, D. (1992). Hand and gesture: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Medin, D., Lynch, E., Coley, J., & Atran, S. (1997). Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: Do all roads lead to Rome?Cognitive Psychology, 32, 49–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, B., & Robinson, M. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15, 243–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. (1990). Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modeling, and self practice in infancy. In , D. Cicchetti & , M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition (pp. 139–64). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Borton, R. (1979). Intermodal matching by human noenates. Nature, 282, 403–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A., & Brooks, R. (2001). “Like me” as building block for understanding other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. In , B. Malle, , L. Moses, & , D. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 171–91). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 479–505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 83–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–92.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (2000). (a) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates (b) Resolving the debate about early imitation. In , D. Muir & , A. Slater (Eds.), Infant development: The essential readings. Essential readings in development psychology (pp. 167–81). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (2001). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates: Resolving the debate about early imitation. In , D. Muir & , A. Slater (Ed.), Infant development: The essential readings (pp. 167–81). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Merzenich, M., Kaas, J., Wall, J., Nelson, R., Sur, M., & Felleman, D. (1983). Topographic reorganization of somatosensory cortical Area 3B and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted deafferentation. Neuroscience, 8, 33–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metzinger, T. (Ed.) (2000). Neural correlation of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Michel, G., Camras, L., & Sullivan, J. (1992). Infant interest expressions as coordinative motor structures. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 347–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Miller, J., & Stigler, J. (1991). Meanings of skill: Effect of abacus expertise in number representation. Cognition & Instruction, 8, 29–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, D., & Dyde, R. (2003). Why do some perceptual illusions affect visually guided action, when others don't?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 10–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, D., & Goodale, M. (1995). The visual brain in action. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R., & Gallaher, M. (2001). Embodying music: Matching music and dance as movement. Music Perception, 19, 65–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Möller, R. (1999). Perception through anticipation: A behavior-based approach to visual perception. In , A. Riegler, , M., Peschl, & , A. von Stein (Eds.), Understanding representation in the cognitive sciences (pp. 169–76). New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Montessori, M. (1914). Dr. Montessori's own handbook. London: Heineman.Google Scholar
Montpare, J., Goldstein, S., & Clausen, A. (1987). The identification of emotions from gait information. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 1, 33–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, D., Bower, G., & Greenspan, S. (1989). Updating situation models during narrative comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 28, 292–312.Google Scholar
Mulligan, N., & Hornstein, S. (2003). Memory for actions: Self-performed tasks and the reenactment effect. Memory & Cognition, 31, 412–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munakata, Y., McClelland, J., Johnson, M., & Siegler, R. (1997). Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive process account of success and failure on object permanence. Psychological Review, 104, 686–713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, G. (1996). On metaphoric representations. Cognition, 60, 173–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, G. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, S. (1990). Models of imagery in sports: A review. Journal of Mental Imagery, 89, 216–23.Google Scholar
Murray, C. (2001). The experience of body boundaries by Siamese twins. New Ideas in Psychology, 19, 117–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, I., & Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant's role in mother-infant communication. Journal of Child Language, 13, 15–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, J., Klin, C., & Myers, J. (1993). Forward inference in narrative text. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 464–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nairne, J., & Walters, V. (1983). Silent mouthing produces modality and suffix-like effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 475–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, R., & Mishkin, M. (1980). Chronic blindness following non-visual cortical lesions. Brain Research, 188, 572–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narayanan, S. (1997). Moving right along: A computational model of metaphoric reasoning about events. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, International Computer Science Institute, University of California, Berkeley.
Needham, A. (2001). Object recognition and object segregation in 4.5 month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 3–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Needham, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1993). Intuitions about support in 4.5-month-olds. Cognition, 47, 121–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Needham, A., Barrett, T., & Peterman, K. (2002). A pick-me-up for infants' exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using ‘sticky mittens’ enhances young infants' object exploration skills. Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 279–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1993). The self perceived. In , U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 3–21). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., Skwerer, D., Goldman, S., Henseler, S., Presler, N., & Walkenfeld, F. (2003). Entering a community of minds: An experientialist approach to a theory of mind. Human Development, 46, 24–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neruda, P. (1972). The captain's verses. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Neumann, C. (2001). Is metaphor universal? Cross-linguistic evidence from German and Japanese. Metaphor and Symbol, 16, 123–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newcombe, N. (2002). The nativist-empiricist controversy in the context of recent research in spatial and quantitative development. Psychological Science, 13, 395–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newton, N. (1996). Foundations of understanding. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newton, N. (2000). Conscious emotion in a dynamic system: How I can know how I feel. In , R. Ellis & , N. Newton (Eds.), Caldrons of consciousness (pp. 91–108). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Nicolelis, M., & Fanselow, E. (2002). Thalamocortical optimization of tactile processing according to behavioral state. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 517–523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, T. (1963). Volition: A new experimental approach. Scandanavian Journal of Psychology, 4, 225–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nieuwenhuyse, B., Offenberg, L., & Frijda, N. (1987). Subjective emotion and reported body experience. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 169–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noe, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noe, A., & O'Reagan, K. (2002). On the brain-basis of visual consciousness: A sensorimotor account. In , A. Noe & , E. Thompson (Eds.), Vision and mind (pp. 567–98). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noe, A., & Thompson, E. (2004). Are there neural correlates of consciousness?Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11, 3–28.Google Scholar
Nolfi, S., & Floreano, D. (2000). Evolutionary robotics: The biology, intelligence, and technology of self-organizing machines. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nyberg, L., Habib, R., McIntosh, A., & Tulving, E. (2000). Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 11, 120–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nygaard, L., Sommers, M., Mitchell, S., & Pisoni, D. (1994). Speech perception as talker-contingent process. Psychological Science, 5, 42–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oatley, K. (1992). The best laid schemes: The psychology of emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
O'Brien, E., & Albrecht, J. (1992). Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 777–84.Google ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E., Jacoby, S., & Gonzales, P. (1994). Interpretive journeys: How physicists talk and travel through graphic space. Configurations, 2, 157–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odling-Smee, F. (1988). Niche-constructing phenotypes. In , H. Plotkin (Ed.), The role of behavior in evolution (pp. 73–132). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ojemann, G. (1994). Cortical stimulation and recording in language. In , A. Kertesz (Ed.). Localization and Neuroimaging in Neuropsychology (pp. 35–55). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ojemann, G., & Mateer, C. (1979). Human language cortex: Localization of memory, syntax, and sequential motor-phoneme identification systems. Science, 205, 1401–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, E. (2003). Personal identity. In , S. Stich & , T. Warfield (Eds.), The Blackwell guide to philosophy of mind (pp. 352–68). New York: Blackwell.Google Scholar
O'Regan, K. (1992). Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46, 461–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Regan, K., & Noe, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 939–1031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Regan, K., Resnick, R., & Clark, J. (1997). Picture changes during blinks: Not seeing where you look and seeing where you don't look. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 38, S707.Google Scholar
Oudejams, R., Michaels, C., Bakker, F., & Dolne, M. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of the catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 879–91.Google Scholar
Paillard, J. (1987). Cognitive versus sensorimotor encoding of spatial information. In , P. Ellen & , C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animals and man (pp. 35–54). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual-coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pandya, V. (1993). Above the forest: A study of Andamanese ethnoemology, cosmology, and the power of ritual. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Papez, J. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 38, 725–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1995). Ideas and realities of emotion. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Parsons, L. (1987a). Imagined spatial transformations of one's body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 172–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L. (1987b). Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 178–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L. (1994). Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 709–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Parsons, L., & Fox, P. (1998). The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 583–615.Google Scholar
Parsons, L., Fox, P., Downs, J., Glass, T., Hirsch, T., Martin, C., Jerabek, P., & Lancaster, J. (1995). Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET. Nature, 375, 54–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlenko, A. (2002). Emotions and the body in Russian and English. Pragmatics & Cognition, 10, 207–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazzani, M. (1997). Influence of prior knowledge on concept acquisition: Experimental and computational results. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 416–32.Google Scholar
Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. (2003). Verifying different modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science, 14, 119–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Raaijmakers, J. (1998). Does pizza prime coin? Perceptual processing in lexical decision and pronunciation. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 407–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennybaker, J. (1982). The psychology of physical symptoms. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philippot, P., Rime, B. (1997). The perception of bodily sensations during emotion: A cross-cultural perspective. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 28, 175–88.Google Scholar
Phillips, R., Wagner, S., Fell, C., & Lynch, M. (1990). Do infants recognize emotion in facial expressions? Categorical and metaphorical evidence. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 71–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicaps. Development & Psychopathology, 4, 375–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in childhood. New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1975). The equilibrium of cognitive structures. Paris: Presses Universitairse de France.Google Scholar
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Pine, K., & Messer, D. (2003). The development of representations as children learn about balancing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 285–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. (2002). The past and future of past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plaut, D. (1995). Double-dissociation without modularity: Evidence from connectionist neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 291–321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollio, H., Henley, T., & Thompson, C. (1997). The phenomenology of everyday life. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, R., & Gelder, T (Eds.) (1995). Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Premack, D. (1990). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Presson, C., & Montello, D. (1994). Updating of rotational and translational body movements: Coordinate structures of perceptual spaces. Perception, 23, 1447–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, S., & Waal, F. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–25.Google ScholarPubMed
Pribram, K. (1991). Brain and perception: Holonomy and studies in figural processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. (2002). Furnishing the mind: Concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Prinz, J., & Barsalou, L. (2000). Steering a course for embodied representations. In , E. Deitrich & , A, Markman (Eds.), Cognitive dynamics: Conceptual and representational changes in humans and machines (pp. 51–78). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Proffitt, D., Creem, S., & Zosh, W. (2001). Seeing mountains in mole hills: Geographical-slant perception. Psychological Science, 12, 418–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermueller, F. (1999). Words in the brain's language. Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 22, 253–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975). Mind, language, and reality: Philosophical papers, Vol. 2. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, J. (1994). Toward a clarification of spatial processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 465–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, N. (1991). The cultural basis of metaphor. In , J. Fernandez (Ed.), Beyond metaphor: The theory of tropes in anthropology (pp. 56–93). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Radcliff-Brown, A. (1964). The Andaman islanders. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V., & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the brain. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V., & Hirstein, W. (1997). Three laws of qualia: What neurology tells us about the biological functions of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4, 429–57.Google Scholar
Redding, G., & Wallace, B. (1997). Adaptive spatial alignment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reed, C., & Farah, M. (1995). The psychological reality of the body schema: A test with normal participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 334–43.Google ScholarPubMed
Regier, T. (1996). The human semantic potential. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Reisberg, D., Rappaport, I., & O'Shaughnessy, M. (1984). Limits of working memory: The digit span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 203–21.Google Scholar
Reiser, J., Doxey, P., McCarrell, N., & Brooks, P. (1982). Wayfinding and toddlers' use of information from an aerial view of a maze. Developmental Psychology, 18, 714–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiser, J., Lockman, J., & Pick, H. (1980). The role of visual experience in knowledge of spatial layout. Perception and Psychophysics, 28, 185–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiser, J., & Rider, E. (1991). Young children's spatial orientation with respect to multiple targets when walking without vision. Developmental Psychology, 27, 97–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Remez, R., Fellowes, J., & Rubin, P. (1997). Talker identification based on phonetic information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 651–661.Google ScholarPubMed
Repp, B. (1998). Musical motion in perception and performance. In , D. Rosenbaum & , C. Collyer (Eds.), Timing of behavior: Neural, psychological, and computational perspectives (pp. 125–41). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Resnick, A., O'Regan, K., & Clark, J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368–73.Google Scholar
Richardson, D., & Spivey, M. (2000). Representation, space, and Hollywood Squares: Looking at things that aren't there anymore. Cognition, 76, 269–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, D., Spivey, M., Barsalou, L., & McRae, K. (2003). Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science, 27, 767–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rieser, J., & Rider, E. (1991). Young children's spatial orientation with respect to multiple targets while walking without vision. Developmental Psychology, 27, 97–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rime, B., Philippot, P., & Cisamolo, D. (1990). Social schemata of peripheral changes in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 38–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G. (1994). Nonconscious motor images. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 188–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1997). Parietal cortex: From sight to action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 562–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B. (1994). Space and selective attention. In , C. Umilta & , M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 232–65). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rochat, P. (1989). Object manipulation and exploration in 2- to 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 28, 871–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochat, P. (2001). The infant's world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 532–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E. (1999). Reclaiming concepts. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 61–77.Google Scholar
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C., Gray, W., Johnson, M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseblad, B., & Hofsten, C. (1994). Repetitive goal-directed arm movements in children with development coordination: Role of visual information. Adapted Physical Activity, 11, 190–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenbaum, D. (1991). Human motor control. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rosler, F., Heil, M., & Hennighausen, E. (1995). Distinct cortical activation patterns during long-term memory retrieval of verbal, spatial, and color information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 51–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, B. (1990). Reminding-based category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 460–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, B. (1999). Postclassification category use: The effects of learning to use categories after learning to classify. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 743–57.Google Scholar
Ross, B., Perkins, S., & Tenpenny, P. (1990). Reminding-based category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 460–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, E., & Mesulam, M-M. (1979). Damaged language functions of the right-hemisphere for prosody and emotional feeling. Archives of Neurology, 36, 144–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossi, S., Tecchio, F., Pasqualetti, P., Ulivelli, M., Pizzella, V., Romani, G., Passero, S., Battistini, N., & Rossini, P. (2002). Somatosensory processing during movement observation in humans. Clincal Neurophysiology, 113, 16–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotenberg, K., & Sullivan, C. (2003). Children's use of gaze and limb movement cues to infer deception. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 175–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth, E., & Shoben, E. (1983). The effect of context on the structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 346–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, G. (1987). Understanding order of movement in youngsters with cerebral palsy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 65, 391–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovee-Collier, C., & Hayne, H. (2000). Memory in infancy and early childhood. In , E. Tulving & , F. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 267–82). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. (1980). Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. In , R. Spiro, , B. Bruce, & , W. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 33–58). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D., & McClelland, J. (1986). (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Volume 1 – Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Runson, S., & Frykolm, G. (1981). Visual perception of lifted weights. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 733–40.Google Scholar
Russell, J. (1995). Facial expressions of emotion: What lies beyond minimal universality?Psychological Bulletin, 118, 379–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salway, A., & Logie, R. (1995). Visuospatial working memory, movement control, and executive demands. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 253–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sartre, J.-P. (1956). Being and nothingness. New York: Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schank, R. (1982). Dynamic memory. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1976). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scheper-Hughs, N., & Lock, M. (1987). The mindful body: A prolegomenon to future work in medical anthropology. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 1, 6–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K., & Wallbott, H. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schladt, M. (1997). Kognitive Strukturen von Korerteilvokabularien in kenianischen Sprachen. Cologne: Institut fur Afrikanistik, University of Cologne.Google Scholar
Schreuder, R., Flores D'Arcais, G., & Glazenborg, G. (1984). Effects of perceptual and conceptual similarity in semantic priming. Psychological Research, 45, 339–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, D. (1999). Physical imagery: Kinematic versus dynamical models. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 433–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, D., & Black, T. (1996). Analog imagery in mental model reasoning: Depictive models. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 154–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, D., & Black, T. (1999). Inferences through imagined actions: Knowing by simulated doing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 116–36.Google Scholar
Schwartz, D., & Holton, D. (2000). Tool use and the effect of action on the imagination. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1655–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, C., Harris, R., & Rothe, A. (2001). Embodied cognition through improvisation improves memory for dramatic monologue. Discourse Processes, 31, 293–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scruton, R. (1997). The aesthetics of music. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Segal, S., & Fusella, S. (1970). Influences of imaged pictures and sounds on detection of visual and auditory signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 458–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segrin, C. (1998). Interpersonal communication patterns associated with depression and loneliness. In , P. Anderson & , L. Guerrero (Eds.), Handbook of communication and emotion (pp. 215–42). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Seifert, C., Robertson, S., & Black, J. (1985). Types of inferences generated during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 405–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shannon, B. (1997). What is the functions of consciousness?Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 295–308.Google Scholar
Shannon, B. (2002). The antipodes of the mind: Charting the phenomenology of the Ayahuasca experience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sharkey, N., & Sharkey, A. (1987). What is the point of integration? The loci of knowledge-based facilitation in sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 255–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharkey, N., & Ziemke, T. (1998). A consideration of the biological and psychological foundations of autonomous robotics. Connection Science, 10, 361–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, R. (2001). Processes, acts, and experiences: Three stances on the problem of intentionality. Ecological Psychology, 13, 636–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, R., & Turvey, M. (1999). Ecological foundations of cognition I: Degrees of freedom and conserved quantities in animal-environment systems. In , R. Nunez & , W. Freeman (Eds.), Reclaiming cognition (pp. 111–23). Bowling Green, OH: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Shebilske, W. (1977). Visuomotor coordination in visual direction and position constancies. In , W. Epstein (Ed.), Stability and constancy in visual perception: Mechanisms and processes (pp. 89–112). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1998). Consciousness: A natural history. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 260–94.Google Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1999). The primacy of movement. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representations: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review, 91, 417–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherrington, C. (1906). The integrative action of the nervous system. New York: C. Scribner's Sons.Google Scholar
Shiffrar, M., & Pinto, J. (2002). The visual analysis of bodily motion. In , W. Prinz and , B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 381–400). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shirouzu, H., Miyake, N., & Masukawa, H. (2002). Cognitively active externalization for situated reflection. Cognitive Science, 26, 469–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shontz, F. (1969). Perceptual and cognitive aspects of bodily experience. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shore, P., & Repp, B. (1995). Musical motion and performance: Theoretical and empirical perspectives. In , J. Link (Ed.), The practice of performance (pp. 55–83). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Explorations in cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Simon, J. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, J., & Ruddell, A. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 433–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, D., & Chabris, C. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, H. (1971). Sensorimotor activity patterns as a condition for the acquisition of syntax. In , R. Huxley & , E. Ingram (Eds.), Language acquisition: Models and methods (pp. 121–45). Oxford: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sitskoorn, M., & Smitsman, A. (1991). Infants' visual perception of relative size in and containment and support events. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Minneapolis.
Sitskoorn, M., & Smitsman, A. (1995). Infants' perception of dynamic relations between objects: Passing through or support?Developmental Psychology, 31, 437–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slamecka, N., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592–604.Google Scholar
Smeets, J., & Brenner, E. (1995). Perception and action are based on the same visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 19–31.Google ScholarPubMed
Smets, G., Strappers, P., & Overbeeke, L., & van der Mast, C. (1995). Designing in virtual reality: Perception-action coupling and affordances. In , K. Carr & , R. England (Eds.), Simulated and virtual realities: Elements of perception (pp. 189–208). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Smith, C. (1981). A search for structure: Selected essays on science, art, and history. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, L., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). Knowing in the context of acting: The task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychological Review, 106, 235–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smyth, M., & Waller, A. (1998). Movement imagery in rock climbing: Patterns of interference from visual, spatial and kinesthetic secondary tasks. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 145–57.3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, K. (1997). The spontaneous use of perceptual representations during conceptual processing. Doctoral dissertation. University of Chicago.
Solomon, K., & Barsalou, L. (2001). Representing properties locally. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 129–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. (1976). Infants' intermodal perception of events. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 626–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. (1988). When perceiving ends and thinking begins: The apprehension of objects in infancy. In Yonas, Albert (Ed.), Perceptual development in infancy (pp. 197–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. (1990). Origins of visual knowledge. In , D. Osherson & , S. Kosslyn (Eds.), Visual cognition and action: An invitation to cognitive science (pp. 92–127). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. (1991). Physical knowledge in infancy: Reflections on Piaget's theory. In , S. Carey & , R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 133–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. (1998). Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge. Infant Behavior & Development, 21, 181–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, S., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E., & Newport, E. (1998). Nativism, empiricism, and the development of knowledge. In , R. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th Edition) (pp. 275–340). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Spelke, E., Philip, A., & Woodward, A. (1995). Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. In , D. Sperber & , D. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition (pp. 44–78). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spence, J., Smith, L., & Thelen, E. (2001). Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: The influence of prior experience in the spatial memory abilities of two-year-olds. Child Development, 72, 1327–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (2001). In defense of massive modularity. In , E. Dupoux (Ed.), Language, brain, and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 47–57). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sperry, R. (1939). Action current study in movement coordination. Journal of General Psychology, 20, 295–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M., & Geng, J. (2001). Oculomotor mechanism activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychology Research, 65, 235–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stampe, D. (1976). Cardinal numeral systems. Chicago Linguistic Society, 12, 594–609.Google Scholar
Stanfield, R., & Zwaan, R. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12, 153–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steels, L. (1994). Cooperation between distributed agents through self-organization. In , Y. Demazeau & , J.-P. Muller (Eds.), Decentralized AI (pp. 175–96). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Steri, A., Spelke, E., & Rameix, E. (1993). Modality-specific and amodal aspects of object perception in infancy: The case of active touch. Cognition, 47, 251–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, J., Fonlupt, P., Shiffrar, M., & Decety, J. (2000). New aspects of motion perception: Selective neural encoding for apparent human movement. Neuroreport, 11, 109–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, K., & Blumstein, S. (1981). The search for invariant acoustic correlates of phonetic features. In , P. Eimas & , J. Miller (Eds.), Perspectives on the study of speech (pp. 1–38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stewart, J. (1983). Perception of animacy. Dissertation Abstracts International, 43, 2376–7.
Stiefehagen, R., Yang, J., & Waibel, A. (2002). Modeling focus of attention for meeting index based on multiple cues. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks, 13, 928–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stigler, J. (1984). Mental abacus: The effect of abacus training on Chinese children's mental calculation. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 145–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stigler, J., Lee, S., & Stevenson, H. (1986). Digit memory in Chinese and English: Evidence for a temporally limited store. Cognition, 23, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoerig, P., & Cowey, A. (1992). Wavelength discrimination in blindsight. Brain, 115, 425–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoffregen, T., Gordan, K., Sheng, Y.-Y., & Flynn, S. (1997). Perceiving affordance for another person's actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 120–36.Google Scholar
Stoller, P. (1989). The taste of ethnographic things: The senses in anthropology. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Stolz, T. (1994). Sprachdynamik. Bochum: Brockmeyer.Google Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L., & Stepper, S. (1998). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1997). A cognitive theory of meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Strathern, A. (1996). Body thoughts. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1981). The emergence of phonetic structure. Cognition, 10, 301–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1983). The phoneme as perceptuomotor stimulus. In , A. Allport & , D. MacKay (Eds.), Language perception and production: Relationships between listening, speaking, reading, and writing (pp. 67–84). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and situated action: The problem of human-machine communication. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sudnow, D. (1978). Ways of the hand: The organization of improvised conduct. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Suprenant, A., Pitt, M., & Crowder, R. (1993). Auditory recency in immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 193–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1986). Polysemy vs. abstraction: Mutually exclusive or complementary. In , D. Feder, , M. Niepokuj, , V. Nikforidou, & , M. Van Clay (Eds.), Papers from the Twelfth Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 528–38). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1990). From etymology to pragmatics: The mind-body metaphor in semantic structure and semantic change. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12, 49–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. (1996). Fictive motion in language and cognition. In , P. Bloom & , M. Peterson (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 211–276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tanaka, S., & Inui, T. (2002). Cortical movement for action imitation of hand/ arm positions versus finger configurations: An fMRI study. NeuroReport, 13, 1599–1602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taub, S. (2001). Language from the body. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P., & Nerb, J. (2002). Emotional gestalts: Appraisal, change, and the dynamics of affect. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 274–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E. (2000). Grounded in the world: Developmental origins of the embodied mind. Infancy, 1, 3–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. (1994). Dynamic systems approach to development: Applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., Schoener, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L. (2001). The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perservative reaching. Behavorial and Brain Science, 24, 1–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, A., Bulevich, J., & Loftus, E. (2003). Exploring the role of repetitive and sensory exploration in the imagination inflation effect. Memory & Cognition, 31, 630–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, N. (1999). Are theories of imagery theories of imagination? An active perception approach to conscious mental content. Cognitive Science, 23, 207–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, E., & Varela, F. (2001). Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 418–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, E., Palacios, A., & Varela, F. (2002). Ways of coloring: Comparative vision as a case study. In , A. Noe & , E. Thompson (Eds.), Vision and mind (pp. 351–418). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tilley, C. (1994). A phenomenology of landscape. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tipper, S. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibiting priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titchner, E. (1909). Lectures on the experimental psychology of the thought processes. New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todd, N. (1999). Motion in music: A neurobiological perspective. Music Perception, 17, 115–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1995). Mapping the evolved functional organization of mind and brain. In , M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1185–95). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1997). A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1319–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Traugott, E., & Dasher, R. (2002). Regularity in semantic change. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1977). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–46.Google ScholarPubMed
Turing, A. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59, 433–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turvey, M., Solomon, H., & Burton, G. (1989). An ecological analysis of knowing by wielding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 387–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungerleider, L., & Miskin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In , D. Ingle, , M. Goodale, &. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–86). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Valenti, S., & Costall, A. (1997). Visual perception of lifted weight from kinematic and static (photographic) displays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 181–98.Google ScholarPubMed
Vallee-Tourangeau, F., Anthony, S., & Austin, N. (1998). Strategies for generating multiple instances of common and ad hoc categories. Memory, 6, 555–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Heijden, A., Mussler, J., & Bridgeman, B. (1999). On the perception of position. In , G. Aschersleben, , T. Bachmann, & , J. Musseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (pp. 19–37). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Meer, A., Weel, R., & Lee, D. (1995). The functional significance of arm movements in neonates. Science, 267, 693–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelder, T. (1998). The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science. Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 21, 615–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Leeuwen, L., Smitsman, A., & Leeuwen, C. (1994). Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 174–91.Google ScholarPubMed
Orden, G., Jansen op de Haar, M., & Bosman, A. (1997). Complex dynamic systems also predict dissociations, but they do not reduce to autonomous components. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 131–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orden, G., Pennington, B., & Stone, G. (2001). What do double dissociations prove?Cognitive Science, 25, 111–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rooij, D., Bongers, R., & Haselager, W. (2002). A non-representational approach to imagined action. Cognitive Science, 26, 345–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varela, F. (2002). Upward and downward causation in the brain: Case studies in the emergence and efficacy of consciousness. In , Y. Yasue & , M. Jibu (Eds.), No matter, never mind: Proceedings of toward a science of consciousness (pp. 95–117). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Varela, F., & Shear, J. (Eds.) (1999). The view from within. Imprint Academics: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vishton, P., Rea, J., Cutting, J., & Nunez, L. (1999). Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: Relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1659–72.Google Scholar
Viviani, P. (2002). Motor competence in the perception of dynamic events: A tutorial. In , W. Prinz & , B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 406–42). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Viviani, P., Baud-Bovy, G., & Redolfi, M. (1997). Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: Further evidence of motor-perception interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1232–52.Google Scholar
Viviani, P., & Stucchi, N. (1992a). Biological movements look uniform: Evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 18, 603–23.Google Scholar
Viviani, P., & Stucchi, N. (1992b). Motor-perceptual interactions. In , G. Stelmach & , J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior II (pp. 229–48). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Vogt, S. (1995). On the relations between perceiving, imagining and performing in the learning of cyclical movement sequences. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 191–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagman, J., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordances and inertial constraints in tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, S., Winner, E., Cicchetti, D., & Gardner, H. (1981). Metaphorical mappings in human infants. Child Development, 52, 728–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, A. (1982). Intermodal perception of expressive behavior by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 514–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallbott, H. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 879–96.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, S.-H., Baillargeon, R., & Brueckner, L. (2004). Young infants' reasoning about hidden objects: Evidence from violation-of-expectation tasks with test trials only. Cognition, 93, 167–98.
Warren, W. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683–703.Google ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E., & McCarthy, R. (1987). Categories of knowledge: Further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 1273–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category-specific semantic impairment. Brain, 107, 829–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Washburn, M. (1916). Movement and mental imagery. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, D., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480–492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L. (1980). Varieties of residual experience. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 365–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E., Sanders, M., & Marshall, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97, 709–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welch, R. (1978). Perceptual modification: Adapting to altered sensory environments. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Werner, A. (1904). Note on the terms used for “right hand” and “lefthand” in the Bantu languages. Journal of the African Society, 13, 112–16.Google Scholar
Wexler, M., Kosslyn, S., & Berthoz, A. (1998). Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition, 68, 77–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, M., Peterson, S., & Buckner, R. (2000). Memory's echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 11, 125–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, P. (1999). Toward a causal realist account of causal understanding. American Journal of Psychology, 112, 605–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures: Diversity and universals. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, P. (2001). Metaphor in American sign language. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. (2001). The case for sensorimotor coding in working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 49–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M., & Emmorey, K. (1997). A visuospatial phonological loop in working memory: Evidence from American Sign Language. Memory & Cognition, 25, 313–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M., Iverson, A., & Emmorey, K. (2000). Further investigation of the phonological similarity effect for sign language: Two effects of spatial similarity. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Wilson, N., & Gibbs, R. (2005). Real and imagined body movement primes metaphor comprehension. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Wilson, R. (2004). Boundaries of the mind: The individual in the fragile sciences – cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winkler, C. (1994). Rape trauma: Contexts of meaning. in , T. Csordas (Ed.), Embodiment and experience (pp. 248–68). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wohlschlager, S., & Wohlschlager, A. (1998). Mental and manual rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 397–412.Google ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. (1999). Space perception and intended action. In , G. Aschersleben, , T. Bachmann & , J. Musseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (pp. 43–630). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wolff, P., & Levin, J. (1972). The role of overt activity in children's imagery production. Child Development, 43, 537–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. (1999). Infants' abilities to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, T. (2001). Karen in motion: The role of physical enactment in developing an understanding of distance, time, and speed. Mathematical Behavior, 20, 145–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, L., & Barsalou, L. (2001). Grounding concepts in perceptual simulations: I. Evidence from property generation. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Xu, F., & Carey, S. (1996). Infants' metaphysics: The case of numerical identity. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 111–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yarbus, A. (1965). Role of eye movements in the visual process. Oxford: Nauka.
Yu, N. (1999). The contemporary theory of metaphor: Perspectives from Chinese. Amsterdam: Benajmins.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (2003). Chinese metaphors of thinking. Cognitive Linguistics, 14, 141–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabalia, M. (2002). Action and mental imagery in children. Anne Psychologique, 102, 409–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R., Murphy, S., Inglehart, M. (1989). Feeling and facial efference: Implication of the vascular theory of emotion. Psychological Review, 96, 395–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziemke, T. (1999). Rethinking grounding. In , A. Riegler, , M. Peschl, & , A. von Stein (Eds.), Understanding representation in the cognitive sciences (pp. 177–90). New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Zimler, J., & Keenan, J. (1983). Imagery in the congenitally blind: How visual are visual images?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 269–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Zubin, D., & Choi, S. (1984). Orientation and gestalt: Conceptual organizing principles in the lexicalization of space. In , D. Testen, , V. Misha, & , J. Drogo (Eds.), Papers from the parasession of lexical semantics (pp. 333–45). Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. (1996). Processing narrative time shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 22, 1196–1207.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R., Magliano, J., & Graesser, A. (1995). Dimensions of situated model construction in narrative comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 386–97.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R., Stanfield, R., & Yaley, R. (2002). Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science, 13, 168–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Abbott, V., Black, J., & Smith, E. (1985). The representation of scripts in memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 179–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adler, A. (1931). What life should mean to you. Oxford: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Adolph, K. (1997). Learning in the development of infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 62.CrossRef
Adolph, K. (2000). Specificity of learning: Why infants fall over a vertiable cliff. Psychological Science, 11, 290–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adolphs, R., Damasio, H., Tranel, D., Cooper, G., & Damasio, A. (2000). A role for somatosensory cortices in the visual recognition of emotion as revealed by three-dimensional lesion mapping. Journal of Neuroscience, 20, 2683–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aggleton, J., & Mishkin, M. (1986). The amygdala: Sensory gateway to the emotions. In , R. Plutchik & , H. Kellerman (Eds.), Emotion: Theory, research, and experience (pp. 281–9). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Aglioti, S., Goodale, M., & DeSouza, J. (1995). Size-contrast illusions deceive the eye but not the hand. Current Biology, 5, 679–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Agre, P., & Chapman, D. (1987). Pengi: An implementation of a theory of activity. Proceedings of AAAI-87. Menlo Park: AAAI.
Ahsen, A. (1995). Self-report questionnaires: New directions for imagery research. Journal of Mental Imagery, 19, 107–22.Google Scholar
Akshoomoff, N., Courchesne, E., & Townsend, J. (1997). Attention coordination and anticipatory control. International Review of Neurobiology, 411, 575–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M., & DiRusso, A. (1999). The function of gesture in learning to count: More than keeping track. Cognitive Development, 14, 37–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M., Kita, S., & Young, A. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 593–613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allison, T., Puce, R., & McCarthy, G. (2000). Social perception for visual cases of the STS region. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 267–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andrews, E. (1995). Seeing is believing: Visual categories in the Russian lexicon. In , E. Contini-Morava & , B. Goldberg (Eds.), Meaning as explanation (pp. 363–77). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Anifeld, M. (1996). Only tongue protrusion modeling is matched by neonates. Developmental Review, 16, 149–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arditi, A., Holtzman, J., & Kosslyn, S. (1988). Mental imagery and seeing experiences in congenital blindness. Neuropsychologia, 26, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnold, M. (1946). On the mechanism of suggestion and hypnosis. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 41, 107–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arnold, M., & Gasson, S. (1954). Feelings and emotions as dynamic factors in personality integration. In , M. Arnold & , S. Gasson (Eds.), The human person (pp. 294–313). New York: Ronald.Google Scholar
Asci, F. (2003). The effect of physical fitness training on trait anxiety and physical self-concept of female university students. Psychology of Sports and Exercise, 4, 255–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Attneave, F., & Olson, R. (1967). Discriminability of stimuli varying in physical and retinal orientation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74, 149–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ax, A. (1953). The physiological differentiation between fear and anger in humans. Psychosomatic Medicine, 15, 433–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ayer, A. (1936). Language, truth, and logic. London: Gollancz.Google Scholar
Ayers, T., & Jonides, J. (1979). Differing suffix effects for the same physical suffix. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 5, 315–21.Google Scholar
Baars, B. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Babcock, M., & Freyd, J. (1988). Perception of dynamic information in static handwritten forms. American Journal of Psychology, 101, 111–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bach-y-Rita (1996). Sustitucion sensorielle et qualia. In , J. Proust (Ed.), Perception et intermodalite (pp. 81–100). Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. Oxford, England: Clarendon Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory. In , G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and memory: Vol. 8 (pp. 47–89). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A., & Lieberman, K. (1980). Spatial working memory. In , R. Nickerson (Ed.), Attention and performance VIII (pp. 521–39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. (1988). Intermodal learning in infancy: Learning on the basis of two kinds of invariant relational in audible and visible events. Child Development, 59, 197–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bahrick, L. (1995). Intermodal origins of self-perception. In , P. Rochat (Ed.), The self in infancy (pp. 349–73). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. (2000). Increasing specificity in development of intermodal perception. In , D. Muir & , A. Slater (Eds.), Infant development: The essential readings (pp. 119–37). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L., & Watson, S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis for self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bai, D., & Bertenthal, B. (1992). Locomotor structure and the development of spatial search skills. Child Development, 63, 215–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bailey, D. (1998). Getting a grip: A computational model of the acquisition of verb semantics for hand actions. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, International Computer Science Institute, University of California, Berkeley.
Baillargeon, R. (1986). Representing the existence and location of hidden objects: Object permanence in 6- and 8-month-old infants. Cognition, 23, 21–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baillargeon, R. (1987a). Object permanence in 3.5 and 4.5 month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1987b). Young infants' responding about the physical and spatial properties of a hidden object. Cognitive Development, 2, 179–200.
Baillargeon, R. (1993). The object concept revisited: New direction in the investigation of infant's physical knowledge. In , C. Granud (Ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 265–313). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1994). Object permanence in young infants: Further evidence. Child Development, 62, 1227–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (1995). Physical reasoning in infancy. In , M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Physical reasoning in infancy (pp. 187–204). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (2000). How do infants learn about the physical world. In , D. Muir & , A. Slater (Eds.), Infant development: The essential readings (pp. 195–212). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants' physical reasoning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 89–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., & DeVos, J. (1991). Object permanence in young infants: Further evidence. Child Development, 114, 1227–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., DeVos, J., & Graber, M. (1989). Location memory in 8-month-old infants in a non-search AB task: Further evidence. Cognitive Development, 4, 345–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., & Graber, M. (1988). Evidence of location memory in 8-month-old infants in a non-search AB task. Developmental Psychology, 24, 502–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Needham, A., DeVos, J. (1992). The development of young infants' intuitions about support. Early Development and Parenting, 1, 69–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Spelke, E., & Wasserman, S. (1985). Object permanence in five-month-old infants. Cognition, 20, 191–208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, L. (2000). Bodies and persons. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D., & Baird, J. (2001). Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 171–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ballard, D., Hayhoe, M., Pook, P., & Rao, R. (1997). Deictic codes and the embodiment of cognition. Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 20, 723–67.Google ScholarPubMed
Bargh, J., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 230–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11, 211–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. (1985). Ideals, central tendency, and frequency of instantiation as determinants of graded structure in categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 11, 629–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. (1987). The instability of graded structure in concepts. In , U. Neisser (Ed.), Concepts and conceptual development: Ecological and intellectual factors in categorization (pp. 101–40). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1989). Intra-concept similarity and its implications for inter-concept similarity. In , S. Vosniadou & , A. Ortony (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 76–121). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1991). Deriving categories to achieve goals. In , G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory, Vol. 27 (pp. 1–64). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1995). Flexibility, structure, and linguistic vagary in concepts: Manifestations of a compositional system of perceptual symbols. In , A. Collins, , S. Gathercole, , M. Conway, & , P. Morris (Eds.), Theories of memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1999a). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (1999b). Language comprehension: Archival memory or preparation for situated action. Discourse Processes, 28, 61–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. (2002). Being there conceptually: Simulating categories in preparation for situated action. In , N. Stein & , P. Bauer (Eds.), Representation, memory, and development: Essays in honor of Jean Mandler (pp. 1–15). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. (2003). Situated simulation in the human conceptual system. Language & Cognitive Processes, 18, 513–562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L., & Medin, D. (1986). Concepts: Fixed definitions or dynamic context-dependent representations?Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 6, 187–202.Google Scholar
Bassili, J. (1978). Facial motion in the perception of faces and emotional expression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 373–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Basso, K. (1990). Western Apache language and culture: Essays in linguistic anthropology. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bauer, P. (1996). What do infants recall of their lives? Memory for specific events by one- to two-year-olds. American Psychologist, 51, 29–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, P. (1997). Development of memory in early childhood. In , N. Cowan (Ed.), The development of memory in childhood (pp. 83–111). Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bavac-Cikoja, D., & Turvey, M. (1995). Does perceived size depend on perceived distance? An argument for extended haptic perception. Perception and Psychophysics, 57, 216–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J., Black, A., Lemery, C., & Mullet, J. (1986). “I show how you feel”: Motor mimicry as a communicative act. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 322–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beach, K. (1988). The role of external mnemonic system in acquiring an occupation. In , M. Gruneberg & , P. Morris (Eds.), Practical aspects of memory (pp. 342–6). Oxford: Wiley.Google Scholar
Beardsworth, T., & Buckner, T. (1981). The ability to recognize oneself from a video recording of one's movement without seeing one's body. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 18, 19–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, A., & Ward, T. (1991). Children's use of shape in extending novel labels to animate objects: Identity versus postural change. Cognitive Development, 6, 3–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, F. (2001). The meanings of war & peace. College Station: Texas A & M Press.Google Scholar
Beer, R. (1997). The dynamics of adaptive behavior. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 20, 257–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beer, R. (2003). The dynamics of active categorical perception in an evolved model agent. Adaptive Behavior, 11, 209–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beitel, D., Gibbs, R., & Sanders, P. (2001). The embodied approach to the polysemy of the spatial preposition “on.” In , H. Cuyckens (Ed.), Polsemy in cognitive linguistics (pp. 241–60). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bergan, B., & Chang, N. (in press). Simulation-based language understanding in embodied construction grammar. In , J.-O. Ostman & , M. Fried (Eds.), Construction grammar(s): Cognitive and cross-linguistic dimensions. Amsterdam: Benjamins.
Berko, J., Burke, L., Craven, J., & Sarlo, N. (1992). The importance of motor activity in sensorimotor development: A perspective for children with physical handicaps. Human Development, 35, 226–40.Google Scholar
Berkowitz, L. (2000). Causes and consequences of feelings. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berkowitz, L., & Troccoli, B. (1990). Feelings, direction of attention, and expressed evaluations of others. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 305–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermond, B., Nieuwenhuyse, B., Fasolti, S., & Schuerman, J. (1991). Spinal cord lesions, peripheral feedback, and intensities of emotional feelings. Cognition and Emotion, 5, 201–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bermudez, J., Marcel, A., & Eilan, N. (Eds.) (1995). The body and the self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B. (1993). Infants' perception of biomechanical motions: Intrinsic and knowledge-based constraints. In , C. Granrud (Ed.), Visual perception and cognition in infancy (pp. 175–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B., & Campos, J. (1987). New directions in the study of early experience. Child Development, 58, 560–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertenthal, B., Campos, J., & Barrett, K. (1984). Self-produced locomotion: An organizer of emotional, cognitive, and social development in infancy. In , R. Ende & , R. Harmon (Eds.), Continuties and discontinuities in development (pp. 174–210). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Bertenthal, B., Campos, J., & Kermoian, R. (1994). An epigenetic perspective on the development of self-produced locomotion and its consequences. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 140–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bertenthal, B., & Pinto, J. (1993). Complementary processes in the perception and production of human movements. In , L. Smith and , E. Thelen (Eds.), A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications (pp. 209–39). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Berthier, N., DeBlois, S., Poirer, C., Novack, M., & Clifton, R. (2000). Where's the ball? Two- and three-year olds reasons about unseen events. Developmental Psychology, 36, 394–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthoz, A. (2000). The brain's sense of movement. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bigelow, A. (1992). Locomotion and search behavior in blind infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 179–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S-J., Wolpert, D., & Firth, C. (2000). Why can't you tickle yourself?NeuroReport, 11, R11–R16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloch, H. (1990). Structure and function of early sensorymotor coordinations. In , H. Bloch & , B. Bertenthal (Eds.), Sensorymotor organization and development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 163–78). New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Bloom, L. (1993). Language development from two to three. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bloom, P., & German, T. (2001). Two reasons to abandon the false belief task as a test of theory of mind. Cognition, 77, B25–B31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bogartz, R., & Shinsky, J. (1998). On the perception of partially-occluded objects in 6-month-olds. Cognitive Development, 13, 141–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boone, T., & Cunningham, J. (1998). Children's decoding of emotion in expressive body movements: The development of cue attunement. Developmental Psychology, 34, 1007–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75, 1–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers' conception of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 1–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13, 185–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boschker, M., Bakker, F., & Michaels, C. (2002). Effect of mental imagery in realizing affordances. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 775–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Botvinick, J., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature, 391, 756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bower, G., Black, J., & Turner, T. (1979). Scripts in memory for texts. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 177–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, S., & Stark, L. (1997). Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflects the contents of the visual scene. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 27–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brass, M., Bekkering, H., Wohlschlager, A., & Prinz, W. (2000). Compatibility 6 between observed and executed finger movements: Comparing symbolic, spatial, and imitative cues. Brain & Cognition, 44, 124–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brecht, M., Singer, W., & Engel, A. (1998). Correlation analysis of corticotectal interactions in the cat visual system. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79, 2394–2407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridgeman, B. (1983). Mechanisms of space constancy. In , A. Hein & , M. Jeannerod (Eds.), Spatially-oriented behavior (pp. 263–79). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B. (2000). Interaction between vision for perception and vision for behavior. In , Y. Rossetti & , A. Revonsuo (Eds.), Interaction between dissociated implicit and explicit processing (pp. 17–40). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Kirch, M., & Sperling, A. (1981). Segregation of cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Perception & Psychophysics, 29, 336–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Lewis, S., Heit, G., & Nagle, M. (1979). Relations between cognitive and motor-oriented systems of visual position perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5, 692–700.Google Scholar
Bridgeman, B., Peery, S., & Anand, S. (1997). Interaction of cognitive and sensorimotor maps of visual space. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 456–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bridges, K., & Goldberg, D. (1992). Somatization in primary health care: Prevalence and determinants. In , B. Cooper & , R. Eastwood (Eds.), Primary health care and psychiatric epidemiology (pp. 341–50). London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Brighton, V., Segal, A., Werther, P., & Steiner, J. (1977). Facial expression and hedonic response to taste stimuli. Journal of Dental Research, 56, B161.Google Scholar
Brooks, L. (1968). Spatial and verbal components of the act of recall. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 22, 349–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. (1991). Intelligence without representations. Artificial Intelligence, 47, 139–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, R. (2002). Flesh and machine: How robots will change us. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Browder, J., & Gallagher, J. (1948). Dorsal cordotomy for painful phantom limb. Annals of Surgery, 128, 456–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (1992). Articulatory phonology: An overview. Phonetica, 49, 155–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (1995). Dynamics and articulatory phonology. In , R. Port & Gelder, T. (Eds.), Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition (pp. 175–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Brown, J., Collins, A., & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of learning. Educational Researcher, 18, 32–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brownlow, S., Dixon, A., Egbert, C., & Radcliffe, R. (1997). Perception of movement and dancer characteristics for point-light displays of dance. Psychological Record, 47, 411–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brugger, P., Regard, M., & Shiffrar, M. (2000). Hand movement observation in a person born without hands: Is body schema innate? Meeting of the Swiss Neurological Society, London, England, Sept. 2000.
Brugman, C., & Lakoff, G. (1988). Cognitive typology and lexical networks. In , S. Small, , G. Gorrell, & , M. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Lexical ambiguity resolution (pp. 477–508). Palo Alto, CA: Morgan Kaufman.Google Scholar
Brugman, C., & McCaulay, M. (1986). Interacting semantic systems: Mixtec expressions of location. Berkeley Linguistic Society, 12, 315–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J., Goodnow, J., & Austin, G. (1956). A study of thinking. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Bruno, N. (2001). When does action resist visual illusions?Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 379–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, D., & Wright, G. (1999). How body asymmetries determine accessibility in spatial function. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52A, 487–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullitt-Jones, M. (1999). Hunger: A memoir of desire. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Burgess, C. (2000). Theory and operational definitions in computational memory models. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 482–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, C., & Lund, K. (2000). The dynamics of meaning in memory. In , E. Dietrich & , A. Markman (Eds.), Cognitive dynamics (pp. 117–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Burton, G. (1992). Nonvisual judgments of the crossability of path gap. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 698–713.Google Scholar
Butcher, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2000). Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In , D. McNeil (Ed.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action (pp. 167–91). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Butler, S., Berthier, N., & Clifton, R. (2002). Two-year-olds' search strategies and visual tracking in a hidden displacement task. Developmental Psychology, 38, 581–90.
Butterworth, B., & Beattie, G. (1978). Gesture and silence as indicators of planning in speech. In , R. Campbell & , P. Smith (Eds.), Recent advances in the psychology of language (pp. 347–60). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Butterworth, B., & Hadar, U. (1989). Gesture, speech, and computational stages: A reply to McNeil. Psychological Review, 96, 168–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, G. (1993). Dynamic approaches to infant perception and action: Old and new theories about the origins of knowledge. In , L. Smith & , E. Thelen (Eds.), A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications (pp. 171–87). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J., Klein, D., Bernston, G., & Hatsfield, E. (1993). The psychophysiology of emotion. In , M. Lewis & , J. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 119–42). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Camras, L., Lambrecht, L., & Michel, G. (1996). Infant “surprise” expressions as coordinative motor structures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 20, 183–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carlson, R. (1997). Experienced cognition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Carpenter, P., & Eisenberg, P. (1978). Mental rotation and the frame of reference in blind and sighted individuals. Perception & Psychophysics, 23, 117–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carpenter, W. (1874). Principles of mental physiology, with their applications to the training and discipline of the mind and the study of its morbid conditions. New York: Appelton.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Carriedo, N., Alonso, M., & Fernandez, A. (1997). The role of verb tense and verb aspect in the foregrounding of information during reading. Memory & Cognition, 25, 438–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrera-Levillain, P., & Fernandez-Dols, J-M. (1994). Neutral faces in context: Their emotional meaning and their function. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 281–289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll-Phelen, B., & Hampson, P. (1996). Multiple components of the perception of musical sequences: A cognitive neuroscience analysis and some implications for auditory imagery. Music Perception, 13, 517–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, L., & Bauer, P. (1999). When the event is more than the sum of its parts: 9-month-olds' long-term ordered recall. Memory, 7, 147–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cary, M., & Carlson, R. (1999). External support and the development of problem-solving routines. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 1053–70.Google Scholar
Case, R. (1992). The mind's staircase. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cassell, J., Bickmore, T., Campbell, L., Vihjammsson, H., & Yan, H. (2001). More than just a pretty face: Conversational protocols and the affordances of embodiment. Knowledge-Based Systems, 14, 55–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castiello, U. (1996). Grasping a fruit: Selection for action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 582–603.Google Scholar
Cataldi, S. (1996). Emotion, depth, and flesh. Albany: State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Chalmers, D. (1996). The conscious mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Chambers, D., & Reisberg, D. (1992). What an image depicts depends on what an image means. Cognitive Psychology, 24, 145–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, Z., Sanchez, R., & Campbell, T. (1997). From beyond to within their grasp: Analogical problem solving in 10- and 13-month-olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 790–801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Churchland, P. (1984). Matter and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Churchland, P. (1985). Reduction, qualia, and the direct introspection of brain states. Journal of Philosophy, 82, 8–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Churchland, P., Ramachandran, V., & Sejinowski, T. (1994). A critique of pure vision. In , C. Koch & , J. Davis (Eds.), Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain (pp. 23–60). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cienki, A. (1998). Straight: An image schema and its metaphorical extensions. Cognitive Linguistics, 9, 107–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clancey, W. (1997). Situated cognition: On human knowledge and computer representations. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1996). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). The dynamic challenge. Cognitive Science, 21, 461–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2003). Natural-born cyborgs: Minds, technologies, and the future of human intelligence. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, H. (1996). Using language. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, E. (2001). Meaning and the specification of motion in music. Musicae Scientiae, 5, 213–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarke, S., & French, R. (1978). Can congenital amputees achieve academically?American Corrective Therapy Journal, 32, 7–11.Google ScholarPubMed
Clearfield, M. (2000). The role of locomotor experience in the development of navigational memory. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation.
Clement, C. (1987). Applying general principles to novel problems as a function of learning history: Abstraction from examples vs. studying general statements. Dissertation Abstracts International, 48, 585.Google Scholar
Coccia, M., Bartolini, M., Luzzi, S., Provinciali, L., & Ralph, M. (2004). Semantic memory is an amodal, dynamic system: Evidence from the interaction of naming and object use in semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 21, 515–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, L., & Oakes, L. (1993). How infants perceive a simple causal event. Developmental Psychology, 29, 421–433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colcombe, S., & Wyer, R. (2001). The role of prototypes in the mental representation of temporally related events. Cognitive Psychology, 44, 67–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cole, J. (1995). Pride and the daily marathon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Cole, J. (1997). On “being faceless”: Selfhood and facial embodiment. Journal of Consciousness, 4, 467–84.Google Scholar
Cole, M., Hood, L., & McDermott, R. (1997). Concepts of ecological validity: Their differing implications for comparative cognition. In , M. Cole & , Y. Engestroem (Eds.), Mind, culture, and activity (pp. 48–58). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Collie, R., & Hayne, H. (1999). Deferred imitation by 6- and 9-month-old infants: More evidence for declarative memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 35, 83–90.3.0.CO;2-S>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooper, L., & Shepard, R. (1982). Mental imagery and their transformations. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Connell, J. (1989). A colony architecture for an artificial machine. AI Tech Report 1152, MIT AI Labs, August.
Corballis, M. (1994). Neuropsychology of perceptual functions. In , D. Zaidel (Ed.), Neuropsychology handbook of perception and cognition (2nd ed.) (pp. 83–104). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Corriss, D., & Kose, G. (1998). Action and imagination in the formation of images. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 87, 979–83.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1997). Dissecting the computational architecture of social inference mechanisms. Characterizing human psychological adaptations. Ciba Foundation symposium, No. 208 (pp. 132–61). New York: Wiley.
Craighero, L. (1996). Grasping a fruit: Selection for action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 582–603.Google Scholar
Craighero, L., Fadiga, L., Rizzolatti, G., & Umilta, C. (1999). Action for perception: A motor-visual attentional effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1673–92.Google ScholarPubMed
Creem, S., & Proffitt, D. (1998). Two memories for geographical slant: Separation and interdependence of action and awareness. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 5, 22–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1996). Why neuroscience may be able to explain consciousness. Scientific American, 273, 84–85.Google Scholar
Crott, W., & Cruse, A. (2004). Cognitive Linguistics. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: Perennial.Google Scholar
Csordas, T. (Ed.) (1994). Embodiment and experience. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cutting, J., Proffitt, D., & Kozlowski, L. (1978). A biomechanical invariant for gait perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 4, 357–72.Google ScholarPubMed
Damasio, A. (1989). Time-locked multiregional retroactivation: A system-level proposal for the neural substrate of recall and recognition. Cognition, 33, 25–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes' error: Emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: G.P. Putnam & Sons.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. New York: Harcourt Brace & Co.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain. New York: Harcourt.Google Scholar
Damasio, A., & Damasio, H. (1994). Cortical systems for retrieval of concrete knowledge: The convergence zone framework. In , C. Koch & , J. Davis (Eds.), Large-scale neuronal theories of the brain (pp. 61–74). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (1999). Neural mechanisms subserving the perception of human action. Trends in Cognitive Science, 3, 172–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., Grezess, J., Costes, N., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Procyk, E., Grassi, F., & Fazio, F. (1997). Brain activity during observation of actions: Influence of action content and subject's strategy. Brain, 120, 1763–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Jeannerod, M., & Problanc, C. (1989). The timing of mentally represented actions. Behavioral Brain Research, 34, 35–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Perani, D., Jeannerod, M., Bettinardi, V., Tadary, B., Woods, R., Mazziotta, J., & Fazio, F. (1994). Mapping motor representations with PET. Nature, 371, 600–2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLoache, J., Uttal, D., & Rosengren, K. (2004). Scale errors offer evidence for a perception-action dissociation early in life. Science, 304, 1027–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dennett, D. (1992). Consciousness explained. Boston: Little Brown.Google Scholar
Rivera, J. (1977). A structural theory of the emotions. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Descartes, R. (1984). The philosophical writings of Descartes: vol. 2. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1896). The reflex arc concept in psychology. Psychological Review, 3, 357–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dewey, J. (1934). Art as experience. New York: Minton, Balch.Google Scholar
Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The theory of inquiry. New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Diamond, A. (1991). Neuro-psychological insights into the meaning of object concept development. In , S. Carey and , R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology & cognition (pp. 67–110). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., & Knippenberg, A. (1998). The relation between perception and behavior, or how to win a game of Trivial Pursuit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 865–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dijksterhuis, A., Bargh, J., & Miedema, J. (2001). Of mice and mackerels: Attention and automatic social behavior. In , H. Bless & , J. Forgas (Eds.), Subjective experience in social cognition and behavior (pp. 37–51). Philadelphia: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
DiPelligrinoi, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
diSessa, A. (1993). Toward an epistemology of physics. Cognition & Instruction, 10, 105–225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodd, B. (1979). Lipreading in infancy: Attention to speech in and out of synchrony. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 478–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doffner, G. (1999). The connectionist route to embodiment and dynamicism. In , A. Riegler, , M. Peschl, & , A. von Stein (Eds.), Understanding representation in the cognitive sciences: Does representational need reality? (pp. 23–32). New York: Kluwer..Google Scholar
Donald, M. (2001). A mind so rare: The evolution of human consciousness. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Dorman, M., Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Raphael, L. (1977). Stop-consonant recognition: Release bursts and formant transitions as functionally equivalent, context-dependent cues. Perception & Psychophysics, 22, 109–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Douglas, M. (1970). Natural symbols. New York: Pantheon.Google Scholar
Downing, L. (2000). Negation, text worlds, and discourse: The pragmatics of fiction. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Duclos, S., Laird, J., Schneider, E., Sexter, M., Stern, L., & Lighten, O. (1989). Emotion-specific effects of facial expressions and postures on emotional experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 100–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eagle, R. (1985). Deprivation of early sensorimotor experience and cognition in the severely involved cerebral palsy child. Journal of Autism, and Developmental Disorders, 15, 269–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G. (1992). Bright air, brilliant fire: On the matter of the mind. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Eimer, M. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and automatic response activation: Evidence from psychophysiological studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 335–59.Google ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1985). Telling lies: clues to deceit in the marketplace, politics, and marriage. New York: Norton.
Ekman, P. (1992). Are there basic emotions?Psychological Review, 99, 550–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1994). Strong evidence for universals in facial expressions: A reply to Russell's mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 268–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P., & Davidson, R. (Eds.) (1994). The nature of emotions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., Levenson, R., & Friesen, W. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishing among emotions. Science, 221, 1208–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N., & Hennelly, R. (1980). A bilingual word-length effect: Implications for intelligence testing and the relative ease of mental calculation in Welsh and English. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 43–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (1995). Questioning consciousness. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, R. (2002). Efferent brain processes and the enactive approach to consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 40–52.Google Scholar
Ellis, R., & Tucker, M. (2000). Micro-affordances: The potentiation of components of action by seen objects. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 457–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Emmorey, K. (2002). Language, cognition, and the brain: Insights from sign language research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Emmorey, K., Kosslyn, S., & Bellugi, U. (1993). Visual imagery and visual-spatial language: Enhanced imagery abilities in deaf and hearing ASL signers. Cognition, 46, 139–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelkamp, J. (1998). Memory for actions. Hove, England: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. (1984). Motor programme information as a separable unit. Psychological Research, 46, 283–297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epstein, W. (1973). The process of ‘taking-into-account’ in visual perception. Perception, 2, 267–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M., Hammond, K., Levine, D., & Calvanio, R. (1988). Visual and spatial mental imagery: Dissociable systems of representation. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 439–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farah, M., & McClelland, J. (1991). A computational model of semantic memory impairment: Modality specificity and emergent category-specificity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 120, 339–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farnell, B. (1995). Do you see what I mean? Plains Indian sign talk and the embodiment of action. Austin: University of Texas Press.Google Scholar
Feldman, J., & Narayanan, S. (2004). Embodiment in a neural theory of language. Brain & Language, 89, 385–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernandez-Dols, J-M., & Carrera, P. (1994). Neutral faces in context: Their emotional meaning and their function. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 287–99.Google Scholar
Fernandez-Dols, J-M., Carrera, P., Casado, C. (2002). The meaning of expression: Views from art and other sources. In , L. Anolli, , R. Ciceri, & , G. Riva (Eds.), Say not to say: New perspectives on miscommunication (pp. 117–34). Amsterdam: IOS Press.Google Scholar
Fernandez-Dols, J-M., & Ruiz-Belda, M. (1995). Are smiles signs of happiness? Gold medal winners at the Olympic games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1113–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrara, K. (1994). Therapeutic ways with words. New York: Oxford.Google Scholar
Fery, J.-C. (2003). Differentiating visual and kinesthetic imagery in mental practiceCanadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, ⅹⅹ–ⅹⅹ.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fillmore, C. (1982). Frame semantics. In Linguistic Society of Korea (Ed.), Linguistics in the morning calm (pp. 111–38). Hansin, Seoul.
Finke, R. (1989). Principles of mental imagery. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Finke, R., & Freyd, J. (1985). Transformations of visual memory induced by implied motions of pattern elements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 11, 780–94.Google ScholarPubMed
Finke, R., Freyd, J., & Shyi, G. (1986). Implied velocity and acceleration induce transformations of visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 175–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, S. (1990). The evolution of psychological concepts about the body. In , T. Cash & , T. Pruzinsky (Eds.), Body images: Development, deviance, and change (pp. 3–20). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Fitzpatrick, P., Carello, C., Schmidt, R., & Corey, D. (1994). Haptic and visual perception of an affordance for upright posture. Ecological Psychology, 6, 265–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flanagan, O. (2002). The problem of the soul: Two visions of mind and how to reconcile them. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Flanagan, R., & Beltzer, M. (2000). Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor prediction in the size-weight illusion. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 737–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flanagan, R., King, S., Wolpert, D., & Johansson, R. (2001). Sensorimotor prediction and memory in object manipulation. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 87–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, C., van den Broek, P., & Arthur, E. (1996). A model of narrative comprehension and recall. In , B. Britton & , A. Graesser (Eds.), Models of understanding text (pp. 142–64). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fletcher, R. (1994). Levels of representation in memory for discourse. In , M. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 589–608). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Flores d'Arcais, G., & Schreuder, R. (1982). Semantic activation during object naming. Psychological Research, 49, 153–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fogel, A., & Hannan, T. (1985). Manual actions of nine- to fifteen-week-old human infants during face-to-face interactions with their mothers. Child Development, 56, 1271–9.Google Scholar
Foster, D. (1983). Visual discrimination, categorical identification, and categorical rating in brief displays of curved lines: Implications for discrete encoding processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 785–806.Google ScholarPubMed
Foster, J., & Strack, F. (1996). Influence of overt head movement on memory for valenced words: A case of conceptual-motor compatibility. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 421–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, C. (1986). An event approach to the study of speech perception from a direct-realist perspective. Journal of Phonetics, 14, 3–28.Google Scholar
Fowler, C. (1987). Perceivers as realists, talkers too: Commentary on papers by Strange, Diehl et al., and Rakerd and Verbrugge. Journal of Memory & Language, 26, 574–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fowler, C. (1994). Auditory “objects” – The role of motor activity in auditory perception and speech perception. In , K. Pribram (Ed.), Origins: Brain and self-organization (pp. 593–603). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Fowler, C., & Rosenblum, D. (1991). The perception of phonetic gestures. In , I. Mattingly & , M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Modularity and the motor theory of speech perception (pp. 33–59). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Franz, V., Gegenfurtner, K., Buelthoff, H., & Fahle, M. (2000). Grasping visual illusions: No evidence for a dissociation between perception and action. Psychological Science, 11, 20–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freeman, N., Lloyd, S., & Sinha, C. (1980). Infant search tasks reveal early concepts of containment and canonical usage of objects. Cognition, 8, 243–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, W. (1991). The physiology of perception. Scientific American, 264, 78–87.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, W. (2001). How brains make up their minds. New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Freyd, J. (1987). Dynamic mental representation. Psychological Review, 94, 429–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freyd, J., & Finke, R. (1984). Representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126–32.Google Scholar
Freyd, J., & Finke, R. (1985). A velocity effect for representational momentum. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 23, 443–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freyd, J., & Johnson, J. (1987). Probing the time course of representational momentum. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 126–32.Google Scholar
Freyd, J., & Jones, K. (1994). Representational momentum for a spiral path. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 968–76.Google ScholarPubMed
Freyd, J., Kelly, M., & DeKay, M. (1990). Representational momentum in memory for pitch. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16, 1107–17.Google ScholarPubMed
Freyd, J., & Pantzer, T. (1995). Static patterns moving in the mind. In , S. Smith, , T. Ward, & , R. Finke (Eds.), The creative cognition approach (pp. 181–204). Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Freyd, J., Pantzer, T., & Cheng, J. (1988). Representing statics as forces in equilibrium. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117, 395–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friberg, A., & Sundberg, J. (1994). Does music performance allude to locomotion? A model of final ritardandi derived from measurements of stopping runners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, 1469–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friberg, A., & Sundberg, J. (1999). Does music performance allude to locomotion? A model of final ritardandi derived from measurements of stopping runners. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 105, 1469–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friberg, A., Sundberg, J., & Fryden, L. (2000). Music for motion: Sound level envelopes of tones expressing human locomotion. Journal of New Musice Rsearch, 24, 199–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fridlund, A. (1994). Human facial expression. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Friedman, R., & Foerster, J. (2000). The effect of approach and avoidance motor actions on the elements of creative insight. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79, 477–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frith, C., Blakemore, S., & Wolport, D. (2000). Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: Abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 357–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gainotti, G., Silveri, M., Daniele, A., & Giustolisi, L. (1995). Neuroimagining correlates of category-specific semantic disorders: A critical survey. Memory, 3, 247–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. (1995). Body schema and intentionality. In , J. L. Bermudez & , A. Marcel (Eds.), The body and self (pp. 225–44). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2001). The practice of mind: Theory, simulation, or primary interaction?Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 83–103.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Marcel, A. (1999). The self in contextualized action. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 212–28.Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Meltzoff, A. (1996). The emerging sense of self and others: Merleau-Ponty and recent developmental studies. Philosophical Psychology, 9, 211–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallese, V. (2000). The inner sense of action: Agency and motor representationsJournal of Consciousness Studies, 7, 23–40.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., Ferari, P., & Umilta, M. (2002). The mirror matching system: A shared manifold for intersubjectivity. Behaviorial and Brain Sciences, 25, 35–6.Google Scholar
Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirron neurons and the simulation theory of mind reading. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 439–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind: The theories of multiple intelligences. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1985). The mind's new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gardner, R., Martinez, R., & Sandoval, Y. (1987). Obesity and body image: An evaluation of sensory and non-sensory components. Psychological Medicine, 17, 927–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrod, S., & Sanford, A. (1985). On the real-time character of interpretation during reading. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 43–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garry, M., & Polaschik, D. (2000). Imagination and memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 6–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (1993). Vagueness's puzzles, polysemy's vagueness. Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 223–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, D. (1997). Diachronic prototype semantics: A contribution to historical lexicography. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1979). Meaning and order in Morocan society. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gelman, R. (1991). Epigenetic foundations of knowledge structures: Initial and transcendent construction. In , S. Carey and , R. Gelman (Eds.), Epigenesis of mind: Essays in biology and cognition (pp. 293–322). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gelman, R., Durgin, F., & Kaufman, L. (1995). Distinguishing between animates and inanimates: Not by motion alone. In , D. Sperber & , D. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition (pp. 150–84). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., Imai, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2002). As time goes by: Understanding time as spatial metaphor. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 537–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Georgopoulos, A., Lurito, J., Petrides, M., Schwartz, A., & Massey, J. (1989). Mental rotation of the neuronal population vector. Science, 243, 234–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., Nadasdy, Z.Csiba, G., & Biro, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56, 165–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gergely, G., & Watson, J. (1999). Early socio-emotional development: Contingency perception and the social-biofeedback model. In , P. Rochat (Ed.), Early social cognition: Understanding others in the first months of life (pp. 101–36). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. (1991). The saturated self. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gerlach, C., Law, I., & Paulson, O. (2002). When action turns into words: Activation of motor-based knowledge during categorization of manipulable objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 1230–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geurts, K. (2002). Culture and the senses: Bodily ways of knowing in an African community. Berkeley: University of California Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1996). Why many concepts are metaphorical. Cognition, 61, 309–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R. (1999a). Intentions in the experience of meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (1999b). Moving metaphor out of the head and into the cultural world. In , R. Gibbs & , G. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 145–66). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., Beitel, D., Harrington, M., & Sanders, P. (1994). Taking a stand on the meanings of “stand”: Bodily experience as motivation for polysemy. Journal of Semantics, 11, 231–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Berg, E. (2002). Mental imagery and embodied activity. Journal of Mental Imagery, 26, 1–30.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Franks, H. (2002). Embodied metaphor in women's narratives about their experiences with cancer. Health Communication, 14, 139–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R., Lima, P., & Francuzo, E. (2004). Metaphor in thought and language is grounded in embodied experience. Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 1189–210.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Matlock, T. (2000). Psycholinguistics and mental representations. Cognitive Linguistics, 10, 263–9.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & O'Brien, J. (1990). Idioms and mental imagery: The metaphorical motivation for idiomatic meaning. Cognition, 36, 35–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R., Strom, L., & Spivey-Knowlton, M. (1997). Conceptual metaphor in mental imagery for proverbs. Journal of Mental Imagery, 21, 83–110.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Tenney, Y. (1980). The concept of scripts in understanding stories. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 9, 275–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibbs, R., & Orden, G. (2003). Are emotional expressions intentional? A self-organizational approach. Consciousness & Emotion, 4, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, E. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and the acquiring of knowledge. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 1–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. (1962). Observations on active touch. Psychological Review, 69, 477–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston.
Gibson, J. (1968). What gives rise to the perception of motion?Psychological Review, 57, 335–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Gilbert, A., Fridlund, A., & Sabini, J. (1987). Hedonic and social determinants of facial displays to odors. Chemical Senses, 12, 355–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. (1997). What is memory for?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. (1999). Why mental models need to be embodied. In , G. Rickert & , C. Habel (Eds.), Mental models in discourse processing (pp. 77–90). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Glenberg, A., & Robertson, D. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379–401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A., Meyer, M., & Lindem, K. (1987). Mental models contribute to foregrounding during text comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 69–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A., Schroeder, J., & Robertson, D. (1998). Averting the gaze disengages the environment and facilitates remembering. Memory & Cognition, 26, 651–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glucksberg, S. (2001). Understanding figurative language. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S. (2002). Emotion language: A new synthesis?Contemporary Psychology, 47, 764–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S., Brown, M., & McGlone, M. (1993). Conceptual metaphors are not automatically accessed during idiom comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 21, 711–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glucksberg, S., & Keysar, B. (1990). Understanding metaphorical comparisons: Beyond similarity. Psychological Review, 97, 3–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goff, L., & Roediger, H. (1998). Imagination inflation for action events: Repeated imaginings leads to illusory recollection. Memory & Cognition, 26, 20–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. New York: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Goffman, E. (1976). Response cries. Language, 54, 787–815.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldap, C. (1992). Morphology and semantics of Yucatec space relators. Zeitschrift fur Phonetik, Sprachwissenschaft und Kommunikationsforschung, 45, 612–25.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldfield, E. (1993). Dynamic systems in development: Action systems. In , L. Smith & , E. Thelen (Eds.), A dynamic systems approach to development: Applications (pp. 51–70). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goldie, P. (2000). Explaining expressions of emotion. Mind, 109, 25–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldinger, S. (1995). Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological Review, 105, 251–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, A. (1970). A theory of action. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Goodale, M., & Humphrey, G. (1998). The objects of action and perception. Cognition, 67, 181–207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodale, M., & Murphy, K. (2000). Space and the brain: Different neural substrates for allocentric and egocentric forms of reference. In , T. Metzinger (ed.), Neural correlates of consciousness (pp. 189–202). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C. (1981). Conversational organization: Interaction between speakers and hearers. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Goossens, L., Pauwels, B., Rudzka-Ostyn, M., Simon-Venderberger, J., & Varpays, J. 1995. By word of mouth: Metaphor, metonymy, and linguistic action in a cognitive perspective. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gorfein, D. (Ed.) (2001). On the consequences of meaning selection: Principles of resolving lexical ambiguity. Washington, DC: APA Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, F. (2002). On the epigenetic evolution of species-specific perception: The developmental manifold concepts. Cognitive Development, 17, 1287–310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gouin-Decarie, T. (1969). A study of the mental and emotional development of the thalidomide child. In , B. Foss (Ed.). Determinants of infant behavior (pp. 167–187). London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Grady, J. (1997). Theories are buildings revisited. Cognitive Linguistics, 8, 267–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grady, J. (1999). A typology of motivation for conceptual metaphor: Correlation vs. resemblance. In , R. Gibbs & , G. Steen (Eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics (pp. 79–100). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Graesser, A., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101, 371–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graesser, A., Woll, S., Kowalski, D., & Smith, D. (1980). Memory for typical and atypical actions in scripted activities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 503–15.Google Scholar
Grafton, S., Fadiga, L., Arbib, M., & Rizzolatti, G. (1997). Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools. Neuroimage, 6, 231–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Granott, N., & Paziale, J. (Eds.) (2002). Microdevelopment: Transition processes in development and learning. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. (2001). Understanding microworlds. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 879–911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenberg, J. (1978). Universals of human language: Vol. 3. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Greene, R., & Samuel, A. (1986). Recency and suffix effects in serial recall of musical stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 12, 517–24.Google ScholarPubMed
Gregory, R., & Wallace, J. (1963). Recovery from early blindness: A case study. Monograph No. 2. Experimental Psychology Society.
Grezes, J., & Decety, J. (2001). Functional anatomy of execution, mental simulation, observation, and verb generation of actions: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 12, 1–19.3.0.CO;2-V>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Science, 27, 377–396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadamard, J. (1945). The psychology of invention in the mathematical field. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hadar, U. (1989). Two types of gesture and their role in speech production. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 8, 221–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hadar, U., Wenkert-Olenik, D., Krauss, R., & Soroker, N. (1998). Gesture and the pro-cessing of speech: Neuropsychological evidence. Brain & Language, 62, 107–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haith, M. (1997). The development of future thinking as essential for the emergence of skill in planning. In S. Friedman & Scholnick (Eds.), The developmental psychology of planning: Why, how, and when do we plan? (pp. 25–42). Mahwah, NJ Erlbaum.
Halff, H., Ortony, A., & Anderson, R. (1976). A context-sensitive representation of word meaning. Menory & Cognition, 4, 378–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, C., Bernoties, L., & Schmidt, D. (1995). Interference effects of mental imagery on a motor task. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 181–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, E., & Cairns, H. (1961). Plato: The collected dialogues. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hanna, E., & Meltzoff, A. (1993). Peer imitation by toddlers in laboratory and daycare contexts: Implications for social learning and memory. Developmental Psychology, 29, 701–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanrahan, C., Tetreau, B., & Sarrazin, C. (1995). Use of imagery while performing dance movement. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 26, 413–30.Google Scholar
Hardy, L., & Callow, N. (1999). Efficacy of external and internal visual imagery perspectives for the enhancement of performances on tasks in which form is important. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 95–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harker, J., & Wierzbicka, A. (Eds.) (2001). Emotion in crosslinguistic perspective. New York: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Harman, K., Humphrey, G., & Goodale, M. (1999). Active manual control of object views facilitate object recognition. Current Biology, 9, 1315–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harnad, S. (1990). The symbol grounding problem. Physica D, 42, 335–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatano, G., & Osawa, K. (1983). Digit memory of grand experts in abacus-derived mental calculation. Cognition, 5, 47–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J., & Rapsom, R. (1992). Primitive emotional contagion. In , M. Clark (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology: Vol. 14. Emotion and social behavior (pp. 151–77). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Google Scholar
Hatsopoulos, N., & Warren, W. (1991). Resonance tuning in rhythmic arm movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 28, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Healey, A. (1982). Short-term memory for order information. In , G. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation: Vol. 16 (pp. 191–238). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hecht, H., Vogt, S., & Prinz, W. (2001). Motor learning enhances perceptual judgment: A case for action-perception transfer. Psychological Research, 65, 3–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Heine, B. (1989). Adpositions in African languages. Linguistique Africaine, 2, 77–127.Google Scholar
Heine, B. (1997). Cognitive foundations of grammar. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Heine, B., Ulrike, C., & Hunnemeyer, F. (1991). Grammaticalization: A conceptual framework. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Heller, J. (1961). Catch-22. New York: Knopf.Google Scholar
Hemingway, E. (1960). The collected poems of Ernest Hemingway. San Francisco: Pirated Edition.Google Scholar
Henderson, W., & Smyth, G. (1948). Phantom limbs. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, Psychiatry, 11, 88–117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heptulla-Chatterjee, S., Freyd, J., & Shiffrar, M. (1996). Configural processing in the perception of apparent biological motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 916–29.Google Scholar
Hermer, L., & Spelke, E. (1994). A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children. Nature, 370, 57–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hertenstein, M. (2002). Touch: Its communicative function in infancy. Human Development, 45, 70–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewes, G. (1983). The invention of phonemically-based language. In Grolier, E. (Ed.), Glossogenetics: The origins and evolution of language (pp. 143–62). Paris: Harwood Publishers.Google Scholar
Heywood, C., & Kentridge, R. (2000). Affective blindsight?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 125–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hohmann, G. (1966). Some effects of spinal cord lesions on experienced emotional feelings. Psychophysiology, 3, 143–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hommel, B. (1995). Stimulus-response compatibility and the Simon effect: Toward an empirical clarification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 764–75.Google Scholar
Hommel, B. (1996). S-R compatibility effects without response uncertainty. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 546–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hommel, B., Musseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hornstein, S., & Mulligan, N. (2001). Memory of action events: The role of objects in memory of self- and other-performed tasks. American Journal of Psychology, 114, 199–217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, D. (2003). Sensual relations: Engaging the senses in culture and social theory. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubbard, T. (1990). Cognitive representation of linear motion: Possible direction and gravity effects in judged displacement. Memory & Cognition, 18, 299–309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, T. (1995). Cognitive representations of motion: Evidence for friction and gravity analogues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 241–54.Google ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, T. (1996). Representational momentum, centripetal force, and curvilinear impetus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1049–62.Google ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, T. (1999). How consequences of physical properties influence mental representation: The environmental invariants hypothesis. In , P. Killeen & , W. Uttal (Eds.), Fechner Day 99: The end of 20th-century psychophysics, Proceedings of the 15th annual meeting of the International Society for Psychophysics (pp. 274–9). Tempe, AZ: International Society for Psychophysics.Google Scholar
Hubbard, T., & Bharacha, J. (1988). Judged displaced in apparent vertical and horizontal motion. Perception & Psychophysics, 44, 211–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. (1974). Vision in a monkey without striate cortex: A case study. Perception, 3, 241–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hupka, R., Zaleski, Z., Otto, J., Reidl, L., Tarabrina, N. (1996). Anger, envy, fear, and jealousy as felt in the body: A five-nation study. Cross-Cultural Research, 30, 243–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Husserl, E. (1977). Phenomenological psychology. The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Husserl, E. (1980). Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. Boston: Kluwer Academic.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutchinson, W., Davis, K., Lozano, A, Tasker, R., & Dostrovsky, J. (1999). Pain-related neurons in the human cingulated cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 403–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iacoboni, M., Woods, R., & Mazziotta, J. (1998). Brain-behavior relationships: Evidence from practice effects or spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Journal of Neurophysiology, 76, 321–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Intos-Peterson, M., & Roskos-Ewoldsen, B. (1987). Sensory-perceptual qualities of images. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 15, 188–99.Google Scholar
Ito, M. (1993). Movement and thought: Identical control mechanisms by the cerebellum. Trends in Neuroscience, 16, 448–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ito, M. (1999). Imagined movement and response programming. Journal of Mental Imagery, 23, 71–84.Google Scholar
Iverson, J., & Thelen, E. (1999). Hand, mouth, and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11–12, 19–40.Google Scholar
Ivry, R., & Fiez, J. (2000). Cerebellum contributions to cognition and imagery. In , M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neuroscience (pp. 999–1011). New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R., & Aaron, D. (1991). Review of G. Lakoff & M. Turner, More than cool reason: A filed guide to poetic metaphor. Language, 67, 320–38.Google Scholar
Jackson, F. (1982). Epiphenomenal qualia. Philosophical Quarterly, 32, 127–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, F. (1986). What Mary didn't know. Journal of Philosophy, 83, 291–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, J. (1994). Chronic pain and the tension between the body as subject and object. In , T. Csordas (Ed.), Embodiment and experience (pp. 201–28). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jacobson, E. (1932). Electrophysiology of mental activities. American Journal of Psychology, 44, 677–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, K., Humphrey, K., & Goodale, M. (2001). Manipulating and recognizing virtual objects: Where the action is. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55, 111–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: MacMillan.Google Scholar
James, W. (1892). Psychology: Briefer course. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1895). The knowing of things together. Psychological Review, 2, 105–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarvella, R., & Collas, J. (1974). Memory for the intentions of sentences. Memory & Cognition, 2, 185–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jeannerod, M. (1994). The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery. Behaviorial and Brain Sciences, 17, 187–245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeannerod, M. (1995). Mental imagery in the motor cortex. Neuropsychologica, 33, 1419–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jeannerod, M. (1999). To act or not to act: Perspectives in the representation of action. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 52, 1–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–11.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (1991). Knowing through the body. Philosophical Psychology, 4, 3–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, M. (1993). Conceptual metaphor and embodied structures of meaning. Philosophical Psychology, 6, 413–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, S. (2000). Thinking ahead: The case for motor imagery in prospective judgments of prehension. Cognition, 74, 33–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Jusczyk, P. (1995). Infants' detection of the sound patterns of words in fluent speech. Cognitive Psychology, 29, 1–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaczmarek, K., & Bach-y-Rita, P. (1995). Tactile displays. In , W. Barfield & Furness, T. (Eds.), Virtual environments and advanced interface design (pp. 349–414). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kaiser, M., & Proffitt, D. (1987). Observers' sensitivity to dynamic anomalies in collision. Perception and Psychophysics, 42, 275–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalnins, I., & Bruner, J. (1973). The coordination of visual observation and instrumental behavior in early infancy. Perception, 2, 307–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kandel, S., Orliaguet, J.-P., & Viviani, P. (2000). Perceptual anticipation in handwriting: The role of implicit motor competence. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 706–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kant, I., (1787/1927). Immanuel Kant's Critique of pure reason : in commemoration of the centenary of its first publication. New York: MacMillan.
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1992). Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective on cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kaschak, M., & Glenberg, A. (2000). Constructing meaning: The role of affordances and grammatical constructions in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 508–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P., & Fillmore, C. (1999). Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The “what's X doing Y?” construction. Language, 75, 1–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keefe, D., & McDaniel, M. (1993). The time course and durability of predictive inferences. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 446–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keen, R. (2003). Representation of objects and events: Why do infants look so smart and toddlers look so dumb?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 79–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, M., & Freyd, J. (1987). Explorations of representational momentum. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 369–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelso, J. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Ellsworth, P., & Edwards, K. (1993). Beyond simple pessimism: Effects of sadness and anger on social perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 740–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, J., Gabia, P., & Nicholls, A. (1991). Tactile pictures. In , M. Heller & , W. Schift (Eds.), The psychology of touch (pp. 263–99). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kermoian, R., & Campos, J. (1988). Locomotor experience: A facilitator of spatial cognitive development. Child Development, 59, 908–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr, N. (1983). The role of vision in “visual imagery” experiments: Evidence from the congenitally blind. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 112, 265–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, D. (1973). The asymmetry of the human brain. Scientific American, 228, 70–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95, 163–82.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kirsh, D. (1995). The intelligent use of space. Artificial Intelligence, 73, 31–68.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatt, D. (1989). Review of selected models of speech perception. In , W. Marseln-Wilson (Ed.), Lexical representations and processes (pp. 169–226). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Klatzky, R. (1994). On the relation between motor imagery and visual imagery. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 212–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, R., Lederman, S., & Metzger, V. (1985). Identifying objects by touch: An expert system. Perception and Psychophysics, 37, 299–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzky, R., Loomis, J., Lederman, S., & Wake, H. (1993). Haptic identification of objects and their depictions. Perception & Psychophysics, 54, 170–78.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klatzky, R., Pellegrino, J., McCloskey, B., & Doherty, S. (1989). Can you squeeze a tomato?: The role of motor representations in semantic sensibility judgments. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 56–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinman, A. (1982). Neurasthenia and depression: A study of somatization and culture in China. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 6, 117–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., & Flach, R. (2001). Predicting the effects of action: Interaction of perception and action. Psychological Science, 12, 467–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knoblich, G., Seigerschmidt, E., Flach, R., & Prinz, W. (2002). Authorship effects in the production of handwriting strokes: Evidence for action simulation during action perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55A, 1027–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knuf, L., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). An analysis of ideomotor action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 779–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umilta, M., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing sounds, understanding actions: Action representation in mirror neurons. Science, 297, 846–48.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koivisto-Alanko, P. (1998). Mechanisms of semantic change in nouns of cognition: A general model. In , J. Coleman & , C. Kay (Eds.), Lexicology, semantics and lexicography (pp. 35–54). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Koleck, M., Bruchon-Schweitzer, M., Cousson-Gelie, F., Gillard, J., & Quintard, B. (2002). The body-image questionnaire: An extension. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 94, 189–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kolstad, V. (1991). Understanding of containment in 5.5 month-old infants. Poster presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle: Washington.Google Scholar
Kopp, C., & Shaperman, J. (1973). Cognitive development in the absence of object manipulation during infancy. Developmental Psychology, 9, 430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koslowski, L., & Cutting, J. (1977). Recognizing the sex of a walker from a dynamic point-light display. Perception & Psychophysics, 21, 575–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemisphere: A computational approach. Psychological Review, 94, 148–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S. (1994). Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Kosslyn, S., Cave, M., Provost, D., & Gierke, S. (1988). Sequential processes in image generation. Cognitive Psychology, 20, 319–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S., DiGirolamo, G., & Thompson, W. (1998). Mental rotation of object versus hands: Neural mechanisms revealed by positron emission tomography. Psychophysiology, 35, 151–61.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kosslyn, S., Thompson, W., Wraga, M., & Alpert, N. (2001). Imagining rotation by endogenous versus exogenuous forces: Distinct neural mechanisms. Neuroreport, 12, 2519–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kotchoubey, B. (2001). About ham and eggs – perception and action, ecology, and neuroscience: A reply to Michaels (2000). Ecological Psychology, 13, 123–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kourtzi, Z., & Kanwisher, N. (2000). Activation in human MT/MST by static images with implied motion. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 48–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kourtzi, Z., & Shiffrar, M. (1997). One-shot view invariance in a moving world. Psychological Science, 8, 461–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kourtzi, Z., & Shiffrar, M. (1999). Dynamic representations of human body movement. Perception, 28, 49–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kovecses, Z. (2000a). Metaphor and emotion: Language, culture and body in human feeling. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kovecses, Z. (2000b). Force and emotion. In , L. Albertazzi (Ed.), Meaning and cognition (pp. 145–68). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Krauss, R. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak?Current Direction in Psychological Science, 7, 54–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R., & Hadar, U. (1999). The role of speech-related arm/hand gestures in word retrieval. In , L. Messing & , R. Campbell (Eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign (pp. 93–116). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kraut, R., Fussell, S., & Siegel, J. (2003). Visual information as a conversational resource in collaborative physical tasks. Human-Computer Interaction, 18, 13–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kraut, R., & Johnston, R. (1979). Social and emotional messages of smiling: An ethological approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 1539–1553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krist, H., Fieberg, E., & Wilkening, F. (1993). Intuitive physics in action and judgment: The development of knowledge about projectile motion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 19, 952–966.Google Scholar
Kugler, P., & Turvey, M. (1987). Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic movement. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P., & Miller, J. (1975). Speech perception by the chinchilla: Voiced-voiceless distinction in alveolar plosive consonants. Science, 190, 69–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P., & Meltzoff, A. (1987). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuzouka, H., Oyama, S., Yamazaki, K., Suzuki, K., & Mitsuishi, M. (2000). GestureMan: A mobile robot that embodies a remote instructor's actions. Proceedings of the CSCW 2000 (pp. 155–62). New York: ACM.
Lachs, K., & Pisoni, D. (2004). Specification of cross-modal source information in isolated kinematic displays of speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 116, 507–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laeng, B., & Teodorescu, D-S. (2002). Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenacts those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science, 26, 207–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things: What our categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1990). The invariance hypothesis: Is abstract reasoning based on image-schemas?Cognitive Linguistics, 1, 39–74.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, G. (1993). The contemporary theory of metaphor. In , A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 202–251). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lakoff, G., & Nunez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Lambek, M., & Strathern, A. (Eds.) (1998). Bodies and persons: Comparative perspectives from Africa and Melanesia. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambie, J., & Marcel, A. (2002). Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: A theoretical framework. Psychological Review, 109, 219–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landau, J., Libkuman, T., & Wildman, J. (2002). Mental simulation inflates performance estimates for physical abilities. Memory & Cognition, 30, 372–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. (1995). The emotion probe: Structure of motivation and attention. American Psychologist, 50, 372–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. (1991). Image, word, and symbol. Berlin: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Larsen, R., Kasimatis, M., & Frey, K. (1992). Facilitating the furrowed brow: An unobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis applied to unpleasant affect. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 321–38.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Leander, K. (2002). Silencing in classroom interaction: Drawing and relating social space. Discourse Processes, 34, 193–235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leder, D. (1990). The absent body. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lederman, S., & Klatzky, R. (1990). Haptic exploration and object representation. In , M. Goodale (Ed.), Vision and action: The control of grasping (pp. 98–109). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Lederman, S., & Klatzky, R. (2003). Feelng surfaces and objects remotely. In , R. Nelson (Ed.), The somatosensory system: Deciphering the brain's own body image (pp. 103–20). New York: C Press.Google Scholar
LeDoux, J. (1998). The emotional brain. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11, 173–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leslie, A. (1988). The necessity of illusion: Perception and thought in infancy. In , L. Weiskrantz (Ed.), Thought without language (pp. 185–210). Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Leslie, A. (1994). ToMM, ToBY, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In , L. Hirschfeld & , S. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind (pp. 119–48). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Leslie, A., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality?Cognition, 25, 265–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levenson, R., Ekman, P., Heider, K., & Friesen, W. (1992). Emotion and autonomic nervous system activity in the Minangkabau of West Sumatra. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 62, 972–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levin, D., & Simons, D. (1997). Failure to detect changes to attended objects in motion pictures. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 501–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levins, J., & Lewontin, R. (1985). The dialectical biologist. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. (1994). Vision, shape, and linguistic description: Tzeltal body-part terminology and object description. Linguistics, 32, 791–855.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lew, A., & Butterworth, G. (1997). The development of hand-mouth coordination in 2- to 5-month-old infants: Similarities with reaching and grasping. Infant Behavior and Development, 20, 59–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewkowicz, D. (1996). Perception of auditory -visual temporal synchrony in human infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 1094–106.Google ScholarPubMed
Leyton, M. (1992). Symmetry, causality, mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Li, L., & Warren, W. (2002). Retinal flow is sufficient for steering during observation. Psychological Science, 13, 485–97.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libby, L. (2003). Imagining perspective and source monitoring in imagination inflation. Memory & Cognition, 31, 1072–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A. (1970). The grammars of speech and language. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 301–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A. (1991). Speech: A special code. In , A. Liberman (Ed.), Learning, development and conceptual change (pp. 121–145). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Liberman, A., Cooper, F., Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A., & Mattingly, I. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revisited. Cognition, 21, 1–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, A., & Mattingly, I. (1989). A specialization for speech perception. Science, 243, 489–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liberman, A., & Whalen, D. (2000). On the relation of speech to language. Trends in Cognitive Science, 4, 187–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B. (1985). Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 8, 529–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liljedahl, P. (2001). Embodied experience of velocity and acceleration: A narrative. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 20, 439–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lloyd, S., Sinha, C., & Freeman, N. (1981). Spatial references systems and the canonicality effect in infant search. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 32, 1–10.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Logie, R. (1995). Visuo-spatial working memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Logie, R., & Marchetti, C. (1991). Visuo-spatial working memory: Visual, spatial, or central executive? In , R. Logie & , M. Denis (Eds.), Mental images in human cognition (pp. 105–15). Oxford: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Logie, R., & Pearson, D. (1997). The inner eye and the inner scribe of visuo-spatial working memory: Evidence for developmental fractionation. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 241–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loring, D., Meador, K., Allison, J., & Wright, J. (2000). Relationship between motion and linguistic activation using fMRI. Neurology, 54, 981–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Low, S. (1994). Embodied metaphors: Nerves as lived experience. In , T. Csordas (Ed.), Embodiment and experience (pp. 139–62). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Luff, P., Heath, C., Kuzuoka, Hi., Hindmarsh, J., Yamazaki, K., & Oyama, S. (2003). Fractured ecologies: Creating environments for collaboration. Human-Computer Interaction, 18, 51–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maalej, Z. (2004). Figurative language in anger expressions in Tunisian Arabic: An extended view of embodiment. Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 51–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mack, A., & Rock, I. (1998). Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
MacNeilage, P. (1975). Preliminaries to the study of single motor unit activity in speech musculature. Journal of Phonetics, 1, 55–71.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, B. (1998). The emergence of language from embodiment. In , B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence of language (pp. 213–56). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Manaster, G., Cleland, C., & Brooks, J. (1978). Emotions as movement in relation to others. Journal of Individual Psychology, 34, 244–53.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1975). Mind and emotion. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Mandler, G. (1984). Mind and body: Psychology of emotion and stress. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Mandler, J. (1992). How to build a baby – 2. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. (1998). Babies think before they speak. Human Development, 41, 116–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, J. (2004). The foundations of mind: Origins of conceptual thoughts. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mandler, J., Bauer, P., & McDonough, L. (1991). Separating the sheep from the goats: Differentiating global categories. Cognitive Psychology, 23, 263–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manusov, V., & Rodriguez, M. (1989). Intentionally based nonverbal messages: A perceiver's perspective. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 15–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, A., & Deitrich, E. (2000). In defense of representation. Cognitive Psychology, 40, 135–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marks, D. (1999). Consciousness, mental imagery, and action. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 567–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marmor, G., & Zaback, L. (1976). Mental rotation by the blind: Does mental rotation depend on visual imagery?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 515–21.Google ScholarPubMed
Marrone, R. (1990). Body of knowledge: An introduction to body/mind psychology. Albany, NY: SUNY Press.Google Scholar
Marschark, M. (1994). Gesture and sign. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 209–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martin, A., Ungerleider, L., & Haxby, J. (2000). Category specificity and the brain: The sensory-motor model of semantic representation of objects. In , M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The new cognitive neurosciences (2nd edition) (pp. 1023–36). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Martin, A., Wiggs, C., Ungerleider, L., & Haxby, J. (1996). Neural correlates of category-specific knowledge. Nature, 379, 649–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, E. (1994). Flexible bodies: Tracking immunity in American culture from the days of polio to the age of AIDS. Boston: Beacon.Google Scholar
Massaro, D. (1987). Speech perception by ear and by eye: A paradigm for psychological inquiry. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Matisoff, J. (1978). Variational semantics in Tibeto-Burman: The organic approach to linguistic comparison. Occasional paper of the Wolfenden Society. Philadelphia: Institute for the Study of Human Issues.Google Scholar
Matlock, T. (2004). Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1389–1400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maturana, H. (1980). Biology of cognition. In , H. Maturana & , F. Varela (Eds.), Autopoiesis and cognition: The realization of the living (pp. 5–58). Boston: Reidel.Google Scholar
Maturana, H. (1983). What it is to see?Archivos de Biologica y Medicina Experimentales, 16, 255–69.Google Scholar
Mayberry, R., & Jacques, J. (2000). Gesture production during stuttered speech: Insights into the nature of gesture-speech integration. In , D. McNeil (Ed.), Language and gestures (pp. 199–214). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCloskey, M., & Kohl, D. (1983). Naive physics: The curvilinear impetus principle and its role in interactions with moving objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 9, 146–56.Google ScholarPubMed
McDonnell, P. (1988). Developmental responses to limb deficiencies and limb replacement. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 42, 120–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGlone, M., & Harding, J. (1998). Back (or forward) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1211–23.Google Scholar
McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeil, D. (1992). Hand and gesture: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Medin, D., Lynch, E., Coley, J., & Atran, S. (1997). Categorization and reasoning among tree experts: Do all roads lead to Rome?Cognitive Psychology, 32, 49–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meier, B., & Robinson, M. (2004). Why the sunny side is up: Associations between affect and vertical position. Psychological Science, 15, 243–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. (1990). Foundations for developing a concept of self: The role of imitation in relating self to other and the value of social mirroring, social modeling, and self practice in infancy. In , D. Cicchetti & , M. Beeghly (Eds.), The self in transition (pp. 139–64). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Borton, R. (1979). Intermodal matching by human noenates. Nature, 282, 403–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A., & Brooks, R. (2001). “Like me” as building block for understanding other minds: Bodily acts, attention, and intention. In , B. Malle, , L. Moses, & , D. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 171–91). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 479–505.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior and Development, 17, 83–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development and Parenting, 6, 179–92.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (2000). (a) Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates (b) Resolving the debate about early imitation. In , D. Muir & , A. Slater (Eds.), Infant development: The essential readings. Essential readings in development psychology (pp. 167–81). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A., & Moore, M. (2001). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates: Resolving the debate about early imitation. In , D. Muir & , A. Slater (Ed.), Infant development: The essential readings (pp. 167–81). Malden, MA: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Merzenich, M., Kaas, J., Wall, J., Nelson, R., Sur, M., & Felleman, D. (1983). Topographic reorganization of somatosensory cortical Area 3B and 1 in adult monkeys following restricted deafferentation. Neuroscience, 8, 33–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metzinger, T. (Ed.) (2000). Neural correlation of consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Michel, G., Camras, L., & Sullivan, J. (1992). Infant interest expressions as coordinative motor structures. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 347–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michotte, A. (1963). The perception of causality. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Miller, J., & Stigler, J. (1991). Meanings of skill: Effect of abacus expertise in number representation. Cognition & Instruction, 8, 29–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, D., & Dyde, R. (2003). Why do some perceptual illusions affect visually guided action, when others don't?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 10–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milner, D., & Goodale, M. (1995). The visual brain in action. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, R., & Gallaher, M. (2001). Embodying music: Matching music and dance as movement. Music Perception, 19, 65–85.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Möller, R. (1999). Perception through anticipation: A behavior-based approach to visual perception. In , A. Riegler, , M., Peschl, & , A. von Stein (Eds.), Understanding representation in the cognitive sciences (pp. 169–76). New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Montessori, M. (1914). Dr. Montessori's own handbook. London: Heineman.Google Scholar
Montpare, J., Goldstein, S., & Clausen, A. (1987). The identification of emotions from gait information. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 1, 33–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrow, D., Bower, G., & Greenspan, S. (1989). Updating situation models during narrative comprehension. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 28, 292–312.Google Scholar
Mulligan, N., & Hornstein, S. (2003). Memory for actions: Self-performed tasks and the reenactment effect. Memory & Cognition, 31, 412–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Munakata, Y., McClelland, J., Johnson, M., & Siegler, R. (1997). Rethinking infant knowledge: Toward an adaptive process account of success and failure on object permanence. Psychological Review, 104, 686–713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, G. (1996). On metaphoric representations. Cognition, 60, 173–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murphy, G. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Murphy, S. (1990). Models of imagery in sports: A review. Journal of Mental Imagery, 89, 216–23.Google Scholar
Murray, C. (2001). The experience of body boundaries by Siamese twins. New Ideas in Psychology, 19, 117–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, I., & Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant's role in mother-infant communication. Journal of Child Language, 13, 15–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, J., Klin, C., & Myers, J. (1993). Forward inference in narrative text. Journal of Memory and Language, 32, 464–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nairne, J., & Walters, V. (1983). Silent mouthing produces modality and suffix-like effects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, 475–483.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nakamura, R., & Mishkin, M. (1980). Chronic blindness following non-visual cortical lesions. Brain Research, 188, 572–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narayanan, S. (1997). Moving right along: A computational model of metaphoric reasoning about events. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, International Computer Science Institute, University of California, Berkeley.
Needham, A. (2001). Object recognition and object segregation in 4.5 month-old infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 3–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Needham, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1993). Intuitions about support in 4.5-month-olds. Cognition, 47, 121–48.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Needham, A., Barrett, T., & Peterman, K. (2002). A pick-me-up for infants' exploratory skills: Early simulated experiences reaching for objects using ‘sticky mittens’ enhances young infants' object exploration skills. Infant Behavior & Development, 25, 279–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1993). The self perceived. In , U. Neisser (Ed.), The perceived self: Ecological and interpersonal sources of self-knowledge (pp. 3–21). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nelson, K., Skwerer, D., Goldman, S., Henseler, S., Presler, N., & Walkenfeld, F. (2003). Entering a community of minds: An experientialist approach to a theory of mind. Human Development, 46, 24–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neruda, P. (1972). The captain's verses. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Neumann, C. (2001). Is metaphor universal? Cross-linguistic evidence from German and Japanese. Metaphor and Symbol, 16, 123–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newcombe, N. (2002). The nativist-empiricist controversy in the context of recent research in spatial and quantitative development. Psychological Science, 13, 395–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newton, N. (1996). Foundations of understanding. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newton, N. (2000). Conscious emotion in a dynamic system: How I can know how I feel. In , R. Ellis & , N. Newton (Eds.), Caldrons of consciousness (pp. 91–108). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Nicolelis, M., & Fanselow, E. (2002). Thalamocortical optimization of tactile processing according to behavioral state. Nature Neuroscience, 5, 517–523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, T. (1963). Volition: A new experimental approach. Scandanavian Journal of Psychology, 4, 225–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nieuwenhuyse, B., Offenberg, L., & Frijda, N. (1987). Subjective emotion and reported body experience. Motivation and Emotion, 11, 169–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noe, A. (2004). Action in perception. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noe, A., & O'Reagan, K. (2002). On the brain-basis of visual consciousness: A sensorimotor account. In , A. Noe & , E. Thompson (Eds.), Vision and mind (pp. 567–98). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noe, A., & Thompson, E. (2004). Are there neural correlates of consciousness?Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11, 3–28.Google Scholar
Nolfi, S., & Floreano, D. (2000). Evolutionary robotics: The biology, intelligence, and technology of self-organizing machines. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nyberg, L., Habib, R., McIntosh, A., & Tulving, E. (2000). Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 11, 120–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nygaard, L., Sommers, M., Mitchell, S., & Pisoni, D. (1994). Speech perception as talker-contingent process. Psychological Science, 5, 42–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oatley, K. (1992). The best laid schemes: The psychology of emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
O'Brien, E., & Albrecht, J. (1992). Comprehension strategies in the development of a mental model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 777–84.Google ScholarPubMed
Ochs, E., Jacoby, S., & Gonzales, P. (1994). Interpretive journeys: How physicists talk and travel through graphic space. Configurations, 2, 157–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Odling-Smee, F. (1988). Niche-constructing phenotypes. In , H. Plotkin (Ed.), The role of behavior in evolution (pp. 73–132). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Ojemann, G. (1994). Cortical stimulation and recording in language. In , A. Kertesz (Ed.). Localization and Neuroimaging in Neuropsychology (pp. 35–55). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ojemann, G., & Mateer, C. (1979). Human language cortex: Localization of memory, syntax, and sequential motor-phoneme identification systems. Science, 205, 1401–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, E. (2003). Personal identity. In , S. Stich & , T. Warfield (Eds.), The Blackwell guide to philosophy of mind (pp. 352–68). New York: Blackwell.Google Scholar
O'Regan, K. (1992). Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46, 461–88.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Regan, K., & Noe, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 939–1031.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Regan, K., Resnick, R., & Clark, J. (1997). Picture changes during blinks: Not seeing where you look and seeing where you don't look. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science, 38, S707.Google Scholar
Oudejams, R., Michaels, C., Bakker, F., & Dolne, M. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of the catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 879–91.Google Scholar
Paillard, J. (1987). Cognitive versus sensorimotor encoding of spatial information. In , P. Ellen & , C. Thinus-Blanc (Eds.), Cognitive processes and spatial orientation in animals and man (pp. 35–54). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Paivio, A. (1986). Mental representations: A dual-coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pandya, V. (1993). Above the forest: A study of Andamanese ethnoemology, cosmology, and the power of ritual. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Papez, J. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 38, 725–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parkinson, B. (1995). Ideas and realities of emotion. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Parsons, L. (1987a). Imagined spatial transformations of one's body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116, 172–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L. (1987b). Imagined spatial transformations of one's hands and feet. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 178–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, L. (1994). Temporal and kinematic properties of motor behavior reflected in mentally simulated action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 709–30.Google ScholarPubMed
Parsons, L., & Fox, P. (1998). The neural basis of implicit movements used in recognising hand shape. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 15, 583–615.Google Scholar
Parsons, L., Fox, P., Downs, J., Glass, T., Hirsch, T., Martin, C., Jerabek, P., & Lancaster, J. (1995). Use of implicit motor imagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET. Nature, 375, 54–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pavlenko, A. (2002). Emotions and the body in Russian and English. Pragmatics & Cognition, 10, 207–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pazzani, M. (1997). Influence of prior knowledge on concept acquisition: Experimental and computational results. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 416–32.Google Scholar
Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Barsalou, L. (2003). Verifying different modality properties for concepts produces switching costs. Psychological Science, 14, 119–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pecher, D., Zeelenberg, R., & Raaijmakers, J. (1998). Does pizza prime coin? Perceptual processing in lexical decision and pronunciation. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 407–18.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pennybaker, J. (1982). The psychology of physical symptoms. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Philippot, P., Rime, B. (1997). The perception of bodily sensations during emotion: A cross-cultural perspective. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 28, 175–88.Google Scholar
Phillips, R., Wagner, S., Fell, C., & Lynch, M. (1990). Do infants recognize emotion in facial expressions? Categorical and metaphorical evidence. Infant Behavior and Development, 13, 71–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, W., Baron-Cohen, S., & Rutter, M. (1992). The role of eye contact in goal detection: Evidence from normal infants and children with autism or mental handicaps. Development & Psychopathology, 4, 375–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1952). The origins of intelligence in childhood. New York: International Universities Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1975). The equilibrium of cognitive structures. Paris: Presses Universitairse de France.Google Scholar
Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Pine, K., & Messer, D. (2003). The development of representations as children learn about balancing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 21, 285–301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Ullman, M. (2002). The past and future of past tense. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 456–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plaut, D. (1995). Double-dissociation without modularity: Evidence from connectionist neuropsychology. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 17, 291–321.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pollio, H., Henley, T., & Thompson, C. (1997). The phenomenology of everyday life. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Port, R., & Gelder, T (Eds.) (1995). Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Premack, D. (1990). The infant's theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Presson, C., & Montello, D. (1994). Updating of rotational and translational body movements: Coordinate structures of perceptual spaces. Perception, 23, 1447–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, S., & Waal, F. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–25.Google ScholarPubMed
Pribram, K. (1991). Brain and perception: Holonomy and studies in figural processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prinz, J. (2002). Furnishing the mind: Concepts and their perceptual basis. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Prinz, J., & Barsalou, L. (2000). Steering a course for embodied representations. In , E. Deitrich & , A, Markman (Eds.), Cognitive dynamics: Conceptual and representational changes in humans and machines (pp. 51–78). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Proffitt, D., Creem, S., & Zosh, W. (2001). Seeing mountains in mole hills: Geographical-slant perception. Psychological Science, 12, 418–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulvermueller, F. (1999). Words in the brain's language. Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 22, 253–336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Putnam, H. (1975). Mind, language, and reality: Philosophical papers, Vol. 2. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, J. (1994). Toward a clarification of spatial processing. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 47A, 465–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quinn, N. (1991). The cultural basis of metaphor. In , J. Fernandez (Ed.), Beyond metaphor: The theory of tropes in anthropology (pp. 56–93). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Radcliff-Brown, A. (1964). The Andaman islanders. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V., & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the brain. London: Fourth Estate.Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V., & Hirstein, W. (1997). Three laws of qualia: What neurology tells us about the biological functions of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 4, 429–57.Google Scholar
Redding, G., & Wallace, B. (1997). Adaptive spatial alignment. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Reed, C., & Farah, M. (1995). The psychological reality of the body schema: A test with normal participants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 334–43.Google ScholarPubMed
Regier, T. (1996). The human semantic potential. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Reisberg, D., Rappaport, I., & O'Shaughnessy, M. (1984). Limits of working memory: The digit span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 203–21.Google Scholar
Reiser, J., Doxey, P., McCarrell, N., & Brooks, P. (1982). Wayfinding and toddlers' use of information from an aerial view of a maze. Developmental Psychology, 18, 714–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiser, J., Lockman, J., & Pick, H. (1980). The role of visual experience in knowledge of spatial layout. Perception and Psychophysics, 28, 185–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiser, J., & Rider, E. (1991). Young children's spatial orientation with respect to multiple targets when walking without vision. Developmental Psychology, 27, 97–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Remez, R., Fellowes, J., & Rubin, P. (1997). Talker identification based on phonetic information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 23, 651–661.Google ScholarPubMed
Repp, B. (1998). Musical motion in perception and performance. In , D. Rosenbaum & , C. Collyer (Eds.), Timing of behavior: Neural, psychological, and computational perspectives (pp. 125–41). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Resnick, A., O'Regan, K., & Clark, J. (1997). To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8, 368–73.Google Scholar
Richardson, D., & Spivey, M. (2000). Representation, space, and Hollywood Squares: Looking at things that aren't there anymore. Cognition, 76, 269–75.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson, D., Spivey, M., Barsalou, L., & McRae, K. (2003). Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science, 27, 767–80.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rieser, J., & Rider, E. (1991). Young children's spatial orientation with respect to multiple targets while walking without vision. Developmental Psychology, 27, 97–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rime, B., Philippot, P., & Cisamolo, D. (1990). Social schemata of peripheral changes in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 38–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G. (1994). Nonconscious motor images. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Arbib, M. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 188–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (1997). Parietal cortex: From sight to action. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 562–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., & Sheliga, B. (1994). Space and selective attention. In , C. Umilta & , M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance 15: Conscious and nonconscious information processing (pp. 232–65). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Rochat, P. (1989). Object manipulation and exploration in 2- to 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 28, 871–84.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rochat, P. (2001). The infant's world. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 532–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E. (1999). Reclaiming concepts. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 61–77.Google Scholar
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. (1975). Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosch, E., Mervis, C., Gray, W., Johnson, M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseblad, B., & Hofsten, C. (1994). Repetitive goal-directed arm movements in children with development coordination: Role of visual information. Adapted Physical Activity, 11, 190–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenbaum, D. (1991). Human motor control. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Rosler, F., Heil, M., & Hennighausen, E. (1995). Distinct cortical activation patterns during long-term memory retrieval of verbal, spatial, and color information. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 51–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, B. (1990). Reminding-based category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 460–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, B. (1999). Postclassification category use: The effects of learning to use categories after learning to classify. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 743–57.Google Scholar
Ross, B., Perkins, S., & Tenpenny, P. (1990). Reminding-based category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 460–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, E., & Mesulam, M-M. (1979). Damaged language functions of the right-hemisphere for prosody and emotional feeling. Archives of Neurology, 36, 144–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rossi, S., Tecchio, F., Pasqualetti, P., Ulivelli, M., Pizzella, V., Romani, G., Passero, S., Battistini, N., & Rossini, P. (2002). Somatosensory processing during movement observation in humans. Clincal Neurophysiology, 113, 16–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rotenberg, K., & Sullivan, C. (2003). Children's use of gaze and limb movement cues to infer deception. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 164, 175–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth, E., & Shoben, E. (1983). The effect of context on the structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 346–78.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothman, G. (1987). Understanding order of movement in youngsters with cerebral palsy. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 65, 391–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rovee-Collier, C., & Hayne, H. (2000). Memory in infancy and early childhood. In , E. Tulving & , F. Craik (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of memory (pp. 267–82). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D. (1980). Schemata: The building blocks of cognition. In , R. Spiro, , B. Bruce, & , W. Brewer (Eds.), Theoretical issues in reading comprehension (pp. 33–58). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rumelhart, D., & McClelland, J. (1986). (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Volume 1 – Foundations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Runson, S., & Frykolm, G. (1981). Visual perception of lifted weights. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 733–40.Google Scholar
Russell, J. (1995). Facial expressions of emotion: What lies beyond minimal universality?Psychological Bulletin, 118, 379–91.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salway, A., & Logie, R. (1995). Visuospatial working memory, movement control, and executive demands. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 253–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sartre, J.-P. (1956). Being and nothingness. New York: Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schank, R. (1982). Dynamic memory. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schank, R., & Abelson, R. (1976). Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Scheper-Hughs, N., & Lock, M. (1987). The mindful body: A prolegomenon to future work in medical anthropology. Medical Anthropology Quarterly, 1, 6–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, K., & Wallbott, H. (1994). Evidence for universality and cultural variation of differential emotion response patterning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 310–28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schladt, M. (1997). Kognitive Strukturen von Korerteilvokabularien in kenianischen Sprachen. Cologne: Institut fur Afrikanistik, University of Cologne.Google Scholar
Schreuder, R., Flores D'Arcais, G., & Glazenborg, G. (1984). Effects of perceptual and conceptual similarity in semantic priming. Psychological Research, 45, 339–354.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, D. (1999). Physical imagery: Kinematic versus dynamical models. Cognitive Psychology, 38, 433–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, D., & Black, T. (1996). Analog imagery in mental model reasoning: Depictive models. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 154–219.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, D., & Black, T. (1999). Inferences through imagined actions: Knowing by simulated doing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 116–36.Google Scholar
Schwartz, D., & Holton, D. (2000). Tool use and the effect of action on the imagination. Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1655–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scott, C., Harris, R., & Rothe, A. (2001). Embodied cognition through improvisation improves memory for dramatic monologue. Discourse Processes, 31, 293–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scruton, R. (1997). The aesthetics of music. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Segal, S., & Fusella, S. (1970). Influences of imaged pictures and sounds on detection of visual and auditory signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 83, 458–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Segrin, C. (1998). Interpersonal communication patterns associated with depression and loneliness. In , P. Anderson & , L. Guerrero (Eds.), Handbook of communication and emotion (pp. 215–42). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Seifert, C., Robertson, S., & Black, J. (1985). Types of inferences generated during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 405–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shallice, T. (1988). From neuropsychology to mental structure. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shannon, B. (1997). What is the functions of consciousness?Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 295–308.Google Scholar
Shannon, B. (2002). The antipodes of the mind: Charting the phenomenology of the Ayahuasca experience. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Sharkey, N., & Sharkey, A. (1987). What is the point of integration? The loci of knowledge-based facilitation in sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 26, 255–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sharkey, N., & Ziemke, T. (1998). A consideration of the biological and psychological foundations of autonomous robotics. Connection Science, 10, 361–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, R. (2001). Processes, acts, and experiences: Three stances on the problem of intentionality. Ecological Psychology, 13, 636–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shaw, R., & Turvey, M. (1999). Ecological foundations of cognition I: Degrees of freedom and conserved quantities in animal-environment systems. In , R. Nunez & , W. Freeman (Eds.), Reclaiming cognition (pp. 111–23). Bowling Green, OH: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Shebilske, W. (1977). Visuomotor coordination in visual direction and position constancies. In , W. Epstein (Ed.), Stability and constancy in visual perception: Mechanisms and processes (pp. 89–112). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1998). Consciousness: A natural history. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5, 260–94.Google Scholar
Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1999). The primacy of movement. Amsterdam: Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. (1984). Ecological constraints on internal representations: Resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking, and dreaming. Psychological Review, 91, 417–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171, 701–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherrington, C. (1906). The integrative action of the nervous system. New York: C. Scribner's Sons.Google Scholar
Shiffrar, M., & Pinto, J. (2002). The visual analysis of bodily motion. In , W. Prinz and , B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 381–400). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shirouzu, H., Miyake, N., & Masukawa, H. (2002). Cognitively active externalization for situated reflection. Cognitive Science, 26, 469–501.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shontz, F. (1969). Perceptual and cognitive aspects of bodily experience. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shore, P., & Repp, B. (1995). Musical motion and performance: Theoretical and empirical perspectives. In , J. Link (Ed.), The practice of performance (pp. 55–83). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Explorations in cultural psychology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Simon, J. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, J., & Ruddell, A. (1967). Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 433–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simons, D., & Chabris, C. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sinclair, H. (1971). Sensorimotor activity patterns as a condition for the acquisition of syntax. In , R. Huxley & , E. Ingram (Eds.), Language acquisition: Models and methods (pp. 121–45). Oxford: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sitskoorn, M., & Smitsman, A. (1991). Infants' visual perception of relative size in and containment and support events. Paper presented at the Biennial Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Minneapolis.
Sitskoorn, M., & Smitsman, A. (1995). Infants' perception of dynamic relations between objects: Passing through or support?Developmental Psychology, 31, 437–47.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slamecka, N., & Graf, P. (1978). The generation effect: Delineation of a phenomenon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4, 592–604.Google Scholar
Smeets, J., & Brenner, E. (1995). Perception and action are based on the same visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 19–31.Google ScholarPubMed
Smets, G., Strappers, P., & Overbeeke, L., & van der Mast, C. (1995). Designing in virtual reality: Perception-action coupling and affordances. In , K. Carr & , R. England (Eds.), Simulated and virtual realities: Elements of perception (pp. 189–208). Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Smith, C. (1981). A search for structure: Selected essays on science, art, and history. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Smith, L., Thelen, E., Titzer, R., & McLin, D. (1999). Knowing in the context of acting: The task dynamics of the A-not-B error. Psychological Review, 106, 235–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smyth, M., & Waller, A. (1998). Movement imagery in rock climbing: Patterns of interference from visual, spatial and kinesthetic secondary tasks. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, 145–57.3.0.CO;2-Z>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Solomon, K. (1997). The spontaneous use of perceptual representations during conceptual processing. Doctoral dissertation. University of Chicago.
Solomon, K., & Barsalou, L. (2001). Representing properties locally. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 129–69.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. (1976). Infants' intermodal perception of events. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 626–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. (1988). When perceiving ends and thinking begins: The apprehension of objects in infancy. In Yonas, Albert (Ed.), Perceptual development in infancy (pp. 197–234). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. (1990). Origins of visual knowledge. In , D. Osherson & , S. Kosslyn (Eds.), Visual cognition and action: An invitation to cognitive science (pp. 92–127). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. (1991). Physical knowledge in infancy: Reflections on Piaget's theory. In , S. Carey & , R. Gelman (Eds.), The epigenesis of mind: Essays on biology and cognition (pp. 133–64). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Spelke, E. (1994). Initial knowledge: Six suggestions. Cognition, 50, 431–45.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E. (1998). Nativism, empiricism, and the origins of knowledge. Infant Behavior & Development, 21, 181–200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E., Breinlinger, K., Macomber, S., & Jacobson, K. (1992). Origins of knowledge. Psychological Review, 99, 605–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spelke, E., & Newport, E. (1998). Nativism, empiricism, and the development of knowledge. In , R. Lerner (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th Edition) (pp. 275–340). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Spelke, E., Philip, A., & Woodward, A. (1995). Infants' knowledge of object motion and human action. In , D. Sperber & , D. Premack (Eds.), Causal cognition (pp. 44–78). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spence, J., Smith, L., & Thelen, E. (2001). Tests of a dynamic systems account of the A-not-B error: The influence of prior experience in the spatial memory abilities of two-year-olds. Child Development, 72, 1327–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperber, D. (2001). In defense of massive modularity. In , E. Dupoux (Ed.), Language, brain, and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 47–57). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sperry, R. (1939). Action current study in movement coordination. Journal of General Psychology, 20, 295–313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spivey, M., & Geng, J. (2001). Oculomotor mechanism activated by imagery and memory: Eye movements to absent objects. Psychology Research, 65, 235–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stampe, D. (1976). Cardinal numeral systems. Chicago Linguistic Society, 12, 594–609.Google Scholar
Stanfield, R., & Zwaan, R. (2001). The effect of implied orientation derived from verbal context on picture recognition. Psychological Science, 12, 153–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steels, L. (1994). Cooperation between distributed agents through self-organization. In , Y. Demazeau & , J.-P. Muller (Eds.), Decentralized AI (pp. 175–96). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Steri, A., Spelke, E., & Rameix, E. (1993). Modality-specific and amodal aspects of object perception in infancy: The case of active touch. Cognition, 47, 251–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stern, D. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, J., Fonlupt, P., Shiffrar, M., & Decety, J. (2000). New aspects of motion perception: Selective neural encoding for apparent human movement. Neuroreport, 11, 109–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stevens, K., & Blumstein, S. (1981). The search for invariant acoustic correlates of phonetic features. In , P. Eimas & , J. Miller (Eds.), Perspectives on the study of speech (pp. 1–38). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Stewart, J. (1983). Perception of animacy. Dissertation Abstracts International, 43, 2376–7.
Stiefehagen, R., Yang, J., & Waibel, A. (2002). Modeling focus of attention for meeting index based on multiple cues. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks, 13, 928–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stigler, J. (1984). Mental abacus: The effect of abacus training on Chinese children's mental calculation. Cognitive Psychology, 16, 145–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stigler, J., Lee, S., & Stevenson, H. (1986). Digit memory in Chinese and English: Evidence for a temporally limited store. Cognition, 23, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoerig, P., & Cowey, A. (1992). Wavelength discrimination in blindsight. Brain, 115, 425–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoffregen, T., Gordan, K., Sheng, Y.-Y., & Flynn, S. (1997). Perceiving affordance for another person's actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 120–36.Google Scholar
Stoller, P. (1989). The taste of ethnographic things: The senses in anthropology. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Stolz, T. (1994). Sprachdynamik. Bochum: Brockmeyer.Google Scholar
Strack, F., Martin, L., & Stepper, S. (1998). Inhibiting and facilitating conditions of the human smile: A nonobtrusive test of the facial feedback hypothesis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 768–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1997). A cognitive theory of meaning. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Strathern, A. (1996). Body thoughts. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1981). The emergence of phonetic structure. Cognition, 10, 301–306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1983). The phoneme as perceptuomotor stimulus. In , A. Allport & , D. MacKay (Eds.), Language perception and production: Relationships between listening, speaking, reading, and writing (pp. 67–84). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and situated action: The problem of human-machine communication. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sudnow, D. (1978). Ways of the hand: The organization of improvised conduct. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Suprenant, A., Pitt, M., & Crowder, R. (1993). Auditory recency in immediate memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 193–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1986). Polysemy vs. abstraction: Mutually exclusive or complementary. In , D. Feder, , M. Niepokuj, , V. Nikforidou, & , M. Van Clay (Eds.), Papers from the Twelfth Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society (pp. 528–38). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistic Society.Google Scholar
Sweetser, E. (1990). From etymology to pragmatics: The mind-body metaphor in semantic structure and semantic change. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. (1988). Force dynamics in language and cognition. Cognitive Science, 12, 49–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. (1996). Fictive motion in language and cognition. In , P. Bloom & , M. Peterson (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 211–276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tanaka, S., & Inui, T. (2002). Cortical movement for action imitation of hand/ arm positions versus finger configurations: An fMRI study. NeuroReport, 13, 1599–1602.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Taub, S. (2001). Language from the body. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thagard, P., & Nerb, J. (2002). Emotional gestalts: Appraisal, change, and the dynamics of affect. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 274–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E. (2000). Grounded in the world: Developmental origins of the embodied mind. Infancy, 1, 3–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. (1994). Dynamic systems approach to development: Applications. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Thelen, E., Schoener, G., Scheier, C., & Smith, L. (2001). The dynamics of embodiment: A field theory of infant perservative reaching. Behavorial and Brain Science, 24, 1–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, A., Bulevich, J., & Loftus, E. (2003). Exploring the role of repetitive and sensory exploration in the imagination inflation effect. Memory & Cognition, 31, 630–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomas, N. (1999). Are theories of imagery theories of imagination? An active perception approach to conscious mental content. Cognitive Science, 23, 207–45.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, E., & Varela, F. (2001). Radical embodiment: Neural dynamics and consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 418–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, E., Palacios, A., & Varela, F. (2002). Ways of coloring: Comparative vision as a case study. In , A. Noe & , E. Thompson (Eds.), Vision and mind (pp. 351–418). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tilley, C. (1994). A phenomenology of landscape. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Tipper, S. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibiting priming by ignored objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 571–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titchner, E. (1909). Lectures on the experimental psychology of the thought processes. New York: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todd, N. (1999). Motion in music: A neurobiological perspective. Music Perception, 17, 115–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (1995). Mapping the evolved functional organization of mind and brain. In , M. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 1185–95). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Tranel, D., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. (1997). A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1319–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Traugott, E., & Dasher, R. (2002). Regularity in semantic change. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C. (1977). The interpersonal world of the infant. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Tucker, M., & Ellis, R. (1998). On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 830–46.Google ScholarPubMed
Turing, A. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59, 433–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Turvey, M., Solomon, H., & Burton, G. (1989). An ecological analysis of knowing by wielding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 387–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ungerleider, L., & Miskin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual systems. In , D. Ingle, , M. Goodale, &. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior (pp. 549–86). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Valenti, S., & Costall, A. (1997). Visual perception of lifted weight from kinematic and static (photographic) displays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 181–98.Google ScholarPubMed
Vallee-Tourangeau, F., Anthony, S., & Austin, N. (1998). Strategies for generating multiple instances of common and ad hoc categories. Memory, 6, 555–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van der Heijden, A., Mussler, J., & Bridgeman, B. (1999). On the perception of position. In , G. Aschersleben, , T. Bachmann, & , J. Musseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (pp. 19–37). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Meer, A., Weel, R., & Lee, D. (1995). The functional significance of arm movements in neonates. Science, 267, 693–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gelder, T. (1998). The dynamical hypothesis in cognitive science. Behavorial and Brain Sciences, 21, 615–65.Google ScholarPubMed
Leeuwen, L., Smitsman, A., & Leeuwen, C. (1994). Affordances, perceptual complexity, and the development of tool use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 174–91.Google ScholarPubMed
Orden, G., Jansen op de Haar, M., & Bosman, A. (1997). Complex dynamic systems also predict dissociations, but they do not reduce to autonomous components. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 131–65.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orden, G., Pennington, B., & Stone, G. (2001). What do double dissociations prove?Cognitive Science, 25, 111–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rooij, D., Bongers, R., & Haselager, W. (2002). A non-representational approach to imagined action. Cognitive Science, 26, 345–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Varela, F. (2002). Upward and downward causation in the brain: Case studies in the emergence and efficacy of consciousness. In , Y. Yasue & , M. Jibu (Eds.), No matter, never mind: Proceedings of toward a science of consciousness (pp. 95–117). Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
Varela, F., & Shear, J. (Eds.) (1999). The view from within. Imprint Academics: Kluwer.Google Scholar
Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vishton, P., Rea, J., Cutting, J., & Nunez, L. (1999). Comparing effects of the horizontal-vertical illusion on grip scaling and judgment: Relative versus absolute, not perception versus action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 1659–72.Google Scholar
Viviani, P. (2002). Motor competence in the perception of dynamic events: A tutorial. In , W. Prinz & , B. Hommel (Eds.), Common mechanisms in perception and action (pp. 406–42). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Viviani, P., Baud-Bovy, G., & Redolfi, M. (1997). Perceiving and tracking kinesthetic stimuli: Further evidence of motor-perception interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 23, 1232–52.Google Scholar
Viviani, P., & Stucchi, N. (1992a). Biological movements look uniform: Evidence of motor-perceptual interactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 18, 603–23.Google Scholar
Viviani, P., & Stucchi, N. (1992b). Motor-perceptual interactions. In , G. Stelmach & , J. Requin (Eds.), Tutorials in motor behavior II (pp. 229–48). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Vogt, S. (1995). On the relations between perceiving, imagining and performing in the learning of cyclical movement sequences. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 191–216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagman, J., & Carello, C. (2001). Affordances and inertial constraints in tool use. Ecological Psychology, 13, 173–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, S., Winner, E., Cicchetti, D., & Gardner, H. (1981). Metaphorical mappings in human infants. Child Development, 52, 728–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, A. (1982). Intermodal perception of expressive behavior by human infants. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33, 514–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallbott, H. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 879–96.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, S.-H., Baillargeon, R., & Brueckner, L. (2004). Young infants' reasoning about hidden objects: Evidence from violation-of-expectation tasks with test trials only. Cognition, 93, 167–98.
Warren, W. (1984). Perceiving affordances: Visual guidance of stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 683–703.Google ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E., & McCarthy, R. (1987). Categories of knowledge: Further fractionations and an attempted integration. Brain, 110, 1273–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Warrington, E., & Shallice, T. (1984). Category-specific semantic impairment. Brain, 107, 829–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Washburn, M. (1916). Movement and mental imagery. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Wegner, D. (2002). The illusion of conscious will. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Wegner, D., & Wheatley, T. (1999). Apparent mental causation: Sources of the experience of will. American Psychologist, 54, 480–492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L. (1980). Varieties of residual experience. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 365–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L., Warrington, E., Sanders, M., & Marshall, J. (1974). Visual capacity in the hemianopic field following restricted occipital ablation. Brain, 97, 709–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welch, R. (1978). Perceptual modification: Adapting to altered sensory environments. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Werner, A. (1904). Note on the terms used for “right hand” and “lefthand” in the Bantu languages. Journal of the African Society, 13, 112–16.Google Scholar
Wexler, M., Kosslyn, S., & Berthoz, A. (1998). Motor processes in mental rotation. Cognition, 68, 77–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wheeler, M., Peterson, S., & Buckner, R. (2000). Memory's echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 97, 11, 125–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, P. (1999). Toward a causal realist account of causal understanding. American Journal of Psychology, 112, 605–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures: Diversity and universals. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, P. (2001). Metaphor in American sign language. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson, M. (2001). The case for sensorimotor coding in working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 8, 49–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M., & Emmorey, K. (1997). A visuospatial phonological loop in working memory: Evidence from American Sign Language. Memory & Cognition, 25, 313–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, M., Iverson, A., & Emmorey, K. (2000). Further investigation of the phonological similarity effect for sign language: Two effects of spatial similarity. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Wilson, N., & Gibbs, R. (2005). Real and imagined body movement primes metaphor comprehension. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Wilson, R. (2004). Boundaries of the mind: The individual in the fragile sciences – cognition. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. (2002). Strangers to ourselves: Discovering the adaptive unconscious. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winkler, C. (1994). Rape trauma: Contexts of meaning. in , T. Csordas (Ed.), Embodiment and experience (pp. 248–68). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wohlschlager, S., & Wohlschlager, A. (1998). Mental and manual rotation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 397–412.Google ScholarPubMed
Wolff, P. (1999). Space perception and intended action. In , G. Aschersleben, , T. Bachmann & , J. Musseler (Eds.), Cognitive contributions to the perception of spatial and temporal events (pp. 43–630). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Wolff, P., & Levin, J. (1972). The role of overt activity in children's imagery production. Child Development, 43, 537–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. (1999). Infants' abilities to distinguish between purposeful and non-purposeful behaviors. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 145–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, T. (2001). Karen in motion: The role of physical enactment in developing an understanding of distance, time, and speed. Mathematical Behavior, 20, 145–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wu, L., & Barsalou, L. (2001). Grounding concepts in perceptual simulations: I. Evidence from property generation. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Xu, F., & Carey, S. (1996). Infants' metaphysics: The case of numerical identity. Cognitive Psychology, 30, 111–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yarbus, A. (1965). Role of eye movements in the visual process. Oxford: Nauka.
Yu, N. (1999). The contemporary theory of metaphor: Perspectives from Chinese. Amsterdam: Benajmins.Google Scholar
Yu, N. (2003). Chinese metaphors of thinking. Cognitive Linguistics, 14, 141–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zabalia, M. (2002). Action and mental imagery in children. Anne Psychologique, 102, 409–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zajonc, R., Murphy, S., Inglehart, M. (1989). Feeling and facial efference: Implication of the vascular theory of emotion. Psychological Review, 96, 395–416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ziemke, T. (1999). Rethinking grounding. In , A. Riegler, , M. Peschl, & , A. von Stein (Eds.), Understanding representation in the cognitive sciences (pp. 177–90). New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Zimler, J., & Keenan, J. (1983). Imagery in the congenitally blind: How visual are visual images?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 269–82.Google ScholarPubMed
Zubin, D., & Choi, S. (1984). Orientation and gestalt: Conceptual organizing principles in the lexicalization of space. In , D. Testen, , V. Misha, & , J. Drogo (Eds.), Papers from the parasession of lexical semantics (pp. 333–45). Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R. (1996). Processing narrative time shifts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 22, 1196–1207.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R., Magliano, J., & Graesser, A. (1995). Dimensions of situated model construction in narrative comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 386–97.Google Scholar
Zwaan, R., Stanfield, R., & Yaley, R. (2002). Language comprehenders mentally represent the shapes of objects. Psychological Science, 13, 168–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr
  • Book: Embodiment and Cognitive Science
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805844.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr
  • Book: Embodiment and Cognitive Science
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805844.010
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr
  • Book: Embodiment and Cognitive Science
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511805844.010
Available formats
×