Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-t6hkb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-12T05:42:25.336Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Re-setting the basis of articulation in the acquisition of new languages: A third language case study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 September 2012

Björn Hammarberg
Affiliation:
Stockholm University
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

It has long been recognised that languages differ phonetically not only in their distinctive segments and prosodic features, but also in the characteristic ways in which the phonetic gestures are ‘set’, i.e. the Artikulationsbasis, articulatory settings (Honikman 1964; Laver 1980), or phonetic settings (Laver 1994). The discussion of Artikulationsbasis has a long history in the phonetic literature, especially from the point of view of the overall characterisation and contrastive description of the pronunciation of different languages (see Kelz 1971; Laver 1978; Jenner 2001 for historical accounts). Not least the great nineteenth-century phoneticians, such as Sievers, Viëtor, Sweet and Jespersen, emphasised and tried to portray cross-language differences concerning basis of articulation. In recent literature in English, the term articulatory settings (introduced by Honikman 1964) is widely used. Laver (1980, 1994) gives an extensive account of various dimensions and values of settings as features of people's habitual voice quality. Although he is primarily concerned with voice phenomena in the individual speaker, he also points out the relevance of settings for the characterisation of specific languages or language varieties (Laver 1994: 423ff). Regional variation of dialects, too, is characterised in part by differences in voice quality, an aspect which Elert has applied to Swedish dialect research (Elert 1984; Elert and Br. Hammarberg 1991). Likewise, voice can be an aspect of sociolectal variation (Esling 1978a, 1978b), and can function as a social marker of the speaker (Trudgill 1974; Laver and Trudgill 1979).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Edinburgh University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×