Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-sjtt6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-02T08:19:11.870Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Empty categories

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Andrew Radford
Affiliation:
University of Essex
Get access

Summary

So far, our discussion of syntactic structure has tacitly assumed that all constituents in a given structure are overt. However, we now turn to argue that syntactic structures may also contain empty (= covert = null) categories – i.e. categories which have no overt phonetic form, and hence which are inaudible or silent. As we shall see, empty categories play a central role in the theory of grammar which we are outlining here.

We begin by looking at clauses which might be argued to contain an empty subject. In this connection, compare the structure of the bracketed infinitive clauses in the (a) and (b) examples below:

  1. (1) (a) We would like [you to stay]

  2. (b) We would like [to stay]

  1. (2) (a) We don't want [anyone to upset them]

  2. (b) We don't want [to upset them]

Each of the bracketed infinitive complement clauses in the (a) examples in (1–2) contains an overt (italicized) subject. By contrast, the bracketed complement clauses in the (b) examples appear to be subjectless. However, we shall argue that apparently subjectless infinitive clauses contain an understood null subject. (By saying that a constituent is null or empty or covert, we mean that it has no overt phonetic form and so is silent.) The kind of null subject found in the bracketed clauses in the (b) examples has much the same grammatical and referential properties as pronouns, and hence is conventionally designated as PRO.

Type
Chapter
Information
Syntax
A Minimalist Introduction
, pp. 82 - 105
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1997

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Empty categories
  • Andrew Radford, University of Essex
  • Book: Syntax
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166898.005
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Empty categories
  • Andrew Radford, University of Essex
  • Book: Syntax
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166898.005
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Empty categories
  • Andrew Radford, University of Essex
  • Book: Syntax
  • Online publication: 05 June 2012
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166898.005
Available formats
×