Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-5wvtr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-16T14:46:38.388Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

CHAPTER XI - THE PRESENT POSITION (1924)

from PART II - CORRELATION

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2016

Get access

Summary

IT appears desirable to utilise the remaining available pages for a statement of the present position of the controversy which forms the subject of the two preceding chapters, especially as in discussion points of difference are apt to be magnified and points of agreement lost sight of.

The term “general intelligence” or “general ability” is liable to have two distinct meanings. On the one hand it may be a statement of a fact, on the other an explanation of that fact. The fact which makes the term a necessary and a useful one is that a man who is good at one kind of mental work is usually above the average in others. In technical language, most measures of correlation between various mental tests, or between various school and university subjects, are positive, and many are high. Though some are low, few are negative. When this is denied, it is generally on the strength of a number of individual cases where marked ability is found in one subject but not in another. These are, however, swamped by the much larger number of cases in agreement with the principle. Because of this fact of predominant positive correlation, it is possible, after administering an intelligence test lasting one or two hours, to predict an individual's performance in various mental activities with more or less probability, though never, of course, with absolute certainty. If the known correlation between the test and a certain other activity is r, then an individual who deviates d from the average in the test (in sigma units) will deviate rd from the average in that activity most probably. In practice however such individuals who deviate d in the test will not all be exactly at rd in the other activity, but will be scattered about it. And that scatter will be less than the scatter of an unselected group in the proportion k : 1, where k= √ (l — r2). The test by its constituent elements probes the mind at a number of different points and strikes an average, just as one finds the depth of a lake by plumbing it at various points.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×