Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-26T04:00:55.544Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false
This chapter is part of a book that is no longer available to purchase from Cambridge Core

31 - Exceptional Memory

from PART V.C - GAMES AND OTHER TYPES OF EXPERTISE

John M. Wilding
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London
Elizabeth R. Valentine
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Royal Holloway, University of London
K. Anders Ericsson
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Neil Charness
Affiliation:
Florida State University
Paul J. Feltovich
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Robert R. Hoffman
Affiliation:
University of West Florida
Get access

Summary

Historical Introduction

Interest in improving methods of memorizing has a long history. Before the widespread use of writing, poets and storytellers devised methods of making memorization and recall easier, using rhythm, imagery, and formulaic descriptions (Rubin, 1995). Non-experts, too, had to rely far more heavily on oral memory than in modern times.

Today equally impressive feats of memory are demonstrated regularly by experts in specific fields of knowledge (chess, music, medicine, literature, law, football, etc.), which embody far more information than was available to our ancestors. As with the poets and storytellers of an earlier age, long and varied exposure, together with motivation to achieve mastery, have enabled these experts to build up a body of information that makes new additions easier by embedding them in an already organized structure, often without conscious effort. Such an organized database is known as semantic memory and depends on the progressive addition and organization of material acquired over a lengthy period.

The focus of this chapter, however, is on individuals who display an unusual ability to memorize information, especially ability to memorize types of information that present particular difficulty for the majority, such as lists of numbers with no inherent structure. This form of expertise involves episodic memory, the ability to reproduce a prior input, event, or episode. Superior ability to retain episodic information will undoubtedly facilitate the development of semantic memory, but is probably not essential to it.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Anschutz, L., Camp, C. L., Markley, R. P., & and Kramer, J. J. (1985). Maintenance and generalization of mnemonics for grocery shopping by older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 11, 157–160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, P. B., & Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of limits of cognitive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psychology, 28, 121–125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartlett, F. C. (1932). Remembering. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Biederman, I., Cooper, E. E., Mahadevan, R. S., & Fox, P. W. (1992). Unexceptional spatial memory in an exceptional memorist. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 18, 654–657.Google Scholar
Binet, A. (1894). Psychologie des Grands Calculateurs et Jouers d'Échecs. Paris: Libraire Hachette.Google Scholar
Camp, C. J. (1998). Memory interventions for normal and pathological older adults. Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 18, 155–189.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor Analytic Studies. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chase, W. G., & Ericsson, K. A. (1981). Skilled memory. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive Skills and Their Acquisition (pp. 141–189). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. (1985). Memory skill. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 188–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., & Chase, W. G. (1982). Exceptional memory. American Scientist, 70, 607–615.Google ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., Chase, W. G., & Faloon, S. (1980). Acquisition of memory skill. Science, 208, 1181–1182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Delaney, P. F., Weaver, G., & Mahadevan, R. (2004). Uncovering the structure of a memorist's “basic” memory capacity: Toward a methodology for resolving theoretical issues in the study of exceptional memory. Unpublished paper.
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–245.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (2000). Shortcomings of generic retrieval structures with slots of the type that Gobet (1993) proposed and modelled. British Journal of Psychology, 91, 571–590.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Polson, P. G. (1988). A cognitive analysis of exceptional memory for restaurant orders. In Chi, M., Glaser, R., & Farr, M. (Eds.), The Nature of Expertise (pp. 23–70). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gordon, P., Valentine, E., & Wilding, J. (1984). One man's memory: A study of a mnemonist. British Journal of Psychology, 75, 1–14.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, R. H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (1998). Cortical plasticity associated with Braille learning. Trends in Cognitive Science, 2, 168–174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunt, E. B., & Love, T. (1972). How good can memory be? In Melton, A. W. & Martin, E. (Eds.), Coding Processes in Human Memory (pp. 237–250). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hunter, I. M. L. (1962). An exceptional talent for calculative thinking. British Journal of Psychology, 53, 243–258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hunter, I. M. L. (1977). An exceptional memory. British Journal of Psychology, 68, 155–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunter, I. M. L. (1996). Remembering a story after 26 years. Unpublished paper.
Ingham, J. G. (1952). Memory and intelligence. British Journal of Psychology, 43, 20–32.Google Scholar
Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1989). Testing-the-limits and the study of adult age differences in cognitive plasticity of a mnemonic skill. Developmental Psychology, 25, 247–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kliegl, R., Smith, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1990). On the locus and process of magnification of age differences during mnemonic training. Developmental Psychology, 26, 894–904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kliegl, R., Smith, J., Heckhausen, J., & Baltes, P. B. (1987). Mnemonic training for the acquisition of skilled digit memory. Cognition and Instruction, 4, 203–223.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalakoski, V., & Saariluoma, P. (2001). Taxi drivers' exceptional memory of street names. Memory and Cognition, 29, 634–638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luria, A. R. (1975). The Mind of a Mnemonist. Harmondsworth, Middlesex: Penguin.Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., John, I. S. srude, Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97, 4398–4403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maguire, E. A., Valentine, E. R., Wilding, J. M., & Kapur, N. (2003a). Routes to remembering: The brains behind superior memory. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 90–95.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Spiers, H. J., Good, C. D., Hartley, T., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Burgess, N. (2003b). Navigation expertise and the human hippocampus: A structural brain imaging analysis. Hippocampus, 13, 250–259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mastropieri, M. A., & Scruggs, T. E. (1989). Constructing more meaningful relationships: Mnemonic instruction for special populations. Educational Psychology Review, 1, 83–111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muller, G. (1911). Zur Analyse der Gedächtnistätigkeit und des Vorstellungsverlaufes. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, Erganzungsband, 5, 7–367.Google Scholar
Muller, G. (1913). Neue Versuche mit Rückle. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 7, 193–213.Google Scholar
Neely, A. S., & Bäckman, L. (1995). Effects of multifactorial memory training in old age: Generalizability across tasks and individuals. Journal of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 50B, P134–P140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nyberg, L, Sandblom, J., Jones, S., Neely, A. S., Petersson, K. M., Ingvar, M., & Bäckman, L. (2003). Neural correlates of training-related memory improvement in adulthood and aging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100, No. 23, 13728–13733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Keefe, J., & Nadel, L. (1978). The Hippocampus as a Cognitive Map. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rabbitt, P. M. A. (1993). Does it all go together when it goes? The Nineteenth Bartlett Memorial Lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46A, 385–434.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rubin, D. C. (1995). Memory in Oral Traditions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1985). A Theory of Cognitive Aging. Amsterdam: North Holland.Google Scholar
Schlaug, G., Jancke, L., Huang, Y. X., Staiger, J. F., & Steinmetz, H. (1995). Increased corpus-callosum size in musicians. Neuropsychologia, 33, 1047–1055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stigsdotter, A., & Bäckman, L. (1989). Multifactorial memory training with older adults: How to foster maintenance of improved performance. Gerontology, 35, 260–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neely, Stigsdotter A., & Bäckman, L. (1993a). Maintenance of gains following multifactorial and unifactorial memory training in late adulthood. Educational Gerontology, 19, 105–117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neely, Stigsdotter A., & Bäckman, L. (1993b). Long-term maintenance of gains from memory training in older adults: Two 3½ year follow-up studies. Journal of Gerontology, 48, 233–237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Susukita, T. (1933). Untersuchung eines ausserordentlichen Gedächtnisses in Japan (I). Tohoku Psychologica Folia, 1, 1–134.Google Scholar
Susukita, T. (1934). Untersuchung eines ausserordentlichen Gedächtnisses in Japan (II). Tohoku Psychologica Folia, 2, 15–42.Google Scholar
Tanaka, S., Michimata, C., Kaminaya, T., Honda, M., & Sadato, N. (2002). Superior digit memory of abacus experts: An event-related functional MRI study. Neuroreport, 13, 2187–2191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. P., Cowan, T., & Frieman, J. (1993). Memory Search by a Memorist. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., & Marcoen, A. (1996). On the mechanisms of plasticity in young and older adults after instruction in the method of loci: Evidence for an amplification model. Psychology and Aging, 11, 164–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verhaeghen, P., Marcoen, A., & Goossens, L. (1992). Improving memory performance in the aged through mnemonic training – A meta-analytic study. Psychology of Aging, 7, 242–251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilding, J., & Marshall, P. (2004). Superior digit span: Validation of reported strategies by the time structure of recall. Unpublished paper.
Wilding, J., & Valentine, E. (1985). One man's memory for prose, faces and names. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 215–219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilding, J., & Valentine, E. (1994a). Memory champions. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 231–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilding, J., & Valentine, E. (1994b). Mnemonic wizardry and the telephone directory – But stories are another story. British Journal of Psychology, 85, 501–509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilding, J., & Valentine, E. (1997). Superior Memory. Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Wilding, J., Valentine, E., Marshall, P., & Cook, S. (1999). Individual differences in memory ability and GCSE performance. Educational Psychology, 19, 117–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yesavage, J. A., & Rose, T. L. (1983). Concentration and mnemonic training in the elderly with memory complaints: A study of combined therapy and order effects. Psychiatry Research, 9, 157–167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yesavage, J. A., Rose, T. L., & Bower, G. H. (1983). Interactive imagery and affective judgments improve face-name learning in the elderly. Journal of Gerontology, 38, 197–203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×