Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-21T02:22:43.021Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Section I - What is Implicit Bias and (How) Can We Measure It?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  aN Invalid Date NaN

Jon A. Krosnick
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Tobias H. Stark
Affiliation:
Utrecht University, The Netherlands
Amanda L. Scott
Affiliation:
The Strategy Team, Columbus, Ohio
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Strauts, E., et al. (2015). Toward a meaningful metric of implicit prejudice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(5), 14681481. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Pearson, A. R. (2017). Aversive racism and contemporary bias. In Sibley, C. G. & Barlow, F. K. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice (pp. 267294). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (pp. 75109). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In Dovidio, J. F. & Gaertner, S. L. (Eds.), Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism (pp. 6189). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, G., & Tetlock, P. E. (2017). Popularity as a poor proxy for utility. In Psychological Science Under Scrutiny: Recent Challenges and Proposed Solutions (pp. 164195). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119095910.ch10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2008). Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes: The perspective of the MODE model. In Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H., & Briñol, P. (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the New Implicit Measures (pp. 1963). Psychology Press.Google Scholar

References

Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2006). See what you want to see: The impact of motivational states on visual perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 612625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Balcetis, E., & Dunning, D. (2010). Wishful seeing: More desired objects are seen as closer. Psychological Science, 21, 147152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., et al. (1998). Ego-Depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 12521265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. (1957). On perceptual readiness. Psychological Review, 64, 123152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. S., & Goodman, C. C. (1947). Value and need as organizing factors in perception. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 42, 3344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, C. D., Payne, B. K., & Knobe, J. (2010). Do theories of implicit race bias change moral judgments? Social Justice Research, 23, 272289.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craske, M. G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., et al. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Czarnecki, (2018, July 6). Timeline of a crisis: Starbucks’ racial bias training. PRWeek. Retrieved from www.prweek.com/article/1486260/timeline-crisis-starbucks-racial-bias-trainingGoogle Scholar
Daumeyer, N. M., Onyeador, I. N., Brown, X., et al. (2019). Consequences of attributing discrimination to implicit vs. explicit bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 84, 133146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunton, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 316326.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H. (1990). Multiple processes by which attitudes guide behavior: The MODE model as an integrative framework. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 23, pp. 75109). San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object-evaluation associations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25, 664703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., et al. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 10131027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2014). The MODE model: Attitude-behavior processes as a function of motivation and opportunity. In Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual Process Theories of the Social Mind. New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 155171.Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R., & Powell, M. C. (1994). Attitudes, perception, and attention. In Niedenthal, P. M. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.), The Heart’s Eye: Emotional Influences in Perception and Attention. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 197216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Sanbonmatsu, D. M., Powell, M. C., et al. (1986). On the automatic activation of attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., & Williams, C. J. (1986). Attitude accessibility as a moderator of the attitude-perception and attitude-behavior relations: An investigation of the 1984 presidential election. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 505514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forscher, P. S., Mitamura, C., Dix, E. L., et al. (2017). Breaking the prejudice habit: Mechanisms, time course, and longevity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 133146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B. (2019). Six lessons for a cogent science of implicit bias and its criticism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14, 574595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenn, N. D. (1998). The course of marital success and failure in five American 10-year marriage cohorts. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 60, 569576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Lai, C. K. (2020). Implicit social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 20, 419445.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1997). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 64, 970986.Google Scholar
Hahn, A., Judd, C. M., Hirsh, H. K., et al. (2014). Awareness of implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 13691392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, H. A., Czellar, S., Olson, M. A., et al. (2010). Malleability of attitudes or malleability of the IAT? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 286298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Han, H. A., Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2006). The influence of experimentally-created extrapersonal associations on the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 259272.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hastorf, A. H., & Cantril, H. (1954). They saw a game: A case study. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 49, 129134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hehman, E., Flake, J. K., & Calanchini, J. (2017). Disproportionate use of lethal force in policing associated with regional racial biases of residents. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 393401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Higgins, E. T. (1996). Knowledge activation: Accessibility, applicability, and salience. In Higgins, E. T. & Kruglanski, A. W. (Eds.), Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles. New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 133168.Google Scholar
Higgins, E. T., Rholes, W. S., & Jones, C. R. (1977). Category accessibility and impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 13, 141154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., & Friese, M. (2008). Impulses got the better of me: Alcohol moderates the influence of implicit attitudes toward food cues on eating behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117, 420427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Rauch, W., & Gawronski, B. (2007). And deplete us not into temptation: Automatic attitudes, dietary restraint, and self-regulatory resources as determinants of eating behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 497504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Houston, D. A., & Fazio, R. H. (1989). Biased processing as a function of attitude accessibility: Making objective judgments subjectively. Social Cognition, 7, 5166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jamieson, D. W., & Zanna, M. P. (1989). Need for structure in attitude formation and expression. In Pratkanis, A. R., Breckler, S. J., & Greenwald, A. G. (Eds.), Attitude Structure and Function. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, pp. 383406.Google Scholar
Koole, S. L., Dijksterhuis, A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2001). What’s in a name: Implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 669685.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruglanski, A. W., & Freund, T. (1983). The freezing and unfreezing of lay-inferences: Effects on impressional primacy, ethnic stereotyping, and numerical anchoring. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 19, 448468.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, S., Rogge, R. D., & Reis, H. T. (2010). Assessing the seeds of relationship decay: Using implicit evaluations to detect the early stages of disillusionment. Psychological Science, 21, 857864.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loersch, C., Bartholow, B. D., Manning, M., et al. (2015). Intoxicated prejudice: The impact of alcohol consumption on implicitly and explicitly measured racial attitudes. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 18, 256268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruction of automobile destruction: An example of the interaction between language and memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585589.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lord, C. G., Ross, L., & Lepper, M. R. (1979). Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidence. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 37, 20982109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563579.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConnell, A. R., & Rydell, R. J. (2014). The systems of evaluation model: A dual-systems approach to attitudes. In Sherman, J., Gawronski, B., & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual Process Theories of the Social Mind. New York, NY: Guilford, pp. 204217.Google Scholar
McNulty, J. K., Olson, M. A., Meltzer, A. L., et al. (2013). Though they may be unaware, newlyweds implicitly know whether their marriage will be satisfying. Science, 342, 11191120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004a). Trait inferences as a function of automatically-activated racial attitudes and motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 26, 111.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004b). Reducing the influence of extrapersonal associations on the Implicit Association Test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 653667.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2007). Discordant evaluations of Blacks affect nonverbal behavior. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 33, 12141224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2009). Implicit and explicit measures of attitudes: The perspective of the MODE model. In Petty, R. E., Fazio, R. H., & Briñol, P. (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the New Implicit Measures. New York, NY: Psychology Press, pp. 1963.Google Scholar
Olson, M. A., Fazio, R. H., & Hermann, A. D. (2007). Reporting tendencies underlie discrepancies between implicit and explicit measures of self-esteem. Psychological Science, 18, 287291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Lundberg, K. B. (2017). The bias of crowds: How implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 233248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 811832.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ranganath, K. A., Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Distinguishing automatic and controlled components of attitudes from direct and indirect measurement methods. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 386396.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redford, L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2016). Perceived moral responsibility for attitude-based discrimination. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55, 279296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richardson-Klavehn, A., & Bjork, R. A. (1988). Measures of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 39, 475543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roediger, H. L. (1990). Implicit memory: Retention without remembering. American Psychologist, 45, 10431056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roskos-Ewoldsen, D. R., & Fazio, R. H. (1992). On the orienting value of attitudes: Attitude accessibility as a determinant of an object’s attraction of visual attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 198211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sanbonmatsu, D. M., & Fazio, R. H. (1990). The role of attitudes in memory-based decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 614622.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501518.Google Scholar
Schuette, R. A., & Fazio, R. H. (1995). Attitude accessibility and motivation as determinants of biased processing: A test of the MODE model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 704710.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stone, J., Moskowitz, G. B., Zestcott, C. A., et al. (2020). Testing active learning workshops for reducing implicit stereotyping of Hispanics by majority and minority group medical students. Stigma and Health, 5, 94103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tang, T. (2018, May 28). Experts: Starbucks training a first step in confronting bias. Associated Press. Retrieved from: www.apnews.com/a7afd1dc61ae4481a1ed9f5e67de1259Google Scholar
Towles-Schwen, T., & Fazio, R. H. (2003). Choosing social situations: The relation between automatically-activated racial attitudes and anticipated comfort interacting with African Americans. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 170182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Towles-Schwen, T., & Fazio, R.H. (2006). Automatically activated racial attitudes as predictors of the success of interracial roommate relationships. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 698705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vasey, M. W., Harbaugh, C. N., Buffington, A. G et al. (2012). Predicting return of fear following exposure therapy with an implicit measure of attitudes. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 50, 767774.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wegener, D. T., & Petty, R. E. (1995). Flexible correction processes in social judgment: The role of naive theories in corrections for perceived bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 3651.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M.C., et al. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104, 77807785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A. I., & Fazio, R. H. (2013). Attitude accessibility as a determinant of object construal and evaluation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 404418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888918.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axt, J. R., Conway, M. A., Westgate, E. C., et al. (2021). Implicit transgender attitudes independently predict beliefs about gender and transgender people. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 47(2), 257274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banaji, M. R. (2004). The opposite of a great truth is also true: Homage to koan #7. In Jost, J. T., Banaji, M. R., & Prentice, D. A. (Eds.), Perspectivism in Social Psychology: The Yin and Yang of Scientific Progress. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 127140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bar-Anan, Y., & Nosek, B. A. (2014). A comparative investigation of seven indirect attitude measures. Behavior Research Methods, 46(3), 668688.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar-Anan, Y., & Vianello, M. (2018). A multi-method multi-trait test of the dual-attitude perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(8), 12641272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bianchi, E. C., Hall, E. V., & Lee, S. (2018). Reexamining the link between economic downturns and racial antipathy: Evidence that prejudice against Blacks rises in recessions. Psychological Science, 29(10), 15841597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, I. V. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6(3), 242261.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carleton, R. N., Norton, M. P. J., & Asmundson, G. J. (2007). Fearing the unknown: A short version of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21(1), 105117.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charlesworth, T. E., & Banaji, M. R. (2019). Patterns of implicit and explicit attitudes: I. Long term change and stability from 2007 to 2016. Psychological Science, 30, 174192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chin, M. J., Quinn, D. M., Dhaliwal, T. K., et al. (2020). Bias in the air: A nationwide exploration of teachers’ implicit racial attitudes, aggregate bias, and student outcomes. Educational Researcher, 49(8), 566578.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Connor, P., & Evers, E. R. (2020). The bias of individuals (in crowds): Why implicit bias is probably a noisily measured individual-level construct. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(6), 13291345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corneille, O., & Hütter, M. (2020). Implicit? What do you mean? A comprehensive review of the delusive implicitness construct in attitude research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 24(3), 212232.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cummins, J., Lindgren, K. P., & De Houwer, J. (2021). On the role of (implicit) drinking self identity in alcohol use and problematic drinking: A comparison of five measures. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 35(4), 458471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, 12(2), 163170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daumeyer, N. M., Onyeador, I. N., Brown, X., et al. (2019). Consequences of attributing discrimination to implicit vs. explicit bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 84, 103812.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, J. (2006). What are implicit measures and why are we using them. In Wiers, R. W. & Stacy, A. W. (Eds.), The Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, pp. 1128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, J. (2014). A propositional model of implicit evaluation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8(7), 342353.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Houwer, J. (2019). Implicit bias is behavior: A functional-cognitive perspective on implicit bias. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(5), 835840.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dentale, F., Vecchione, M., Ghezzi, V., et al. (2020). Beyond an associative conception of automatic self-evaluation: Applying the Relational Responding Task to measure self-esteem. The Psychological Record, 70(2), 227242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. A., Schaller, M., & Park, J. H. (2009). Perceived vulnerability to disease: Development and validation of a 15-item self-report instrument. Personality and Individual differences, 47(6), 541546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H. (2007). Attitudes as object–evaluation associations of varying strength. Social Cognition, 25(5), 603637.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., et al. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(6), 10131027.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fazio, R. H., & Zanna, M. P. (1981). Direct experience and attitude–behavior consistency. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 14, pp. 161202). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Friese, M., Hofmann, W., & Schmitt, M. (2008). When and why do implicit measures predict behaviour? Empirical evidence for the moderating role of opportunity, motivation, and process reliance. European Review of Social Psychology, 19, 285338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friese, M., Smith, C. T., Plischke, T., et al. (2012). Do implicit attitudes predict actual voting behavior particularly for undecided voters?. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e44130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawronski, B. (2019). Six lessons for a cogent science of implicit bias and its criticism. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 14(4), 574595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132(5), 692.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2011). The associative–propositional evaluation model: Theory, evidence, and open questions. In Gawronski, B. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 44, pp. 59127). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gawronski, B., Hofmann, W., & Wilbur, C. J. (2006). Are “implicit” attitudes unconscious? Consciousness and Cognition, 15(3), 485499.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., Morrison, M., Phills, C. E., et al. (2017). Temporal stability of implicit and explicit measures: A longitudinal analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43(3), 300312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gladwell, M. (2005). Blink: The Power of Thinking Without Thinking. New York, NY: Little Brown & Co.Google Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102(1), 427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Statistically small effects of the Implicit Association Test can have societally large effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(4), 553561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., & Lai, C. K. (2020). Implicit social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 71(1), 419445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(6), 14641480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., et al. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 1741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Smith, C. T., Sriram, N., et al. (2009). Implicit race attitudes predicted vote in the 2008 US presidential election. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 9(1), 241253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, A., & Gawronski, B. (2018). Implicit social cognition. Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience, 4, 133.Google Scholar
Hahn, A., & Gawronski, B. (2019). Facing one’s implicit biases: From awareness to acknowledgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(5), 769794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hahn, A., & Goedderz, A. (2020). Trait-unconsciousness, state-unconsciousness, preconsciousness, and social miscalibriation in the context of implicit evaluations. Social Cognition, 38(Supplement), s155s134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hahn, A., Judd, C. M., Hirsh, H. K., et al. (2014). Awareness of implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(3), 13691392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haines, N., Kvam, P. D., Irving, L. H., et al. (2020). Learning from the reliability paradox: How theoretically informed generative models can advance the social, behavioral, and brain sciences. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Hehman, E., Calanchini, J., Flake, J. K., et al. (2019). Establishing construct validity evidence for regional measures of explicit and implicit racial bias. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(6), 1022.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hehman, E., Flake, J. K., & Calanchini, J. (2018). Disproportionate use of lethal force in policing is associated with regional racial biases of residents. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(4), 393401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., et al. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 13691385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Nosek, B. A., et al. (2005). What moderates implicit–explicit consistency? European Review of Social Psychology, 16, 335390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoover, J., & Dehghani, M. (2020). The big, the bad, and the ugly: Geographic estimation with flawed psychological data. Psychological Methods, 25, 412429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howell, J. L., Gaither, S. E., & Ratliff, K. A. (2015). Caught in the middle: Defensive responses to IAT feedback among whites, blacks, and biracial black/whites. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 373381. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550614561127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, J. L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2017). Not your average bigot: The better‐than‐average effect and defensive responding to Implicit Association Test feedback. British Journal of Social Psychology, 56, 125145. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12168CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howell, J. L., Redford, L., Pogge, G., et al. (2017). Defensive responding to IAT feedback. Social Cognition, 35, 520562. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2017.35.5.520CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Inbar, Y., Westgate, E. C., Pizarro, D. A., et al. (2016). Can a naturally occurring pathogen threat change social attitudes? Evaluations of gay men and lesbians during the 2014 Ebola epidemic. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7, 420427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Irving, L. H., & Smith, C. T. (2020). Measure what you are trying to predict: Applying the correspondence principle to the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 86, 103898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jiang, C., Vitiello, C., Axt, J., et al. (2021). An examination of ingroup preferences among people with multiply socially stigmatized identities. Self and Identity, 20(4), 569586.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T. (2019). The IAT is dead, long live the IAT: Context-sensitive measures of implicit attitudes are indispensable to social and political psychology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(1), 1019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karpinski, A., Steinman, R. B., & Hilton, J. L. (2005). Attitude importance as a moderator of the relationship between implicit and explicit attitude measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 949962.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraus, S. J. (1995). Attitudes and the prediction of behavior: A meta-analysis of the empirical literature. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21(1), 5875.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurdi, B., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). Repeated evaluative pairings and evaluative statements: How effectively do they shift implicit attitudes?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 146(2), 194213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurdi, B., Ratliff, K. A., & Cunningham, W. A. (2021). Can the Implicit Association Test serve as a valid measure of automatic cognition? A response to Schimmack (2021). Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 422434.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). Relationship between the Implicit Association Test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 74(5), 569586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. K., Skinner, A. L., Cooley, E., et al. (2016). Reducing implicit racial preferences: II. Intervention effectiveness across time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 10011016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowe, C., Beach, M. C., & Roter, D. L. (2020). Individuation and implicit racial bias in genetic counseling communication. Patient Education and Counseling, 103(4), 804810.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacInnis, C. C., Page-Gould, E., & Hodson, G. (2017). Multilevel intergroup contact and antigay prejudice (explicit and implicit) evidence of contextual contact benefits in a less visible group domain. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8, 243251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maximiano-Barreto, M. A., Fabrício, D. D. M., Luchesi, B. M., et al. (2020). Factors associated with levels of empathy among students and professionals in the health field: A systematic review. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 42(2), 207215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moors, A., & Houwer, J. D. (2007). What is Automaticity? An analysis of its component features and their interrelations. In Bargh, J. A. (Ed.), Frontiers of Social Psychology. Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes. New York, NY: Psychology Press, pp. 1150.Google Scholar
Neville, H. A., Gallardo, M. E., & Sue, D. W. E. (2016). The Myth of Racial Color Blindness: Manifestations, Dynamics, and Impact. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A. (2005). Moderators of the relationship between implicit and explicit evaluation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 565584.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002a). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002b). E‐research: Ethics, security, design, and control in psychological research on the Internet. Journal of Social Issues, 58(1), 161176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Hansen, J. J., et al. (2007). Pervasiveness and correlates of implicit attitudes and stereotypes. European Review of Social Psychology, 18(1), 3688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ofosu, E. K., Chambers, M. K., Chen, J. M., et al. (2019). Same-sex marriage legalization associated with reduced implicit and explicit antigay bias. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(18), 88468851CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Orchard, J., & Price, J. (2017). County-level racial prejudice and the black–white gap in infant health outcomes. Social Science & Medicine, 181, 191198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., et al. (2013). Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: a meta-analysis of IAT criterion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(2), 171192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., & Gawronski, B. (2010). A history of implicit social cognition: Where is it coming from? Where is it now? Where is it going? In Gawronski, B., & Payne, B. K. (Eds.), Handbook of Implicit Social Cognition: Measurement, Theory, and Applications. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Brown-Iannuzzi, J. L. (2019). Historical roots of implicit bias in slavery. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(24), 1169311698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Lundberg, K. B. (2017). The bias of crowds: How implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 233248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prairie, T. M., Wrye, B., Bowman, A. S., et al. (2019). Does location of practice or religiosity predict negative physician attitudes or beliefs toward LGB+ individuals? Journal of Religion and Health, 58(6), 22082218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rae, J. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2017). Persons or situations? Individual differences explain variance in aggregated implicit race attitudes. Psychological Inquiry, 28(4), 297300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rae, J. R., Newheiser, A. K., & Olson, K. R. (2015). Exposure to racial out-groups and implicit race bias in the United States. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 535543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratliff, K. A., Lofaro, N., Howell, J. L., et al. (2020). Documenting Bias from 2007-2015: Pervasiveness and Correlates of Implicit Attitudes and Stereotypes II. Unpublished Manuscript.Google Scholar
Ratliff[Ranganath], K. A., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Implicit attitude generalization occurs immediately; explicit attitude generalization takes time. Psychological Science, 19(3), 249254.Google Scholar
Ratliff[Ranganath], K. A., Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2008). Distinguishing automatic and controlled components of attitudes from direct and indirect measurement methods. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 386396.Google Scholar
Ratliff, K. A., Redford, L., Conway, J., et al. (2019). Engendering support: Hostile sexism predicts voting for Donald Trump over Hillary Clinton in the 2016 US presidential election. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 22(4), 578593.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravary, A., Baldwin, M. W., & Bartz, J. A. (2019). Shaping the body politic: Mass media fat shaming affects implicit anti-fat attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(11), 15801589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Redford, L., & Ratliff, K. A. (2016). Perceived moral responsibility for attitude‐based discrimination. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55(2), 279296.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Riddle, T., & Sinclair, S. (2019). Racial disparities in school-based disciplinary actions are associated with county-level rates of racial bias. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(17), 82558260. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1808307116CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rydell, R. J., & McConnell, A. R. (2006). Understanding implicit and explicit attitude change: a systems of reasoning analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(6), 9951008.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rydell, R. J., McConnell, A. R., Mackie, D. M., et al. (2006). Of two minds: Forming and changing valence-inconsistent implicit and explicit attitudes. Psychological Science, 17(11), 954958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabin, J. A., Riskind, R. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Health care providers’ implicit and explicit attitudes toward lesbian women and gay men. American Journal of Public Health, 105, 18311841.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schimmack, U. (2021). The Implicit Association Test: A method in search of a construct. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 396414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, K., & Axt, J. R. (2016). Implicit and explicit attitudes toward African Americans and Barack Obama did not substantively change during Obama’s presidency. Social Cognition, 34, 559588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2005). A latent state-trait analysis of implicit and explicit personality measures. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21(2), 100107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. T., Calanchini, J., Hughes, S., et al. (2020). The impact of instruction- and experience-based evaluative learning on IAT performance: A quad model perspective. Cognition and Emotion, 34(1), 2141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. T., & De Houwer, J. (2014). The impact of persuasive messages on IAT performance is moderated by source attractiveness and likeability. Social Psychology, 45(6), 437448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. T., De Houwer, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2013). Consider the source: Persuasion of implicit evaluations is moderated by source credibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 39(2), 193205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, C. T., & Nosek, B. A. (2011). Affective focus increases the concordance between implicit and explicit attitudes. Social Psychology, 42, 300313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. T., Ratliff, K. A., & Nosek, B. A. (2012). Rapid assimilation: Automatically integrating new information with existing beliefs. Social Cognition, 30(2), 199219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 108131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Starck, J. G., Riddle, T., Sinclair, S., et al. (2020). Teachers are people too: Examining the racial bias of teachers compared to other American adults. Educational Researcher. 49(4), 273284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vianello, M., & Bar-Anan, Y. (2021). Can the Implicit Association Test measure automatic judgment? The validation continues. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(2), 415421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuletich, H. A. & Payne, B. K. (2019). Stability and change in implicit bias. Psychological Science, 30(6), 854862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Westgate, E. C., Riskind, R. G., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Implicit preferences for straight people over Lesbian Women and Gay Men weakened from 2006 to 2013. Collabra, 1, (Art.1).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107(1), 101126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zanon, R., De Houwer, J., Gast, A., et al. (2014). When does relational information influence evaluative conditioning? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(11), 21052212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Aberson, C. L. (2017). Psychological perspectives on implicit bias and behavior. In Blume, A. W. (Ed.), Social Issues in Living Color. Westport, CT: Praeger, pp. 261282.Google Scholar
Aberson, C. L., & Ettlin, T. E. (2004). The aversive racism paradigm and responses favoring African Americans: Meta-analytic evidence of two types of favoritism. Social Justice Research, 17, 2546.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axt, J. R. (2018). The best way to measure explicit racial attitudes is to ask about them. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9, 896906.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balliet, D., Wu, J., & de Dreu, C. K. W. (2014). Ingroup favoritism in cooperation: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 15561581.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: Evidence of race evaluations from ages 6, 10 & adulthood. Psychological Science, 17(1), 5358.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bezrukova, K., Spell, C. S., Perry, J. L., et al. (2016). A meta-analytical integration of over 40 years of research on diversity training evaluation. Psychological Bulletin, 142(11), 12271274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bilewicz, M. (2012). Traditional prejudice remains outside of the WEIRD world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(6), 427428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, I. V., Steiner, J. F., Fairclough, D. L., et al. (2013). Clinicians’ implicit ethnic/racial bias and perceptions of care among Black and Latino patients. Annals of Family Medicine, 11(1), 4352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brandt, M. J. (2011). Sexism and gender inequality across 57 societies. Psychological Science, 22(11), 14131418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brewer, M. B. (2017). Intergroup discrimination: Ingroup love or outgroup hate? In Sibley, C. G. & Barlow, F. K. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 90110.Google Scholar
Brief, A. P., Dietz, J., Cohen, R. R., et al. (2000). Just doing business: Modern racism and obedience to authority as explanations for employment discrimination. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 81(1), 7297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buttelmann, D., & Böhm, R. (2014). The ontogeny of the motivation that underlies in-group bias. Psychological Science, 25(4), 921927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Calanchini, J., Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J. W., et al. (2013). Counter-prejudicial training reduces activation of biased associations and enhances response monitoring. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(5), 321325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, E. H., Milkman, K. L., Gromet, D. M., et al. (2019). The mixed effects of online diversity training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 116(16), 77787783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Charlesworth, T. E. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2019). Patterns of implicit and explicit attitudes: I. Long-term change and stability from 2007 to 2016. Psychological Science, 30(2), 174192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cone, J., Mann, T. C., & Ferguson, M. J. (2017). Changing our implicit minds: How, when, and why implicit evaluations can be rapidly revised. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 131199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coons, J. V., & Espinoza, R. K. E. (2018). An examination of aversive heterosexism in the courtroom: Effects of defendants’ sexual orientation and attractiveness, and juror gender on legal decision making. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 5(1), 3543.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, L. A., Roter, D. L., Carson, K. A., et al. (2012). The associations of clinicians’ implicit attitudes about race with medical visit communication and patient ratings of interpersonal care. American Journal of Public Health, 102(5), 979987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costa-Lopes, R., Dovidio, J. F., Pereira, C. R., et al. (2013). Social psychological perspectives on the legitimation of social inequality: Past, present and future. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(4), 229237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crandall, C. S., & Eshleman, A. (2003). A justification-suppression model of the expression and experience of prejudice. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 414446.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crandall, C. S., Eshleman, A., & O’Brien, L. (2002). Social norms and the expression and suppression of prejudice: The struggle for internalization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(3), 359378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crandall, C. S., Miller, J. M., & White, M. H. (2018). Changing norms following the 2016 U.S. Presidential Election: The Trump effect on prejudice. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(2), 186192.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crandall, C. S., & Stangor, C. (2005). Conformity and prejudice. In Dovidio, J. F., Glick, P., & Rudman, L. A. (Eds.), On the Nature of Prejudice: Fifty Years after Allport. Oxford: Blackwell, pp. 295309.Google Scholar
Crisp, R. J., Stone, C. H., & Hall, N. R. (2006). Recategorization and subgroup identification: Predicting and preventing threats from common ingroups. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(2), 230243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crosby, F., Bromley, S., & Saxe, L. (1980). Recent unobtrusive studies of Black and White discrimination and prejudice: A literature review. Psychological Bulletin, 87(3), 546563.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crosby, F., Clayton, S., Alksnis, O., et al. (1986). Cognitive biases in the perception of discrimination: The importance of format. Sex Roles, 14, 637646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de França, D. X., & Monteiro, M. B. (2013). Social norms and the expression of prejudice: The development of aversive racism in children. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(4), 263271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degner, J., & Dalege, J. (2013). The apple does not fall far from the tree, or does it? A meta-analysis of parent-child similarity in intergroup attitudes. Psychological Bulletin, 139(6), 12701304.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Desai, M. U., Paranamana, N., Dovidio, J. F., et al. (2022). System-centered care: How bureaucracy and racialization decenter attempts at person-centered mental health care. Clinical Psychological Science. OnlineFirst publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026221133053CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, M., & Gerard, H. B. (1955). A study of normative and informational social influences upon individual judgment. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(3), 629636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, P. G. (1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: The automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56(1), 518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Devine, P. G., & Ash, T. L. (2022). Diversity training goals, limitations, and promise: A review of the multidisciplinary literature. Annual Review of Psychology, 73, 403429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Austin, A. J., et al. (2012). Long-term reduction in implicit bias: A prejudice habit-breaking intervention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(6), 12671278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dobbin, F., & Kalev, F. (2016). Why diversity programs fail: And what works better. Harvard Business Review, 94(7), 133149.Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F. (2001). On the nature of contemporary prejudice: The third wave. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 829849.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Casados, A. T., & Roussos, G. (2019). Implicit attitudes and behavior toward people with disabilities. In Dunn, D. S. (Ed.), Disability: Social Psychological Perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 239251.Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2000). Aversive racism and selection decisions: 1989 and 1999. Psychological Science, 11(4), 319323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2004). Aversive racism. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 36, pp. 152). Cambridge, MA: Elsevier Academic Press.Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2010). Intergroup bias. In Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of Social Psychology (Vol. 2, 5th ed.). New York, NY: Wiley, pp. 10841121.Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Pearson, A. R. (2017). Aversive racism and contemporary bias. In Sibley, C. G. & Barlow, F. K. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 267294.Google Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Hewstone, M., Glick, P., et al. (2010). Prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination: Theoretical and empirical overview. In Dovidio, J. F., Hewstone, M., Glick, P., & Esses, V. M. (Eds.), Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination. London: SAGE Publications, pp. 328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(1), 6268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Smoak, N., et al. (2009). The nature of contemporary racial prejudice: Insights from implicit and explicit measures of attitudes. In Petty, E., Fazio, R. H., & Brinol, P. (Eds.), Attitudes: Insights from the New Implicit Measures. London: Psychology Press, pp. 165192.Google Scholar
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2008). The development of implicit intergroup cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(7), 248253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2014). The MODE model: Attitude-behavior processes as a function of motivation and opportunity. In Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., & Trope, Y. (Eds.), Dual Process Theories of the Social Mind. Hove: Guilford Press, pp. 155171.Google Scholar
Ferguson, M. J., Mann, T. C., Cone, J., et al., (2019). When and how first impressions can be updated. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(4), 331336.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiscella, K., Epstein, R. M., Griggs, J. J., et al. (2021). Is physician implicit bias associated with differences in care by patient race for metastatic cancer-related pain? PLoS ONE, 16(10), e0257794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, S. T., & Taylor, S. E. (2013). Social Cognition: From Brains to Culture. London: SAGE Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ford, T. E., & Ferguson, M. A. (2004). Social consequences of disparagement humor: A prejudice norm theory. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8(1), 7994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Forscher, P. S., Lai, C. K., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). A meta-analysis of procedures to change implicit measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(13), 522559. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedman, C. (2018). Aversive ableism. Modern prejudice toward disabled people. Review of Disability Studies: An International Journal, 14(4).Google Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (1986). The aversive form of racism. In Dovidio, J. F. & Gaertner, S. L. (Eds.), Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 6189.Google Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2000). Reducing Intergroup Bias: The Common Ingroup Identity Model. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2009). A common ingroup identity: A categorization-based approach for reducing intergroup bias. In. Nelson, T. (Ed.), Handbook of Prejudice. Abingdon: Taylor & Francis, pp. 489506.Google Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., & Dovidio, J. F. (2012). Reducing intergroup bias: The Common Ingroup Identity Model. In Van Lange, P. A. M., Kruglanski, A. W., & Higgins, E. T. (Eds.), Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology (Vol. 2), Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, pp. 439457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., Guerra, R., et al. (2016). A common ingroup identity: A categorization-based approach for reducing intergroup bias. In Nelson, T. (Ed.), Handbook of Prejudice, Discrimination, and Stereotyping (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Psychology Press, pp. 433454.Google Scholar
Gaertner, S. L., & McLaughlin, J. (1983). Racial stereotypes: Associations and ascriptions of positive and negative characteristics. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46(1), 2330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, A. R., Carney, D. R., Pallin, D. J., et al. (2007). Implicit bias among physicians and its predictions of thrombolysis decisions for Black and White patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22(9), 12311238.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Statistically small effects of the Implicit Association Test can have societally large effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(4), 553561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Brendl, M., Cai, H., et al. (2022). Best practices for using the Implicit Association Test. Behavior Research Methods, 54, 11611180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Dasgupta, N., Dovidio, J. F., et al. (2022). Implicit-bias remedies: Treating discriminatory bias as a public-health problem. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 23(1), 740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., & Krieger, L. H. (2006). Implicit bias: Scientific foundations. California Law Review, 94(4), 945967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Lai, C. K. (2020). Implicit social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 419445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., et al. (2009). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 1741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gregg, A. P., Seibt, B., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Easier done than undone: Asymmetry in the malleability of implicit preferences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(1), 120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, W., Schmader, T., Aday, A., et al. (2018). Climate control: Cultural predictors of social identity threat for women in engineering. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 115(3), 446467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hagiwara, N., Dovidio, J. F., Eggly, S., et al. (2016). The effects of racial attitudes on affect and engagement in racially discordant medical interaction between non-Black physicians and Black patients. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 19(4), 509527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hehman, E., Flake, J. K., & Calanchini, J. (2018). Disproportional use of lethal force in policing is associated with regional racial biases of residents. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(4), 393401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, P. J., & Sears, D. O. (2002). The symbolic racism 2000 scale. Political Psychology, 23(2), 253283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ho, A. K., Sidanius, J., Kteily, N., et al. (2015). The nature of social dominance orientation: Theorizing and measuring preferences for intergroup inequality using the new SDO7 scale. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 10031028.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodson, G., Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Processes in racial discrimination: Differential weighting of conflicting information. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(4), 460471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodson, G, Hooper, H., Dovidio, J. F., et al. (2005). Aversive racism in Britain: Legal decisions and the use of inadmissible evidence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35(4), 437448.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofmann, W. H., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., et al. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the Implicit Association Test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(10), 13691385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogg, M. A., Abrams, D., & Brewer, M. B. (2017). Social identity: The role of self in group processes and intergroup relations. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 20(5), 570581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T. (2020). A Theory of System Justification. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jost, J. T., Gaucher, D., & Stern, D. (2015). “The world isn’t fair”: A system justification perspective on social stratification and inequality. In Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Dovidio, J. F., & Simpson, J. A. (Eds.), APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 2: Group Processes. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 317340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kalev, A., Dobbin, F., & Kelly, E. (2006). Best practices or best guesses? Assessing the efficacy of corporate affirmative action and diversity policies. American Sociological Review, 71(4), 589617.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kawakami, K., Amodio, D. M., & Hugenberg, K. (2017). Intergroup perception and cognition: An integrative framework for understanding the causes and consequences of social categorization. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 55, 180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinpenning, G., & Hagendoorn, L. (1993). Forms of racism and the cumulative dimension. Social Psychology Quarterly, 56, 2136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kluegel, J. R., & Smith, E. R. (1986). Beliefs About Inequality: Americans’ Views About What Is and What Ought to Be. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Knight, J. L., Guiliano, T. A., & Sanchez-Ross, M. G. (2001). Famous or infamous? The influence of celebrity status and race on perceptions of responsibility for rape. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 23(3), 183190.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krosnick, J. A., Judd, C. M., & Wittenbrink, B. (2019). The measurement of attitudes. In Albaraccin, D. & Johnson, B. T. (Eds.), The Handbook of Attitudes (2nd ed., Vol. 1). London: Routledge, pp. 45105.Google Scholar
Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). Relationship between the Implicit Association Test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 74(5), 569586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. K., Marini, M., Lehr, S. A., et al. (2014). Reducing implicit racial preferences: I. A comparative investigation of 17 interventions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(4), 17651785.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, C. K., Skinner, A. L., Cooley, E., et al. (2016). Reducing implicit racial preferences: II. Intervention effectiveness across time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(8), 10011016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leitner, J. B., Hehman, E., Ayduk, O., et al. (2016). Blacks’ death rate due to circulatory diseases is positively related to Whites’ explicit racial bias: A nationwide investigation using Project Implicit. Psychological Science, 27(10), 12991311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leyens, J. P., Demoulin, S., Vaes, J., et al. (2007). Infra-humanization: The wall of group differences. Social Issues and Policy Review, 1(1), 139172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Martell, R. F., Lane, D. M., & Emrich, C. E. (1996). Male–female differences: A computer simulation. American Psychologist, 51, 157158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConahay, J. B. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the Modern Racism Scale. In Dovidio, J. F. & Gaertner, S. L. (Eds.), Prejudice, Discrimination, and Racism. New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 99125.Google Scholar
McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37(5), 435442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V., Daly, M., & Neal, A. (2006). Children’s distributive justice judgments: Aversive racism in Euro-American children? Child Development, 77(4), 10631080.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McManus, J. L., Saucier, D. A., O’Dea, C. J., et al. (2019). Aversive affect versus racism as predictors of racial discrimination in helping. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 41(4), 230253.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Michaels, E. K., Board, C., Mujahid, M. S., et al. (2022). Area-level prejudice and health: A systematic review. Health Psychology, 41(3), 211224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Monteith, M. J., Arthur, S. A., & Flynn, S. M. (2010). Self-regulation and bias. In Dovidio, J. F., Hewstone, M., Glick, P., & Esses, V. M. (Eds.). SAGE Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping, and Discrimination. London: SAGE Publications, pp. 493507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moskowitz, G. B., & Ignarri, C. (2009). Implicit volition and stereotype control. European Review of Social Psychology, 20(1), 97145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moskowitz, G. B., Salomon, A. R., & Taylor, C. M. (2000). Preconsciously controlling stereotyping: Implicitly activated egalitarian goals prevent the activation of stereotypes. Social Cognition, 18(2), 151177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nadler, J. T., Lowery, M. R., Grebinski, J., et al. (2014). Aversive discrimination in employment interviews: Reducing effects of sexual orientation bias with accountability. Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity, 1(4), 480488.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niven, D. (2021). Aversive racism at the ballot box: A field experiment on the effects of race and negative information in local elections. The Social Science Journal (online). https://doi.org/10.1080/03623319.2021.1899358CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Norton, M. I., & Sommers, S. R. (2011). Whites see racism as a zero-sum game that they are now losing. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(3), 215218.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6(1), 101115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Onyeador, I. N., Wittlin, N. M., Burke, S. E., et al. (2020). The value of interracial contact for reducing anti-black bias among non-black physicians: A cognitive habits and growth evaluation (CHANGE) study report. Psychological Science, 31(1), 1830.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., et al. (2013). Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: A meta-analysis of IAT criterion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105(2), 171192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paluck, E. L., & Green, D. P. (2009). Prejudice reduction: What works? A critical look at evidence from the field and the laboratory. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 339367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paluck, E. L., Porat, R., Clark, C. S., et al. (2021). Prejudice reduction: Progress and challenges. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 533560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Park, S. H., Glaser, J., & Knowles, E. D. (2008). Implicit motivation to control prejudice moderates the effect of cognitive depletion on unintended discrimination. Social Cognition, 26(4), 401419.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Lundberg, K. B. (2017). The bias of crowds: How implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 28(4), 233248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pearson, A. R., Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2009). The nature of contemporary prejudice: Insights from aversive racism. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3(3), 314338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., Gonzalez, R., et al. (2016). The effects of oncologist implicit racial bias in racially discordant oncology interactions. Journal of Clinical Oncology, 34(24), 28742880.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penner, L. A., Dovidio, J. F., West, T. W., et al. (2010). Aversive racism and medical interactions with Black patients: A field study. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(2), 436440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penner, L. A., Gaertner, S. L., Dovidio, J. F., et al. (2013). A social psychological approach to improving the outcomes of racially discordant medical interactions. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 28(9), 11431149.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perry, S. P., Murphy, M. C., & Dovidio, J. F. (2015). Modern prejudice: Subtle but unconscious? The role of bias awareness in Whites’ perceptions of personal and others’ biases. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 61(11), 6478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E. A., O’Conner, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., et al. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(5), 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pratto, F., & Glasford, D. E. (2008). Ethnocentrism and the value of a human life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(6), 14111428.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quillian, L., Pager, D., Hexel, O., et al. (2017). Meta-analysis of field experiments shows no change in racial discrimination in hiring over time. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, USA, 114(4), 1087010875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, R., Levari, D. E., Akhtar, M., et al. (2022). White (but not Black) Americans continue to see racism as a zero-sum game; White conservatives (but not moderates or liberals) see themselves losing. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(6) 18001810.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richman, L. S., Pascoe, E., & Lattanner, M. (2018). Interpersonal discrimination and physical health. In Major, B., Dovidio, J. F., & Link, B. G. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Stigma, Discrimination, and Health. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 203218.Google Scholar
Riek, B. M., Mania, E. W., & Gaertner, S. L. (2006). Intergroup threat and outgroup attitudes: A meta-analytic review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(4), 336353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roese, N. J., & Jamieson, D. W. (1993). Twenty years of bogus pipeline research: A critical review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114(2), 363375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rooth, D. (2010). Automatic associations and discrimination in hiring: Real world evidence. Labour Economics, 17(3), 523534.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A., Phelan, J. E., & Heppen, J. B. (2007). Developmental sources of implicit attitudes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(12), 17001713.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutland, A., Cameron, L., Milne, A., et al. (2005). Social norms and self-presentation: Children’s implicit and explicit intergroup attitudes. Child Development, 76(2), 451466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabin, J. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2012). The influence of implicit bias on treatment recommendations for 4 common pediatric conditions: Pain, urinary tract infection, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and asthma. American Journal of Public Health, 102(5), 988995.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sabin, J. A., Rivara, F. P., & Greenwald, A. G. (2008). Physician implicit attitudes and stereotypes about race and quality of medical care. Medical Care, 46(7), 678685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvatore, J., & Shelton, J. N. (2007). Cognitive costs of exposure to racial prejudice. Psychological Science, 18(9), 810815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saucier, D. A., Miller, C. T., & Doucet, N. (2005). Differences in helping Whites and Blacks: A meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9(1), 216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmader, T., Dennehy, T. C., & Baron, A. S. (2022). Why antibias interventions (need not) fail. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 17(5), 13811403. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211057565CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scroggins, W. A., Mackie, D. M., Allen, T. J., et al. (2016). Reducing prejudice with labels: Shared group memberships attenuate implicit bias and expand implicit group boundaries. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 42(2), 219229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sears, D. O., & Henry, P. J. (2005). Over thirty years later: A contemporary look at symbolic racism. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Vol. 37). New York, NY: Academic Press, pp. 95150.Google Scholar
Sidanius, J., Cotterill, S., Sheehy-Skeffington, J., et al. (2017). Social dominance theory: Explorations in the psychology of oppression. In Sibley, C. G. & Barlow, F. K. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 149187.Google Scholar
Sidanius, J., & Pratto, F. (1999). Social Dominance: An Intergroup Theory of Social Hierarchy and Oppression. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, S., Lowery, B.S., Hardin, C.D., et al. (2005). Social tuning of automatic racial attitudes: The role of affiliative motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(4), 583592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (2000). Dual-process models in social and cognitive psychology: Conceptual integration and links to underlying memory systems. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4(2), 108131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommers, S. R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2000). Race in the courtroom: Perceptions of guilt and dispositional attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(11), 13671379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Son Hing, L. S., Chung-Yan, G., Hamilton, L., et al. (2008). A two-dimensional model that employs explicit and implicit attitudes to characterize prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94(6), 971987.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Son Hing, L. S., Li, W., & Zanna, M. P. (2002). Inducing hypocrisy to reduce prejudicial responses among aversive racists. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(1), 7178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (2001). Improving Intergroup Relations. London: SAGE Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In Austin, W. G. & Worchel, S. (Eds.), The Social Psychology of Intergroup Relations. Salt Lake City, UT: Brooks/Cole, pp. 3348.Google Scholar
Tracy, S. J., & Rivera, K. D. (2010). Endorsing equity and applauding stay-at-home moms: How male voices on work-life reveal aversive sexism and flickers of transformation. Management Communication Quarterly, 24(1), 343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
United States Equal Employment Opportunity Commission. (n.d). Discrimination by Type. Retrieved from: www.eeoc.gov/laws/types/index.cfmGoogle Scholar
Van Bavel, J. J., & Cunnigham, W. A. (2009). Self-categorization with a novel mixed-race group moderates automatic social and racial biases. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(3), 321335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, I. I.., & Crandall, C. S. (2017). Freedom of racist speech: Ego and expressive threats. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(3), 413429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wojcieszak, M. (2015). Aversive racism in Spain: Testing the theory. International Journal of Public Opinion Research, 27(1), 2245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeager, D. S, & Walton, G. M. (2011). Social-psychological interventions in education: They’re not magic. Review of Educational Research, 81(2), 267301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yogeeswaran, K., Devos, T., & Nash, K. (2017). Understanding the nature, measurement, and utility of implicit intergroup biases. In Sibley, C. G. & Barlow, F. K. (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of the Psychology of Prejudice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 241266.Google Scholar
Ziegert, J. C., & Hanges, P. J. (2005). Employment discrimination: The role of implicit attitudes, motivation, and a climate for racial bias. Journal of Applied Psychology, 90(3), 553562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Arkes, H. R. (this volume). The rationality, interpretation, and overselling of tests of implicit cognition. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark & A. L. Scott (eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Arkes, H. R., & Tetlock, P. E. (2004). Attributions of implicit prejudice, or “Would Jesse Jackson ‘fail’ the Implicit Association Test?” Psychological Inquiry, 15, 257278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2013). Blindspot: Hidden Biases of Good People. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2004). No place for nostalgia in science: A response to Arkes & Tetlock. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 279289.Google Scholar
Bartlett, T. (2017, Jan. 5). Can we really measure implicit bias? Maybe not. Chronicle of Higher Education. Available at: www.chronicle.com/article/Can-We-Really-Measure-Implicit/238807.Google Scholar
Blanton, H., & Jaccard, J. (2008). Unconscious racism: A concept in pursuit of a measure. Annual Review of Sociology, 34, 277297.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., & Burrows, C. N. (2015). Implications of the Implicit Association Test D-transformation for psychological assessment. Assessment, 22(4), 429440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Klick, J., et al. (2009). Strong claims and weak evidence: Reassessing the predictive validity of the IAT. Journal of Applied Psychology, 94(3), 567582. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014665CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blanton, H., Jaccard, J., Strauts, E., et al. (2015). Toward a meaningful metric of implicit prejudice. Journal of Applied Psychology, 100(5), 14681481. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bodenhausen, G. V., & Petsko, C. D. (this volume). Complications in predicting intergroup behavior from implicit biases: One size does not fit all. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Carlsson, R., & Agerström, J. (2016). A closer look at the discrimination outcomes in the IAT literature. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 57(4), 278287. https://doi.org/10.1111/sjop.12288CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chugh, D., Bazerman, M. H., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Bounded ethicality as a psychological barrier to recognizing conflicts of interest. In Moore, D. A., Cain, D. M., Loewenstein, G., & Bazerman, M. H. (Eds.), Conflicts of Interest: Challenges and Solutions in Business, Law, Medicine, and Public Policy. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 7495. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511610332.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corneille, O., & Hütter, M. (2020). Implicit? What do you mean? A comprehensive review of the delusive implicitness construct in attitude research. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 24, 212232. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868320911325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., Pearson, A. R., Schellhaas, F. M. H., et al. (this volume). Aversive racism and implicit bias. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fazio, R. H., Granados Samayoa, J. A., Boggs, S. T., et al. (this volume). Implicit bias: What is it? In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Forscher, P. S., Lai, C. K., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). A meta-analysis of procedures to change implicit measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(3), 522559. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenn, D. (2008, April 25). Our hidden prejudices, on trial. Chronicle of Higher Education. Available at: www.chronicle.com/article/Our-Hidden-Prejudices-on/4421Google Scholar
Green, A. R., Carney, D. R., Pallin, D. J., et al. (2007). Implicit bias among physicians and its prediction of thrombolysis decisions for Black and White patients. Journal of General Internal Medicine, 22(9), 12311238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11606–007-0258-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2017). The implicit revolution: Reconceiving the relation between conscious and unconscious. American Psychologist, 72(9), 861871. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). Statistically small effects of the Implicit Association Test can have societally large effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 553561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Dasgupta, N., Dovidio, J. F., Kang, J., Moss-Racusin, C. A., & Teachman, B. A. (2022). Implicit-bias remedies: Treating discriminatory bias as a public-health problem. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 23, 740. https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006211070CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., & Krieger, L. H. (2006). Implicit bias: Scientific foundations. California Law Review, 94, 945967.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenwald, A. G., & Lai, C. K. (2020). Implicit social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 71, 419445. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010419-050837CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E. L., et al. (2009). Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 1741.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith v. Fred Meyer Stores, Inc., 2019 WL 9362530 (May 26, 2019 Washington Superior Court).Google Scholar
Hahn, A., Judd, C. M., Hirsh, H. K., et al. (2014). Awareness of implicit attitudes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 13691392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haynes v. Indiana University, 2017 WL 4225597 (June 5, 2017 S.D. Ind.).Google Scholar
Janis, I. L. (1982). Groupthink. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.Google Scholar
Jost, J. T., Rudman, L. A., Blair, I. V., et al. (2009). The existence of implicit bias is beyond reasonable doubt: A refutation of ideological and methodological objections and executive summary of ten studies that no manager should ignore. Research in Organizational Behavior, 29, 3969. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.riob.2009.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jussim, L., Careem, A., Goldberg, Z., et al. (this volume). IAT scores, racial gaps, and scientific gaps. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark, & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kam, C. D., & Engelhardt, A. W. (this volume). The mass public’s view of implicit bias, with implications for scientific communication in a politically polarized age. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark, & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kang, J., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). Fair measures: A behavioral realist revision of “affirmative action.” California Law Review, 94, 10631118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karpinski, A., & Hilton, J. L. (2001). Attitudes and the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 774788. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.774CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klauer, K. C., & Becker, M. (this volume). Latent state-trait analyses for process models of implicit measures. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark, & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kurdi, B., Seitchik, A. E., Axt, J. R., et al. (2019). Relationship between the Implicit Association Test and intergroup behavior: A meta-analysis. American Psychologist, 74(5), 569586. https://doi.org/10.1037/amp0000364CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lai, C. K., Marini, M., Lehr, S. A., et al. (2014). A comparative investigation of 17 interventions to reduce implicit racial preferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 17651785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lane, K. A., Kang, J., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). Implicit social cognition and law. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 3, 427451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, G., Bashakova, Y., Krosnick, J. A., et al. (this volume). Public attitudes on implicit bias. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark, & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lublin, J. S. (2014, January 9). Bringing hidden biases into the light – big businesses teach staffers how “unconscious bias” impacts decisions. Wall Street Journal. Available at: http://online.wsj.com/news/articles/SB10001424052702303754404579308562690896896Google Scholar
Lundberg, K. B., & Payne, B. K. (this volume). The bias of crowds: Rethinking implicit bias in social context. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark, & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McConnell, A. R., & Leibold, J. M. (2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37(5), 435442. https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2000.1470CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meissner, , F., & Rothermund, , K. (this volume). Increasing the validity of implicit measures: New solutions for assessment, conceptualization, and action explanation. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark, & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mitchell, G. (2018). Jumping to conclusions: Advocacy and application of psychological research. In Jussim, L. & Crawford, J. (Eds.), The Politics of Social Psychology (pp. 139155). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.Google Scholar
Mitchell, G., & Tetlock, P. E. (2006). Antidiscrimination law and the perils of mindreading. Ohio State Law Journal, 67, 10231121.Google Scholar
Mitchell, G., & Tetlock, P. E. (2017). Popularity as a poor proxy for utility: The case of implicit prejudice. In Lilienfeld, S. O. & Waldman, I. D. (Eds.), Psychological Science Under Scrutiny: Recent Challenges and Proposed Solutions. New York, NY: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 164195. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119095910.ch10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (2007). The implicit association test at age 7: A methodological and conceptual review. In Bargh, J. A. (Ed.), Social Psychology and the Unconscious: The Automaticity of Higher Mental Processes. New York, NY: Psychology Press, pp. 265292.Google Scholar
Olson, M. A., & Fazio, R. H. (2004). Reducing the influence of extrapersonal associations on the implicit association test: Personalizing the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(5), 653667. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.86.5.653CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., et al. (2013). Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination: A meta-analysis of IAT criterion studies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 105, 171192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oswald, F. L., Mitchell, G., Blanton, H., et al. (2015). Predicting ethnic and racial discrimination with the IAT: Small effect sizes of unknown societal significance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 562571.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paul, A. M. (1998, May 1). Where bias begins: The truth about stereotypes. Psychology Today, 31.Google Scholar
Payne, B. K., Doris, J. M. (2018, Mar. 27). How to think about “implicit bias.” Scientific American. Available at: www.scientificamerican.com/article/how-to-think-about-implicit-bias/.Google Scholar
Perkins, A., Forehand, M., Greenwald, A., et al. (2008). Measuring the nonconscious: Implicit social cognition in consumer behavior. In Haugtvedt, C. P., Herr, P. M., & Kardes, F. R. (Eds.), Marketing and Consumer Psychology Series: Vol. 4. Handbook of Consumer Psychology. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis Group/Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 461475.Google Scholar
Pippen v. State of Iowa (Iowa District Court for Polk County, April 17, 2012). Available at: https://iowaappeals.com/wp-content/uploads/2012/05/Pippen-Ruling.pdf.Google Scholar
Plessy v. Ferguson, 163 U.S. 537 (1896).Google Scholar
Potier, B. (2004, December 16). Making case for concept of “implicit prejudice”: Extending the legal definition of discrimination, Harvard University Gazette. Available at: https://news.harvard.edu/gazette/story/2004/12/making-case-for-concept-of-implicit-prejudice/Google Scholar
Quillian, L. (2006). New approaches to understanding racial prejudice and discrimination. Annual Review of Sociology, 32, 299328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rao, M. (2020, Mar. 21). Demand for diversity training is on the rise in Minnesota. Star Tribune. Available at: www.startribune.com/diversity-workshops-gaining-popularity-in-minnesota/568962792/.Google Scholar
Ratliff, K. A., & Smith, C. T. (2022). Implicit bias as automatic behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 33, 213218. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2022.2106764CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rudman, L. A. (2008). The validity of the Implicit Association Test is a scientific certainty. Industrial and Organizational Psychology, 1, 426429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ryan, C. (1974). A Bridge Too Far. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Schimmack, U. (2021). The Implicit Association Test: A method in search of a construct. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16, 396414. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1745691619863798CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singal, J. (2017). Psychology’s favorite tool for measuring racism isn’t up to the job. The Cut. www.thecut.com/2017/01/psychologys-racism-measuring-tool-isnt-up-to-the-job.htmlGoogle Scholar
Skagway Jewelry v. Westmark Hotels, 2016 WL 11595221 (Oct. 20, 2016 D. Alaska).Google Scholar
Tetlock, P. E., & Mitchell, G. (2009). Implicit bias and accountability systems: What must organizations do to prevent discrimination? In Staw, B. M. & Brief, A. (Eds.), Research in Organizational Behavior (Vol. 29, pp. 338). New York, NY: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Tetlock, P. E., Mitchell, G., & Anastasopoulos, L. J. (2013). Detecting and punishing unconscious bias. Journal of Legal Studies, 42, 83110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, G. L., Kovera, M. B., Douglass, A. B., et al. (2020). Policy and procedure recommendations for the collection and preservation of eyewitness identification evidence. Law and Human Behavior, 44(1), 336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolsiefer, , K. J., & Blair, , I. V. (this volume). Implicit bias and discrimination: Evidence on causality. In J. A. Krosnick, T. H. Stark, & A. L. Scott (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Implicit Bias and Racism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Zelevansky, N. (2019, Nov. 20). The big business of unconscious bias. New York Times. Available at: www.nytimes.com/2019/11/20/style/diversity-consultants.html?smid=url-shareGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×