Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2plfb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T14:35:40.488Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

21 - Children’s Intergroup Prosocial Behavior

The Role of Group Stereotypes

from Part III - Development of Prosociality in Context

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 May 2023

Tina Malti
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
Maayan Davidov
Affiliation:
The Hebrew University of Jerusalem
Get access

Summary

Children are prosocial from a young age onward, but their prosocial actions are not necessarily egalitarian. From around 4 years of age children tend to help and share more with in-group members compared with out-group members. However, a growing body of findings also suggest that sometimes children act more prosocially toward out-group members. How can we reconcile such seemingly contradicting behaviors? Here, the author describes how the salience of group stereotypes might shed light on these inconsistent findings. Specifically, different helping contexts can activate different group stereotypes. These different stereotypes could lead children to sometimes act more prosocially toward in-group peers, but sometimes show out-group bias in their helping or sharing behavior. Taking into account group stereotypes in children’s prosocial behavior will provide a deeper understanding of the underlying motivations that lead to selective prosociality in children and, in the long run, contribute to combating discrimination and prejudice early in life.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Cambridge Handbook of Prosociality
Development, Mechanisms, Promotion
, pp. 427 - 441
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ajzen, I. (2001). Nature and operation of attitudes. Annual Review of Psychology, 52, 2758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angerer, S., Glätzle-Rützler, D., Lergetporer, P., & Sutter, M. (2016). Cooperation and discrimination within and across language borders: Evidence from children in a bilingual city. European Economic Review, 90, 254264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beelmann, A., & Heinemann, K. S. (2014). Preventing prejudice and improving intergroup attitudes: A meta-analysis of child and adolescent training programs. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 35, 1024.Google Scholar
Benozio, A., & Diesendruck, G. (2015). Parochialism in preschool boys’ resource allocation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36, 256264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigler, R. S., Jones, L. C., & Lobliner, D. B. (1997). Social categorization and the formation of intergroup attitudes in children. Child Development, 68, 530543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bindra, P. C., Glätzle-Rützler, D., & Lergetporer, P. (2020). Discrimination at young age: Experimental evidence from preschool children. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 175, 5570.Google Scholar
Böhm, R., & Buttelmann, D. (2017). The impact of resource valence on children’s other-regarding preferences. Developmental Psychology, 53, 16561665.Google Scholar
Brown, C. S., Ali, H., Stone, E. A., & Jewell, J. A. (2017). US children’s stereotypes and prejudicial attitudes toward Arab Muslims. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy, 17, 6083.Google Scholar
Buttelmann, D., & Böhm, R. (2014). The ontogeny of the motivation that underlies in-group bias. Psychological Science, 25, 921927.Google Scholar
Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math–gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development, 82, 766779.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Copping, K. E., Kurtz‐Costes, B., Rowley, S. J., & Wood, D. (2013). Age and race differences in racial stereotype awareness and endorsement. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 43, 971980.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidov, M., Vaish, A., Knafo‐Noam, A., & Hastings, P. D. (2016). The motivational foundations of prosocial behavior from a developmental perspective – Evolutionary roots and key psychological mechanisms: Introduction to the special section. Child Development, 87, 16551667.Google Scholar
Dunfield, K. A., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2013). Classifying prosocial behavior: Children’s responses to instrumental need, emotional distress, and material desire. Child Development, 84, 17661776.Google Scholar
Dunham, Y. (2018). Mere membership. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 780793.Google Scholar
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82, 793811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elenbaas, L., Rizzo, M. T., & Killen, M. (2020). A developmental-science perspective on social inequality. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29, 610616.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., Bernhard, H., & Rockenbach, B. (2008). Egalitarianism in young children. Nature, 45, 10791083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415, 137140.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 878902.Google Scholar
Glasman, L. R., & Albarracín, D. (2006). Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: A meta-analysis of the attitude-behavior relation. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 778822.Google Scholar
Gönültaş, S., & Mulvey, K. L. (2021). The role of immigration background, intergroup processes, and social‐cognitive skills in bystanders’ responses to bias‐based bullying toward immigrants during adolescence. Child Development, 92, e296e316.Google Scholar
Gummerum, M., Takezawa, M., & Keller, M. (2009). The influence of social category and reciprocity on adults’ and children’s altruistic behavior. Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 295316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilton, B. C., O’Neill, A. C., & Kuhlmeier, V. A. (2021). Emerging selectivity: Group membership and early prosociality. Journal of Cognition and Development, 22(2), 267280. doi:10.1080/15248372.2021.1890601Google Scholar
Jara-Ettinger, J., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Schulz, L. E. (2015). Not so innocent: Toddlers’ inferences about costs and culpability. Psychological Science, 26, 633640.Google Scholar
Jin, K. S., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Infants possess an abstract expectation of ingroup support. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114, 81998204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, P. A., Katz, I., & Cohen, S. (1976). White children’s attitudes towards blacks and the physically handicapped: A developmental study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 68, 2024.Google Scholar
Killen, M., & Rutland, A. (2011). Children and social exclusion: Morality, prejudice, and group identity. Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., & Spelke, E. S. (2012). “Native” objects and collaborators: Infants’ object choices and acts of giving reflect favor for native over foreign speakers. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 6781.Google Scholar
Kogut, T. (2012). Knowing what I should, doing what I want: From selfishness to inequity aversion in young children’s sharing behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33, 226236.Google Scholar
Kuang, Y., Wang, F., & Wang, Z. J. (2021). Social class and children’s prosociality: A study in the context of China’s dual urban–rural structure. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 12, 6370.Google Scholar
Kuhlmeier, V. A., Dunfield, K. A., & O’Neill, A. C. (2014). Selectivity in early prosocial behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 836.Google Scholar
Kuipers, G. (2000). The difference between a Surinamese and a Turk: Ethnic jokes and the position of ethnic minorities in the Netherlands. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 13, 141176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, J. D., Wellman, H. M., & Gelman, S. A. (2013). Informants’ traits weigh heavily in young children’s trust in testimony and in their epistemic inferences. Child Development, 84, 12531268.Google Scholar
Lazić, A., Purić, D., & Krstić, K. (2021). Does parochial cooperation exist in childhood and adolescence? A meta-analysis. International Journal of Psychology, 56, 917933.Google Scholar
Li, V., Spitzer, B., & Olson, K. R. (2014). Preschoolers reduce inequality while favoring individuals with more. Child Development, 85, 11231133.Google Scholar
List, A. J., List, J. A., & Samek, A. (2017). Discrimination among pre-school children: Field experimental evidence. Economics Letters, 157, 159162.Google Scholar
Mackie, D. M., Hamilton, D. L., Susskind, J., & Rosselli, F. (1996). Social psychological foundations of stereotype formation. In Macrae, C. N., Stangor, C., & Hewstone, M. (Eds.), Stereotypes and stereotyping (pp. 4178). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Malti, T., & Cheah, C. S. (2021). Toward complementarity: Specificity and commonality in social‐emotional development. Child Development, 92, e1085e1094.Google Scholar
Mandalaywala, T. M., Benitez, J. Sagar, K., & Rhodes, M. (2021). Why do children show racial biases in their resource allocation decisions? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 211, 105224.Google Scholar
Mandalaywala, T. M., Rhodes, M., & Tai, C. (2020). Children’s use of race and gender as cues to social status. PLoS ONE, 15, e0234398.Google Scholar
Marquis, A. R., & Sugden, N. A. (2019). Meta-analytic review of infants’ preferential attention to familiar and unfamiliar face types based on gender and race. Developmental Review, 53, 100868.Google Scholar
Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. (2004). Children’s search for gender cues: Cognitive perspectives on gender development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 6770.Google Scholar
McAuliffe, K., & Dunham, Y. (2016). Group bias in cooperative norm enforcement. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371, 20150073.Google Scholar
McGuire, L., Rizzo, M. T., Killen, M., & Rutland, A. (2018). The development of intergroup resource allocation: The role of cooperative and competitive in-group norms. Developmental Psychology, 54, 14991506.Google Scholar
Misch, A., Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2016). I won’t tell: Young children show loyalty to their group by keeping group secrets. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 142, 96106.Google Scholar
Misch, A., Paulus, M., & Dunham, Y. (2021). Anticipation of future cooperation eliminates minimal ingroup bias in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150, 20362056.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, C. (2009). Fairness in children’s resource allocation depends on the recipient. Psychological Science, 20, 944948.Google Scholar
Nasir, N. I. S., McKinney de Royston, M., O’Connor, K., & Wischnia, S. (2017). Knowing about racial stereotypes versus believing them. Urban Education, 52, 491524.Google Scholar
Newheiser, A. K., Dunham, Y., Merrill, A., Hoosain, L., & Olson, K. R. (2014). Preference for high status predicts implicit outgroup bias among children from low-status groups. Developmental Psychology, 50, 10811090.Google Scholar
Olson, K. R., Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., & Weisman, K. G. (2012). Children associate racial groups with wealth: Evidence from South Africa. Child Development, 83, 18841899.Google Scholar
Over, H. (2018). The influence of group membership on young children’s prosocial behaviour. Current Opinion in Psychology, 20, 1720.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmer, S. B., Rutland, A., & Cameron, L. (2015). The development of bystander intentions and social–moral reasoning about intergroup verbal aggression. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 419433.Google Scholar
Pauker, K., Ambady, N., & Apfelbaum, E. P. (2010). Race salience and essentialist thinking in racial stereotype development. Child Development, 81, 17991813.Google Scholar
Pauker, K., Xu, Y., Williams, A., & Biddle, A. M. (2016). Race essentialism and social contextual differences in children’s racial stereotyping. Child Development, 87, 14091422.Google Scholar
Paulus, M., & Essler, S. (2020). Why do preschoolers perpetuate inequalities? Theoretical perspectives on inequity preferences in the face of emerging concerns for equality. Developmental Review, 58, 100933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, M., & Moore, C. (2014). The development of recipient-dependent sharing behavior and sharing expectations in preschool children. Developmental Psychology, 50, 914921.Google Scholar
Pettigrew, T., & Tropp, L. (2006). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 751783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Plötner, M., Over, H., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2015). The effects of collaboration and minimal-group membership on children’s prosocial behavior, liking, affiliation, and trust. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 139, 161173.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pun, A., Birch, S. A., & Baron, A. S. (2021). The power of allies: Infants’ expectations of social obligations during intergroup conflict. Cognition, 211, 104630.Google Scholar
Quinn, P. C., Anzures, G., Izard, C. E., Lee, K., Pascalis, O., Slater, A. M., & Tanaka, J. W. (2011). Looking across domains to understand infant representation of emotion. Emotion Review, 3, 197206.Google Scholar
Raabe, T., & Beelmann, A. (2011). Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: A multinational meta‐analysis of age differences. Child Development, 82, 17151737.Google Scholar
Renno, M. P., & Shutts, K. (2015). Children’s social category-based giving and its correlates: Expectations and preferences. Developmental Psychology, 51, 533543.Google Scholar
Rhodes, M., Leslie, S. J., Saunders, K., Dunham, Y., & Cimpian, A. (2018). How does social essentialism affect the development of inter‐group relations? Developmental Science, 21(1), e12509. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12509Google Scholar
Rowley, S. J., Kurtz‐Costes, B., Mistry, R., & Feagans, L. (2007). Social status as a predictor of race and gender stereotypes in late childhood and early adolescence. Social Development, 16, 150168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutland, A., Hitti, A., Mulvey, K. L., Abrams, D., & Killen, M. (2015). When does the in-group like the out-group? Bias among children as a function of group norms. Psychological Science, 26, 834842.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shutts, K., Brey, E. L., Dornbusch, L. A., Slywotzky, N., & Olson, K. R. (2016). Children use wealth cues to evaluate others. PLoS ONE, 11(3).Google Scholar
Shutts, K., Kinzler, K. D., Katz, R. C., Tredoux, C., & Spelke, E. S. (2011). Race preferences in children: Insights from South Africa. Developmental Science, 14, 12831291.Google Scholar
Shutts, K., Roben, C. K. P., & Spelke, E. S. (2013). Children’s use of social categories in thinking about people and social relationships. Journal of Cognition and Development, 14, 3562.Google Scholar
Sierksma, J. (2018). Costs of helping only influence children’s intention to help ethnic out-group peers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 173, 8599.Google Scholar
Sierksma, J., Brey, E., & Shutts, K. (2022). Racial stereotype application in 4- to 8-year-old white American children: Emergence and specificity. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1–26.Google Scholar
Sierksma, J., Lansu, T. A., Karremans, J. C., & Bijlstra, G. (2018). Children’s helping behavior in an ethnic intergroup context: Evidence for outgroup helping. Developmental Psychology, 54, 916928.Google Scholar
Sierksma, J., & Shutts, K. (2021). Competence-based helping: Children’s consideration of need when providing others with help. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 210, 105206.Google Scholar
Sierksma, J., Thijs, J., & Verkuyten, M. (2014a). Ethnic helping and group identity: A study among majority group children. Social Development, 23, 803819.Google Scholar
Sierksma, J., Thijs, J., & Verkuyten, M. (2014b). Children’s intergroup helping: The role of empathy and peer group norms. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 369383.Google Scholar
Sierksma, J., Thijs, J., & Verkuyten, M. (2015). In‐group bias in children’s intention to help can be overpowered by inducing empathy. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 4556. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12065CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sparks, E., Schinkel, M. G., & Moore, C. (2017). Affiliation affects generosity in young children: The roles of minimal group membership and shared interests. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 159, 242262.Google Scholar
Stangor, C. (2016). The study of stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination within social psychology: A quick history of theory and research. In Nelson, T. D. (Ed.), Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination (pp. 327). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Stürmer, S., & Snyder, M. (Eds.). (2010). The psychology of prosocial behavior. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Taylor, L. K. (2020). The Developmental Peacebuilding Model (DPM) of children’s prosocial behaviors in settings of intergroup conflict. Child Development Perspectives, 14, 127134.Google Scholar
Umaña-Taylor, A. J. (2016). A post-racial society in which ethnic-racial discrimination still exists and has significant consequences for youths’ adjustment. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 25, 111118.Google Scholar
Vaish, A., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Young children selectively avoid helping people with harmful intentions. Child Development, 81, 16611669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Leeuwen, E., & Zagefka, H. (Eds.). (2017). Intergroup helping. Springer.Google Scholar
Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 13011303.Google Scholar
Weller, D., & Hansen Lagattuta, K. (2013). Helping the in‐group feels better: Children’s judgments and emotion attributions in response to prosocial dilemmas. Child Development, 84, 253268.Google Scholar
Zimmerman, B. J., & Levy, G. D. (2000). Social cognitive predictors of prosocial behavior toward same and alternate race children among white pre-schoolers. Current Psychology, 19, 175193. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-000-1014-8Google Scholar
Zinser, O., Perry, J. S., Bailey, R. C., & Lydiatt, E. W. (1976). Racial recipients, value of donations, and sharing behavior in children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 129, 2935.Google Scholar
Zinser, O., Rich, M., & Bailey, R. (1981). Sharing behavior and racial preference in children. Motivation and Emotion, 65, 179187.Google Scholar
Zou, L. X., & Cheryan, S. (2017). Two axes of subordination: A new model of racial position. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112, 696717.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×