Book contents
- Frontmatter
- Contents
- List of contributors
- Preface
- Acknowledgments
- Introduction – Biological factors in crime causation: the reactions of social scientists
- Part I Methodological questions and implications
- Part II Evidence for the role of genetics
- Part III Psychophysiological and neurophysiological factors
- Part IV Neurological factors
- Part V Biochemical factors
- 13 The neuroendocrinology and neurochemistry of antisocial behavior
- 14 Testosterone and adrenaline: aggressive antisocial behavior in normal adolescent males
- 15 Personality correlates of plasma testosterone levels in young delinquents: an example of person–situation interaction?
- 16 Metabolic dysfunctions among habitually violent offenders: reactive hypoglycemia and cholesterol levels
- Part VI Treatment issues
- Author index
- Subject index
15 - Personality correlates of plasma testosterone levels in young delinquents: an example of person–situation interaction?
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 04 August 2010
- Frontmatter
- Contents
- List of contributors
- Preface
- Acknowledgments
- Introduction – Biological factors in crime causation: the reactions of social scientists
- Part I Methodological questions and implications
- Part II Evidence for the role of genetics
- Part III Psychophysiological and neurophysiological factors
- Part IV Neurological factors
- Part V Biochemical factors
- 13 The neuroendocrinology and neurochemistry of antisocial behavior
- 14 Testosterone and adrenaline: aggressive antisocial behavior in normal adolescent males
- 15 Personality correlates of plasma testosterone levels in young delinquents: an example of person–situation interaction?
- 16 Metabolic dysfunctions among habitually violent offenders: reactive hypoglycemia and cholesterol levels
- Part VI Treatment issues
- Author index
- Subject index
Summary
Studies of the influence of male sex hormones on aggression and violence have had inconsistent results (for reviews see Meyer-Bahlburg, 1981; Rose, 1975; Rubin, Reinisch, & Haskett, 1981). The empirical bases are rather weak for two commonly held assumptions: that aggressive behavior in animals is determined by testosterone level and that testosterone also activates violent and aggressive behavior in humans.
The animal research gives little evidence for a direct effect of sex hormones on fighting but indicates a relation between changes in social rank or status and dominant–submissive behavior and changes in testosterone levels, at least in monkeys (Rose, Holaday, & Bernstein, 1971) and mice (Henry & Stephens, 1978). Keverne, Meller, and Eberhart (1982) found that high-rank monkeys had a 300–500% increase in testosterone when introduced into a mixed-sex group. They displayed (as expected) higher sexual activity than the low-rank animals, who had only a slight increase in testosterone. Although they did not show more aggressive behavior, the high-rank, high-testosterone monkeys were the objects of significantly less aggression by other males. When placed again in single cages, they no longer differed in testosterone level from the low-rank monkeys. Nor were there any differences between high- and low-rank monkeys when they were separately introduced to females, with no other males present. In the latter situation, the only endocrine difference observed was higher “stress” hormone (cortisol and prolactin) levels in the low-rank animals.
- Type
- Chapter
- Information
- The Causes of CrimeNew Biological Approaches, pp. 283 - 291Publisher: Cambridge University PressPrint publication year: 1987
- 5
- Cited by