Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-lvwk9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-12T22:53:29.904Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Polysemy and related phenomena from a cognitive linguistic viewpoint

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 September 2009

Patrick Saint-Dizier
Affiliation:
Institut de Recherche en Informatique, Toulouse
Evelyn Viegas
Affiliation:
Brandeis University, Massachusetts
Get access

Summary

Introduction

One of the fundamental problems of lexical semantics is the fact that what C. Ruhl (1989) calls the ‘perceived meaning’ of a word can vary so greatly from one context to another. In this chapter I want to survey the ways in which the contribution the same grammatical word makes to the meaning of a larger unit may differ in different contexts. There are two main sources of explanatory hypotheses for contextual variations in word meaning: lexical semantics and pragmatics. While there are probably no contexts where each of these is not involved in some way, their relative contributions can vary. For instance, in the following examples the difference between 1 and 2 in respect of the interpretation of the word teacher (i.e.,“male teacher” and “female teacher”, respectively) can be accounted for entirely by differential contextual enrichment of a single lexical meaning for teacher (in other words, pragmatically):

  1. 1. The teacher stroked his beard.

  2. 2. Our maths teacher is on maternity leave.

The only involvement of lexical semantics here is that the specification of the meaning of teacher must somehow make it clear that although it is unspecified for sex, it is, unlike, say, chair, specifiable for sex. Examples 3 and 4 exemplify a slightly different type of contextual enrichment, in that the extra specificity in context is of a meronymous rather than a hyponymous type:

  1. 3. John washed the car.

  2. 4. The mechanic lubricated the car.

The different actions performed on the car allow us to infer that the agents were occupied with different parts of the car in each case.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1995

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×