Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-k7p5g Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-12T08:01:38.471Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Psychology in America: the early years

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 September 2015

John D. Greenwood
Affiliation:
City University of New York
Get access

Summary

Psychology first became an academic scientific discipline in Germany because Germany developed the modern university system that enabled Wundt and his colleagues to create an experimental psychology grounded upon the earlier success of experimental physiology. However, psychology developed faster institutionally in America than it did in Germany (Ash, 1980; Danziger, 1979). By the end of the nineteenth century, American psychology had an active professional organization, the American Psychological Association (APA), founded in 1892; journals devoted to general, experimental, and applied psychology, such as the American Journal of Psychology, Pedagogical Seminary, Psychological Review, Psychological Index, and Psychological Monographs; and a substantial academic presence within the American university system. By 1892, America had more and better laboratories than did Germany. Formal research laboratories were established at the University of Chicago, and Clark, Columbia, Cornell, Harvard, Johns Hopkins, Pennsylvania, Wisconsin, and Yale Universities; and demonstration facilities for teaching and training were available at Brown University, Catholic University of America, and Indiana, Kansas, Michigan, Nebraska, Illinois, Iowa, and Wellesley Universities (Hale, 1980; O'Donnell, 1985). By 1904, there were forty-nine laboratories, 169 members of the APA, and sixty-two institutions offering three or more courses in psychology; in addition, psychology ranked fourth in the sciences with respect to the number of PhD degrees awarded (Camfield, 1973; Miner, 1904). By 1913, the year that Watson published his behaviorist manifesto Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It, America had surpassed Germany in research publications (Cattell, 1917).

Much the same was true of the academic institutionalization of other social sciences in the early twentieth century. Émile Durkheim (1858–1917) introduced sociology in France at the end of the nineteenth century, but it made slow progress, and by 1930 there were only three chairs in sociology. Yet by 1910, there were about fifty full-time professors of sociology in America, and the subject was taught at around 400 colleges (Smith, 1997).

Type
Chapter
Information
A Conceptual History of Psychology
Exploring the Tangled Web
, pp. 289 - 343
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, G. W. (1967). William James: A Biography. New York, NY: Viking.Google Scholar
Allport, F. H. (1920). The influence of the group upon association and thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3, 159–182.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allport, G. W. (1967). Autobiography. In Boring, E. G. and Lindzey, G. (Eds.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. (Vol. 5.) New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Ash, M. G. (1980). Experimental psychology in Germany before 1914: aspects of an academic identity problem. Psychological Research, 42, 75–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1895). Types of reaction. Psychological Review, 2, 259–273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1896). The “type-theory” of reaction. Mind, 5, 81–90.Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1926). Between Two Wars, 1861–1921. London: Stratford.Google Scholar
Benjamin, L. T. Jr. (1991). Harry Kirke Wolfe: Pioneer in Psychology. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Benjamin, L. T. Jr. (1997). Wilhelm Wundt: The American connection. In Bringmann, W. G., Lück, H. E., Miller, R., and Early, C. E. (Eds.), A Pictorial History of Psychology. Chicago, IL: Quintessence.Google Scholar
Benjamin, L. T. Jr., Durkin, M., Link, M., Vesta, M., and Accord, J. (1992). Wundt's American doctoral students. American Psychologist, 47, 123–131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A. (1903). L'étude expérimentale de l'intelligence [Experimental Studies of Intelligence]. Paris: Schleichter.Google Scholar
Blumenthal, A. L. (1975). A reappraisal of Wilhelm Wundt. American Psychologist, 30, 1081–1088.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumenthal, A. L. (1979). The founding father we never knew. Contemporary Psychology, 24, 547–550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boakes, R. (1984). From Darwin to Behaviorism: Psychology and the Minds of Animals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bolles, R. C. (1993). The Story of Psychology: A Thematic History. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1938). The Society of Experimental Psychologists: 1904–1938. American Journal of Psychology, 51, 410–423.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boring, E. G. (1953). A history of introspection. Psychological Bulletin, 50, 169–189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boring, E. G. (1957). A History of Experimental Psychology. (edn.) New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Bramel, D. and Friend, R. (1981). Hawthorne, the myth of the docile worker, and class bias in American psychology. American Psychologist, 36, 867–878.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. S. and Allport, G. W. (1940). Fifty years of change in American psychology. Psychological Review, 37, 757–776.Google Scholar
Cahan, E. (2005). Science, Practice, and Gender Roles in Early Child Psychology. Paper presented at the 37th Annual Meeting of Cheiron, University of California at Berkeley, CA, June 23–26.
Camfield, T. M. (1973). The professionalization of American psychology, 1870–1917. Journal for the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 9, 66–75.3.0.CO;2-X>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, J. M. (1885). Über die Zeit der Erkennung und Benennung von Scriftzeichen, Bilden und Farben [About the time of recognition, and the naming of written signs, pictures and colors]. Philosophische Studien, 2, 635–650.Google Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1886). On the time it takes to see and name objects. Mind, 11, 63–65.Google Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1890). Mental tests and measurements. Mind, 15, 373–381.Google Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1903). Statistics of American psychologists. American Journal of Psychology, 14, 310–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1904). The conceptions and methods of psychology. Popular Science Monthly, 66, 176–186.Google Scholar
Cattell, J. M. (1917). Our psychological association and research. Science, 45, 275–284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, J. M. (1929). Psychology in America. Science, 70, 335–347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark., H. M. (1911). Conscious attitudes. American Journal of Psychology, 22, 214–249.Google Scholar
Creighton, J. E. and Titchener, E. B. (1894). Translator's preface to Wundt, W., Lectures on Animal and Human Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Dallenbach, K. (1967). Autobiography. In Boring, E. G. and Lindzey, G. (Eds.), A History of Psychology in Autobiography. (Vol. 5.) New York, NY: Appleton–Century–Crofts.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (1979). The social origins of modern psychology. In Buss, A. R. (Ed.), Psychology in Social Context. New York, NY: Irvington.Google Scholar
Dodge, R. (1912). The theory and limitations of introspection. American Journal of Psychology, 23, 214–229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dods, J. B. (1850). Electrical Psychology. (edn.) New York, NY: Fowler and Wells.Google Scholar
Dunlap, K. (1912). The case against introspection. Psychological Review, 19, 404–413.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, L. (1962). The Heritage of Industrial Psychology: Walter Dill Scott, First Industrial Psychologist. Privately printed.
Fernberger, S. W. (1932). The American Psychological Association, a historical summary, 1892–1930. Psychological Bulletin, 29, 1–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, A. H. (2000). Contributions of American moral philosophers to psychology in the United States. History of Psychology, 3, 3–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furumoto, L. (1988). Shared knowledge: the Experimentalists, 1904–1929. In Morawski, J. G. (Ed.), The Rise of Experimentation in American Psychology. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Gilman, D. C. (1885). Annual address to the university. Johns Hopkins Circular, 4, 48–49.Google Scholar
Guthrie, R. V. (1988). Even the Rat was White (edn.) Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.Google Scholar
Hale, M. (1980). Human Science and Order: Hugo Münsterberg and the Origins of Applied Psychology. Philadelphia, PA: Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Hall, G. S. (1885). The new psychology. Andover Review, 3, 120–135, 239–248.Google Scholar
Hall, G. S. (1904). Adolescence: Its Psychology and its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology, Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion and Education. (Vols. 1–2.) New York, NY: Appleton.Google Scholar
Hall, G. S. (1917). Jesus, the Christ, in the Light of Psychology. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.Google Scholar
Hall, G. S. (1920). Recreations of a Psychologist. New York, NY: Appleton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, G. S. (1922). Senescence: The Last Half of Life. New York, NY: Appleton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, G. S. (1923). Life and Confessions of a Psychologist. New York, NY: Appleton.Google Scholar
Hulse, S. H. and Green, B. F. (Eds.) (1986). One Hundred Years of Psychological Research in America: G. Stanley Hall and the American Tradition. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Humphrey, G. (1951). Thinking: An Introduction to its Experimental Psychology. New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
James, W. (1876). The teaching of philosophy in our colleges. Nation, 23, 178–179.Google Scholar
James, W. (1884). What is an emotion?Mind, 9, 188–205.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). The Principles of Psychology. (Vols. 1–2.) New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
James, W. (1892a). A plea for psychology as a “natural science.”Philosophical Review, 1, 146–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1892b). Psychology: A Briefer Course. New York, NY: Holt.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (1893). Letter to Hugo Münsterberg, February 21. In Benjamin, L. T., A History of Psychology in Letters. Malden, MA: Blackwell, 2006.Google Scholar
James, W. (1897). The Will to Believe, and Other Essays in Popular Philosophy. New York, NY: Longmans.Google Scholar
James, W. (1907). Pragmatism. New York, NY: Longmans.Google Scholar
James, W. (1909). The Meaning of Truth. New York, NY: Longmans.Google Scholar
James, W. (1920). Letter to Thomas W. Ward (November, 1867). In James, H. (Ed.), Letters of William James. (Vols. 1–2.) Boston, MA: Atlantic Monthly Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jastrow, J. (1887). Physiological psychology. The Christian Union, October 27.
Jastrow, J. (1890). Minor contributions. Studies from the laboratory of experimental psychology of the University of Wisconsin. American Journal of Psychology, 3, 43–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Judd, C. H. (1907). Psychology: General Introduction. Boston, MA: Ginn.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kihlstrom, J. F. (2004). Joseph Jastrow and his duck – or is it a rabbit? Letter to the editor submitted to Trends in the Cognitive Sciences, November 16.
Kline, L. W. (1899). Suggestions towards a laboratory course in comparative psychology. American Journal of Psychology, 10, 399–430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuklick, B. (1977). The Rise of American Philosophy: Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1860–1930. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Külpe, O. (1964). The modern psychology of thinking. In Mandler, G. and Mandler, J. M. (Eds.), Thinking: From Association to Gestalt. New York, NY: Wiley. (Original work published 1912.)Google Scholar
Ladd, G. T. (1887). Elements of Physiological Psychology. New York, NY: Scribner.Google Scholar
Ladd, G. T. (1895). Philosophy of Mind: An Essay in the Metaphysics of Psychology. New York, NY: Scribner.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladd, G. T. and Woodworth, R. S. (1911). Elements of Physiological Psychology. (Rev. edn.) New York, NY: Scribner.Google Scholar
Lange, C. G. (1912). The mechanism of the emotions. In Rand, B. (Ed., Trans.), The Classical Psychologists. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin. (Original work published 1885.)Google Scholar
Leahey, T. H. (1979). Something old, something new: attention in Wundt and modern cognitive psychology. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 15, 242–252.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leahey, T. H. (1981). The mistaken mirror: on Wundt's and Titchener's psychologies. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 17, 273–282.3.0.CO;2-G>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leary, D (1987). Telling likely stories: the rhetoric of the new psychology, 1880–1920. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 23, 315–331.3.0.CO;2-V>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logan, C. (1999). The altered rationale for the choice of a standard animal in experimental psychology: Henry H. Donaldson, Adolf Meyer, and “the” albino rat. History of Psychology, 2, 3–24.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manicas, P. T. (1987). A History and Philosophy of the Social Sciences. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Mayo, E. (1933). The Human Problems of an Industrial Civilization. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
McCosh, J. M. (1886). Psychology: The Cognitive Powers. New York, NY: Scribner.Google Scholar
McCosh, J. M. (1980). The Scottish Philosophy. New York, NY: ASM Press. (Original work published 1875.)Google Scholar
McReynolds, P. (1996). Lightner Witmer: a centennial tribute. American Psychologist, 51, 237–240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McReynolds, P. (1997). Lightner Witmer: His Life and Times. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meumann, E. (1907). Introductory Lectures on Pedagogy and its Physiological Basis. Leipzig: W. Engelmann.Google Scholar
Miner, B. C. (1904). The changing attitude of universities towards psychology. Science, 20, 299–307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, G. E. (1911–1913). Analysis of the Processes of Memory and Mental Representation. Leipzig: Edelmann.Google Scholar
Münsterberg, H. (1888). Voluntary Action. Freiburg, Germany: Mohr.Google Scholar
Münsterberg, H. (1908). On the Witness Stand. New York, NY: Clark Boardman.Google Scholar
Münsterberg, H. (1909). Psychotherapy. New York, NY: Moffat, Yard.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Münsterberg, H. (1912). Vocation and Learning. St. Louis, MO: People's University.Google Scholar
Münsterberg, H. (1913a). Psychology and Industrial Efficiency. New York, NY: Houghton Mifflin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Münsterberg, H. (1913b). The mind of the juryman. With a side-light on women as jurors. Century Illustrated Monthly Magazine, 86, 711–716.Google Scholar
O'Donnell, J. M. (1985). The Origins of Behaviorism: American Psychology, 1870–1920. New York, NY: New York University Press.Google Scholar
Ogden, R. M. (1911). Imageless thought. Psychological Bulletin, 8, 183–197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peirce, C. S. (1982). What pragmaticism is. In Thayer, H. S. (Ed.), Pragmatism: The Classic Writings. Indianapolis, IN: Hackett. (Original work published 1905.)Google Scholar
Peirce, C. S. and Jastrow, J. (1884). On small differences in sensation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 3, 75–83.Google Scholar
Pickren, W. E. and Fowler, R. D. (2003). Professional organizations. In Freedheim, D. K. (Ed.), Handbook of Psychology: Volume 1. History of Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Pillsbury, W. B. (1911). Essentials of Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Porter, N. (1868). The Human Intellect: with an Introduction upon Psychology and the Soul. New York, NY: Scribner.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, E. S. (1997). From Soul to Mind. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Rieber, R. W. (1980). Wundt and the Americans: from flirtation to abandonment. In Rieber, R. W. (Ed.), Wilhelm Wundt and the Making of American Psychology. New York, NY: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roback, A. A. (1952). History of American Psychology. New York, NY: Library Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Routh, D. K. and Reisman, J. M. (2003). Clinical psychology. In Freedheim, D. K. (Ed.), Handbook of Psychology: Vol. 1. History of Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rozenzweig, S. (1984). Hail to Hall. APA Monitor (March), 5–6.
Scarborough, E. and Furumoto, L. (1987). Untold Lives: The First Generation of American Women Psychologists. New York, NY: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Schultz, D. P. and Schultz, S. E. (1992). A History of Modern Psychology. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.Google Scholar
Scott, W. D. (1903). The Theory of Advertising: A Simple Exposition of the Principles of Psychology in their Relation to Successful Advertising. Boston, MA: Small, Maynard.Google Scholar
Scott, W. D. (1906). The Psychology of Public Speaking. New York, NY: Noble and Noble.Google Scholar
Scott, W. D. (1911a). Increasing Human Efficiency in Business. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Scott, W. D. (1911b). Influencing Men of Business. New York, NY: Ronald Press.Google Scholar
Scripture, E. W. (1895). Thinking, Feeling, and Doing. New York, NY: Putnam.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scripture, E. W. (1897). The New Psychology. New York, NY: Scribner.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Small, W. S. (1900). An experimental study of the mental processes of the white rat: I. American Journal of Psychology, 11, 133–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Small, W. S. (1901). An experimental study of the mental processes of the white rat: II. American Journal of Psychology, 12, 206–239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, R. (1997). Norton History of the Human Sciences. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Sokal, M. M. (1971). The unpublished biography of James McKeen Cattell. American Psychologist, 26, 626–635.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sokal, M. M. (1981). An Education in Psychology: James McKeen Cattell's Journal and Letters from Germany and England, 1880–1888. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sokal, M. M. (1992). Origins and early years of the American Psychological Association, 1890–1906. American Psychologist, 47, 111–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sokal, M. M. (2009). James McKeen Cattell, Columbia University, and academic freedom at Columbia University, 1902–1923. History of Psychology, 12, 87–122.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, D. (2006). Elements of the Philosophy of the Human Mind. Whitefish, MT: Kessinger Publishing. (Original work published 1792.)Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1905). The Elements of Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1895a). Simple reactions. Mind, 4, 74–81.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1895b). The “‘type-theory’” of simple reaction. Mind, 4, 506–514.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1896a). The “‘type-theory’” of simple reaction. Mind, 5, 236–241.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1896b). An Outline of Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1898a). A Primer of Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1898b). The postulates of a structural psychology. Philosophical Review, 7, 449–465.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1899). Structural and functional psychology. Philosophical Review, 8, 290–299.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1901–1905). Experimental Psychology: A Manual of Laboratory Practice. (Vols. 1–2.) New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1910). A Textbook of Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1912). The schema of introspection. American Journal of Psychology, 23, 485–508.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1914). Psychology: science or technology?Popular Science Monthly, 39, 51.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1916). On ethnological tests of sensation and perception. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society, 55, 204–236.Google Scholar
Titchener, E. B. (1921). Wilhelm Wundt. American Journal of Psychology, 32, 161–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tweney, R. D. (1987). Programmatic research in experimental psychology: E. B. Titchener's laboratory investigations, 1891–1927. In Ash, M. G. and Woodward, W. R. (Eds.), Psychology in Twentieth-Century Thought and Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tweney, R. D. (1997). Edward Bradford Titchener (1867–1927). In Bringmann, W. G., Lück, H. E., Miller, R., and Early, C. E. (Eds.), A Pictorial History of Psychology. Chicago, IL: Quintessence.Google Scholar
Veysey, L. (1965). The Emergence of the American University. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
von Mayrhauser, R. T. (1989). Making psychology functional: Walter Dill Scott and applied psychology testing in World War I. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 25, 60–72.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Washburn, M. F. (1908). The Animal Mind: A Text-Book of Comparative Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Washburn, M. F. (1916). Movement and Mental Imagery: Outline of a Motor Theory of Consciousness. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it. Psychological Review, 20, 158–177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, R. I. (1978). The Great Psychologists from Aristotle to Freud. (edn.) New York, NY: Lippincott.Google Scholar
White, A. D. (1960). A History of the Warfare of Science with Theology in Christendom. New York, NY: Dover. (Original work published 1896.)Google Scholar
Wigmore, J. (1909). Professor Münsterberg and the psychology of testimony: being a report of the case of Cokestone vs. Münsterberg. Illinois Law Review, 3, 412–413.Google Scholar
Wissler, C. (1901). The correlation of mental and physical tests. Psychological Review Monograph Supplements, 3, No 6.Google Scholar
Witmer, L. (1897). The organization of practical work in psychology. Psychological Review, 4, 116.Google Scholar
Witmer, L. (1902). Analytical Psychology: A Practical Manual for Schools and Normal Colleges. Boston, MA: Ginn.Google Scholar
Witmer, L. (1907). Clinical psychology. The Psychological Clinic, 1, 1–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Wolfe, H. K. (1886). Untersuchungen über das Tongedächtniss [Studies on the memory of tone]. Philosophische Studien, 3, 534–571.Google Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1906). Imageless thought. Journal of Philosophy, Psychology and Scientific Methods, 3, 701–708.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodworth, R. S. (1938). Experimental Psychology. New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1900). Bumerkungen zur Theorie der Gefühle [Remarks on the theory of feeling]. Philosophische Studien, 15, 149–182.Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1902). Outlines of Psychology. (Judd, C. H., Trans.) Leipzig: Engelmann. (Original work published 1897.)Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1907). Über Ausfrageexperimente und über die Methoden zur Psychologie des Denkens [About interrogative experiments and the methods regarding the psychology of thinking]. Psychologische Studien, 3, 301–360.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×