Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-tf8b9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-25T09:36:37.292Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 19 - What the Evolutionary and Cognitive Sciences Offer the Sciences of Crime and Justice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 September 2022

Riadh Abed
Affiliation:
Mental Health Tribunals, Ministry of Justice, UK
Paul St John-Smith
Affiliation:
Hertfordshire Partnership University NHS Foundation Trust, UK
Get access

Summary

Science has become increasingly interdisciplinary, marked by the rapid expansion of social science fields melding with ‘natural’ sciences previously considered less relevant for the study of humans. Psychology in particular now depends heavily on insights from medicine, biology, sociology, genetics and cognitive science and has done so for years. By grounding itself in evolutionary theory, moreover, psychology has moved towards a more mature science of human mind and behaviour. The crime sciences – criminology and criminal justice – are poised to make similar progress. While already interdisciplinary fields, we make the case that the evolutionary and cognitive sciences can unify existing knowledge about crime and justice, can help to pose new and interesting questions to study and can push the fields forward in ways that will benefit not only the scientific world, but society in general as well.

Type
Chapter
Information
Evolutionary Psychiatry
Current Perspectives on Evolution and Mental Health
, pp. 295 - 304
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aharoni, E., Funk, C., Sinnott‐Armstrong, W. and Gazzaniga, M. (2008). Can neurological evidence help courts assess criminal responsibility? Lessons from law and neuroscienceAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences1124, 145160.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (eds.). (1992). The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barnes, J. C., Wright, J. P., Boutwell, B. B., Schwartz, J. A., Connolly, E. J., Nedelec, J. L. and Beaver, K. M. (2014). Demonstrating the validity of twin research in criminologyCriminology52, 588626.Google Scholar
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. and Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition21, 3746.Google Scholar
Beaver, K. M., Barnes, J. C. and Boutwell, B. B. (eds.). (2014). The Nurture versus Biosocial Debate in Criminology: On the Origins of Criminal Behavior and Criminality. New York: SAGE Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Black, D. (1976). The Behavior of Law. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Bogen, J. E., Fisher, E. D. and Vogel, P. J. (1965). Cerebral commissurotomy: a second case report. JAMA, 194, 13281329.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boutwell, B. B. and Adams, C. D. (2020). A research note on Mendelian randomization and causal inference in criminology: promises and considerations. Journal of Experimental Criminology, doi: 10.1007/s11292-020-09436-9.Google Scholar
Brown, R. L., Wood, A., Carter, J. D. and Kannis-Dymand, L. (2022). The metacognitive model of PTSD and metacognitive therapy for PTSD: a systematic review. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 29, 131146.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (1991). Evolutionary personality psychologyAnnual Review of Psychology42, 459491.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. (2007). Evolutionary Psychology: The New Science of the Mind (2nd ed.). New York: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Buss, D. M. and Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Human aggression in evolutionary psychological perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 605619.Google Scholar
Clune, J., Mouret, J. B. and Lipson, H. (2013). The evolutionary origins of modularity. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences280, 20122863.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1996). The Blind Watchmaker: Why the Evidence of Evolution Reveals a Universe without Design. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1984). Elbow Room: The Varieties of Free Will Worth Wanting. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
DeVore, I. and Tooby, J. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modeling. In Kinzey, W. G. (ed.), The Evolution of Human Behavior: Primate Models. Albany: State University of New York Press, pp. 183237.Google Scholar
Duntley, J. D. (2005). Adaptations to dangers from other humans. In Buss, D. M. (ed.), The Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Wiley, pp. 224249.Google Scholar
Duntley, J. D. and Shackelford, T. K. (2008a). Darwinian foundations of crime and lawAggression and Violent Behavior13, 373382.Google Scholar
Duntley, J. D. and Shackelford, T. K. (eds.). (2008b). Evolutionary Forensic Psychology. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Falconer, D. S. and Mackay, T. F. C. (1996). Introduction to Quantitative Genetics (4th ed.). Harlow: Longmans Green.Google Scholar
Franklin, S. (1995). Artificial Minds. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1980). The role of language for conscious experience: observations from split-brain man. Progress in Brain Research, 54, 689696).Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (1995). Principles of human brain organization derived from split-brain studiesNeuron, 14, 217228.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). Forty-five years of split-brain research and still going strongNature Reviews Neuroscience6, 653659.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2008). The law and neuroscienceNeuron60, 412415.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2009). The Cognitive Neurosciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2018). The Consciousness Instinct: Unraveling the Mystery of How the Brain Makes the Mind. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. and Sperry, R. W. (1967). Language after section of the cerebral commissuresBrain90, 131148.Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S. and Steven, M. S. (2005). Neuroscience and the lawScientific American Mind16, 4249Google Scholar
Glickstein, M. (2007). What does the cerebellum really do? Current Biology17, R824R827.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gómez-Robles, A., Hopkins, W. D. and Sherwood, C. C. (2014). Modular structure facilitates mosaic evolution of the brain in chimpanzees and humansNature Communications5, 19.Google Scholar
Goodenough, O. R. and Tucker, M. (2010). Law and cognitive neuroscience. Annual Review of Law and Social Science, 6, 6192.Google Scholar
Harris, S. (2012). Free Will. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Heinrich, J., Boyd, R. and Richardson, P. J. (2008). Five misunderstandings about cultural evolutionHuman Nature19, 119137.Google Scholar
Kavish, N. and Boutwell, B. (2018). The unified crime theory and the social correlates of crime and violence: problems and solutionsJournal of Criminal Psychology, 4, 287301.Google Scholar
Lalumiere, M. L., Harris, G. T. and Rice, M. E. (2001). Psychopathy and developmental instability. Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 7592.Google Scholar
Marcus, G. (2009). Kluge: The Haphazard Evolution of the Human Mind. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
McGilchrist, I. (2010). Reciprocal organization of the cerebral hemispheresDialogues in Clinical Neuroscience12, 503515.Google Scholar
Mealey, L. (1995). The sociobiology of sociopathy: an integrated evolutionary model. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 18, 523599.Google Scholar
Miller, M. B., Sinnott-Armstrong, W., Young, L., King, D., Paggi, A., Fabri, M., Polonara, G. and Gazzaniga, M. S. (2010). Abnormal moral reasoning in complete and partial callosotomy patients. Neuropsychologia, 48, 22152220.Google Scholar
Mitchell, J. M., Bogenschutz, M., Lilienstein, A., Harrison, C., Kleiman, S., Parker-Guilbert, K., … Doblin, R. (2021). MDMA-assisted therapy for severe PTSD: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled phase 3 studyNature Medicine27, 10251033.Google Scholar
Nilsson, D. E. and Pelger, S. (1994). A pessimistic estimate of the time required for an eye to evolveProceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences256, 5358.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (1997). How the Mind Works. New York: W.W. Norton & Co.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2002). The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2007). The Stuff of Thought: Language as a Window into Human Nature. New York: Penguin.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2010). The cognitive niche: coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 89938999.Google Scholar
Pinker, S. (2011). The Better Angels of Our Nature: Why Violence Has Declined. New York: Viking Books.Google Scholar
Pitchford, I. (2001). The origins of violence: is psychopathy an adaptation? Human Nature Review, 1, 2836.Google Scholar
Polderman, T. J., Benyamin, B., De Leeuw, C. A., Sullivan, P. F., Van Bochoven, A., Visscher, P. M. and Posthuma, D. (2015). Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nature Genetics, 47, 702709.Google Scholar
Pratt, T. C. and Cullen, F. T. (2000). The empirical status of Gottfredson and Hirschi’s general theory of crime: a meta‐analysis. Criminology, 38, 931964.Google Scholar
Premack, D. and Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515526.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L. and Loftus, E. F. (2013). Memory and law: what can cognitive neuroscience contribute? Nature Neuroscience, 16, 119123.Google Scholar
Shackelford, T. K. (ed.). (2020). The SAGE Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology (vols 1–4). London: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Sperry, R. W. (1976). Mental phenomena as causal determinants in brain function. In Globus, G. G., Maxwell, G. and Savodnik, I. (eds.), Consciousness and the Brain. Boston, MA: Springer, pp. 163177.Google Scholar
Sporns, O. and Betzel, R. F. (2016). Modular brain networks. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 613640.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Symons, D. (1992). On the use and misuse of Darwinism in the study of human behavior. In Barkow, J. H., Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 137159.Google Scholar
Tanksley, P. T., Barnes, J. C., Boutwell, B. B., Arseneault, L., Caspi, A., Danese, A., Fisher, H. L. and Moffitt, T. E. (2020). Identifying psychological pathways to polyvictimization: evidence from a longitudinal cohort study of twins from the UK. Journal of experimental criminology, 16, 431461.Google Scholar
Tielbeek, J. J., Barnes, J. C., Popma, A., Polderman, T. J., Lee, J. J., Perry, J. R., Posthuma, D. and Boutwell, B. B. (2018). Exploring the genetic correlations of antisocial behaviour and life history traitsBJPsych Open4, 467470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tooby, J. and Cosmides, L. (1992). The psychological foundations of culture. In Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L. and Tooby, J. (eds.), The Adapted Mind: Evolutionary Psychology and the Generation of Culture. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 19136.Google Scholar
Tooby, J. and DeVore, I. (1987). The reconstruction of hominid behavioral evolution through strategic modeling. In Kinzey, W. G. (ed.), The Evolution of Human Behavior. Albany: State University of New York Press, pp. 183238.Google Scholar
Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557.Google Scholar
Tyler, T. R. (1990). Why People Obey the Law. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Vazsonyi, A. T., Mikuška, J. and Kelley, E. L. (2017). It’s time: a meta-analysis on the self-control–deviance link. Journal of Criminal Justice, 48, 4863.Google Scholar
Volz, L. J. and Gazzaniga, M. S. (2017). Interaction in isolation: 50 years of insights from split-brain research. Brain, 140, 20512060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilson, E. O. (1999). Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge. New York: Vintage.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×