Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-20T17:53:11.777Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 1 - Social learning among wild orang-utans

Is it affective?

from Part I - On the evolutionary foundations of affective social learning processes

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 August 2019

Daniel Dukes
Affiliation:
Université de Fribourg, Switzerland
Fabrice Clément
Affiliation:
Université de Neuchâtel, Switzerland
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, we apply the affective social learning (ASL) concept to the social learning of natural skill sets in immature orang-utans since it can serve as an illustration of the majority of learning that occurs in wild apes. Most orang-utan social learning happens during everyday tasks and without any active involvement of the role model. Consequently, detecting the emotional state(s) of the role model is nearly impossible. We focus therefore on the emotional responses of the immature learners to the role models’ behaviours. Our data on peering (attentive, sustained close-range watching of conspecifics), which is often followed by selective practice of the observed behaviour by the peerer, suggests that there is some highly specific emotional arousal of the immatures during social learning. The role models’ actions with the object seem to play a central role in the learning process. However, immatures appear to decide on their own whether to attend to the information or not, as in affective observation, the second stage of ASL. Developmental changes in role-model preferences support the notion that trust in the role model is critical for ASL to work. Given that we can use the learners’ responses as proof of the affective states of the role models, ASL may be an important part of the mechanism that guides and optimizes the acquisition of learned skills in wild great apes. However, the lower we set the bar for the affective states (or emotions) of the role models for ASL to work, the more difficult it is to verify their presence and the more ASL will overlap with ordinary social learning.

Type
Chapter
Information
Foundations of Affective Social Learning
Conceptualizing the Social Transmission of Value
, pp. 25 - 40
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aureli, F., & Schaffner, C. M. (2002). Relationship assessment through emotional mediation. Behaviour, 139(2), 393420.Google Scholar
Aureli, F., & Whiten, A. (2003). Emotions and behavioral flexibility. Primate Psychology, 289323.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. (1991). Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530532.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & van Schaik, C. P. (2012). Even simple forms of social learning rely on intention attribution in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 129138.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates. Animal Cognition, 13, 119. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0263-7Google Scholar
Cacioppo, J. T., Tassinary, L. G., & Berntson, G. (2007). Handbook of psychophysiology. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M. (1990). How monkeys see the world: Inside the mind of another species. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Clutton-Brock, T. H. (2002). Breeding together: Kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebrates. Science, 296(5565), 6972. doi:10.1126/science.296.5565.69Google Scholar
Corp, N., & Byrne, R. (2001). Sex difference in chimpanzee handedness. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 123, 6268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, F. B. M. (2001). The ape and the sushi master: cultural reflections of a primatologist. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I., & Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1975). Ethology, the biology of behavior. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.Google Scholar
Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2013). Natural pedagogy. In Banaji, M. R. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 127132). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hardus, M. E., Lameira, A. R., Singleton, I., Morrogh-Bernard, H. C., Knott, C. D., Ancrenaz, M., … Wich, S. A. (2009). A description of the orangutan’s vocal and sound repertoire, with a focus on geographical variation. In Wich, S. A., Utami-Atmoko, S. S., Mitra Setia, T., & van Schaik, C. P. (Eds.), Orangutans: Geographic variation in behavioral ecology and conservation (pp. 4964). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hopper, L. M. (2010). “Ghost” experiments and the dissection of social learning in humans and animals. Biological Reviews, 85(4), 685701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howard, L. H., Wagner, K. E., Woodward, A. L., Ross, S. R., & Hopper, L. M. (2017). Social models enhance apes’ memory for novel events. Scientific Reports, 7, 40926.Google Scholar
Humle, T., & Snowdon, C. T. (2008). Socially biased learning in the acquisition of a complex foraging task in juvenile cottontop tamarins, Saguinus oedipus. Animal Behaviour, 75, 267277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humle, T., Snowdon, C. T., & Matsuzawa, T. (2009). Social influences on ant-dipping acquisition in the wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) of Bossou, Guinea, West Africa. Animal Cognition, 12, S37S48. doi:10.1007/s10071-009-0272-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jaeggi, A. V., Dunkel, L. P., van Noordwijk, M. A., Wich, S. A., Sura, A. A. L., & van Schaik, C. P. (2010). Social learning of diet and foraging skills by wild immature Bornean orangutans: Implications for culture. American Journal of Primatology, 72(1), 6271. doi:10.1002/ajp.20752Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., & Harris, P. L. (2016). The ontogeny of cultural learning. Child Development, 87(3), 633642.Google Scholar
Lehmann, L., Wakano, J. Y., & Aoki, K. (2013). On optimal learning schedules and marginal value of cumulative cultural evolution. Evolution, 67, 14351445.Google Scholar
Lonsdorf, E. V. (2006). What is the role of mothers in the acquisition of termite-fishing behaviors in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii)? Animal Cognition, 9, 3646.Google Scholar
Maestripieri, D. (2009). The past, present and future of primate psychology. In Maestripieri, D. (Ed.), Primate psychology (pp. 117). Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Martins, E. M. G., & Burkart, J. M. (2013). Common marmosets preferentially share difficult to obtain food items. Folia Primatologica, 84(3–5), 281282.Google Scholar
Matsuzawa, T., Biro, D., Humle, T., Inoue-Nakamura, N., Tonooka, R., & Yamakoshi, G. (2008). Emergence of culture in wild chimpanzees: education by master-apprenticeship. In Matsuzawa, T. (Ed.), Primate origins of human cognition and behavior (pp. 557574). Tokyo, Japan: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meulman, E. J. M., Seed, A. M., & Mann, J. (2013). If at first you don’t succeed … Studies of ontogeny shed light on the cognitive demands of habitual tool use. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 368(1630), 20130050.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (2004). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Perry, S. (2009). Social influence and the development of food processing techniques in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys (Cebus Capucinus) at Lomas Barbudal, Costa Rica. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 9999.Google Scholar
Rapaport, L. G. (2011). Progressive parenting behavior in wild golden lion tamarins. Behavioral Ecology, 22(4), 745754.Google Scholar
Sargent, B., & Mann, J. (2009). From social learning to culture: Intrapopulation variation in bottlenose dolphins. In Laland, K. N. & Galef, B. G. (Eds.), The question of animal culture (pp. 152173). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schiel, N., & Huber, L. (2006). Social influences on the development of foraging behavior in free-living common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 68(12), 11501160.Google Scholar
Schuppli, C., Forss, S. I. F., Meulman, E. J. M., Atmoko, S. U., Noordwijk, M., & van Schaik, C. P. (2017). The effects of sociability on exploratory tendency and innovation repertoires in wild Sumatran and Bornean orangutans. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 15464.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schuppli, C., Forss, S. I. F., Meulman, E. J. M., Zweifel, N., Lee, K. C., Rukmana, E., … van Schaik, C. P. (2016). Development of foraging skills in two orangutan populations: Needing to learn or needing to grow? Frontiers in Zoology, 13(43), 117.Google Scholar
Schuppli, C., Meulman, E. J. M., Forss, S. I. F., Aprilinayati, F., van Noordwijk, M. A., & van Schaik, C. P. (2016). Observational social learning and socially induced practice of routine skills in immature wild orang-utans. Animal Behaviour, 119, 8798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Silvia, P. J. (2008). Interest: The curious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 5760. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00548.xGoogle Scholar
Snowdon, C. T., & Boe, C. Y. (2003). Social communication about unpalatable foods in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117(2), 142148.Google Scholar
Tarnaud, L. (2004). Ontogeny of feeding behavior of Eulemur fulvus in the dry forest of Mayotte. International Journal of Primatology, 25(4), 803824. doi:10.1023/b:ijop.0000029123.78167.63CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tarnaud, L., & Yamagiwa, J. (2008). Age-dependent patterns of intensive observation on elders by free-ranging juvenile Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) within foraging context on Yakushima. American Journal of Primatology, 70(12), 11031113. doi:10.1002/ajp.20603Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Schaik, C. P. (2016). The primate origins of human nature. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., & Burkart, J. M. (2010). Mind the gap: Cooperative breeding and the evolution of our unique features. In Kappeler, P. M. & Silk, J. B. (Eds.), Mind the gap: Tracing the origins of Human Universals (pp. 477496). Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Graber, S., Schuppli, C., & Burkart, J. M. (2017). The ecology of social learning in animals and its link with intelligence. Spanish Journal of Psychology, 19(e99), 112.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. A., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape? Learning & Behavior, 32, 3652.Google Scholar
Wich, S. A., Krützen, M., Lameira, A. R., Nater, A., Arora, N., Bastian, M. L., … van Schaik, C. P. (2012). Call cultures in Orang-Utans? PLoS One, 7(5), e36180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×