Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T01:39:04.705Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Modeling brain connectivity

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2011

Russell A. Poldrack
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Jeanette A. Mumford
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Thomas E. Nichols
Affiliation:
University of Oxford
Get access

Summary

Introduction

One of the oldest debates in the history of neuroscience centers on the localization of function in the brain; that is, whether specific mental functions are localized to specific brain regions or instead rely more diffusely upon the entire brain (Finger, 1994). The concept of localization first arose from work by Franz Gall and the phrenologists, who attempted to localize mental functions to specific brain regions based on the shape of the skull. Although Gall was an outstanding neuroscientist (Zola-Morgan, 1995), he was wrong in his assumption about how the skull relates to the brain, and phrenology was in the end taken over by charlatans. In the early twentieth century, researchers such as Karl Lashley argued against localization of function, on the basis of research showing that cortical lesions in rats had relatively global effects on behavior. However, across the twentieth century the pendulum shifted toward a localizationist view, such that most neuroscientists now agree that there is at least some degree of localization of mental function. At the same time, the function of each of these regions must be integrated in order to acheive coherent mental function and behavior. These concepts have been referred to as functional specialization and functional integration, respectively (Friston, 1994).

Today, nearly all neuroimaging studies are centered on functional localization. However, there is increasing recognition that neuroimaging research must take functional integration seriously to fully explain brain function (Friston, 2005; McIntosh, 2000).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×