Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-77c89778f8-m42fx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-20T05:33:52.786Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - The question of ‘what to imitate’: inferring goals and intentions from demonstrations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 December 2009

Malinda Carpenter
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
Josep Call
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany
Chrystopher L. Nehaniv
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Kerstin Dautenhahn
Affiliation:
University of Hertfordshire
Get access

Summary

Getting a robot to imitate is a difficult task. Currently, researchers are tackling difficult issues such as the ‘correspondence problem’ (Nehaniv and Dautenhahn, 2002) of how to map different aspects of a demonstration to one's own imitative response, how to decide whom, when and what to imitate, and how to evaluate success (Dautenhahn and Nehaniv, 2002a, b). From our perspective, the most interesting of these issues is the question of what to imitate, that is, which aspects of a demonstration robots should copy. This is because in many situations, instead of copying others' actions and their results on the environment exactly, it is often more useful to extract and adopt the goal of the demonstrator's actions, and only copy the actions and results of the demonstrator if they are intended and relevant or essential to the task.

To illustrate, imagine the following scenarios. A robot is presented with the following demonstration and then told to imitate.

  1. (1) An actor approaches a lever and pushes it with his foot.

  2. Here, copying the actor's action and its result might be appropriate: the robot should also push the lever with its foot (assuming it has one).

  3. (2) An actor whose arms are broken pushes the lever with his foot.

  4. Now it might be more appropriate for the robot to use a different action to achieve the same result: it could push the lever with its hands (assuming it has them), as the constraint of not having usable hands – the normal thing to use to push things – does not apply to it.

  5. […]

Type
Chapter
Information
Imitation and Social Learning in Robots, Humans and Animals
Behavioural, Social and Communicative Dimensions
, pp. 135 - 152
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alissandrakis, A., Nehaniv, D. and Dautenhahn, K. (2002). Imitation with ALICE: learning to imitate corresponding actions across dissimilar embodiments. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man and Cybernetics – Part A: Systems and Humans, 32, 482–96.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkins, C. K., Klein, E. D. and Zentall, T. R. (2002). Imitative learning in Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the bidirectional control procedure. Animal Learning & Behavior, 30, 275–81.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atkins, C. K. and Zentall, T. R. (1996). Imitative learning in male Japanese quail (Coturnix japonica) using the two-action method. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 110, 316–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldwin, D. A. and Baird, J. A. (2001). Discerning intentions in dynamic human action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5, 171–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baldwin, D. A., Baird, J. A., Saylor, M. M. and Clark, M. A. (2001). Infants parse dynamic action. Child Development, 72, 708–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behne, T., Carpenter, M. and Tomasello, M. (2006a). From attention to intention: 18-month-olds use others' focus of attention for action interpretation. Poster presented at the meetings of the International Conference on Infancy Studies, Kyoto, Japan.
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., van Veen, A. and Tomasello, M. (2006b). Infants use attention and reaction information to infer others' goals. Unpublished data.
Bekkering, H., Wohlschläger, A. and Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53A, 153–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bellagamba, F. and Tomasello, M. (1999). Re-enacting intended acts: comparing 12- and 18-month-olds. Infant Behavior and Development, 22, 277–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Billard, A., Epars, Y., Cheng, G. and Schaal, S. (2003). Discovering imitation strategies through categorization of multi-dimensional data. Proceedings of the 2003 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, Las Vegas, Nevada.Google Scholar
Billard, A. and Schaal, S. (2001). Robust learning of arm trajectories through human demonstration. In Proceedings of the 2001 IEEE/RSJ International Conference on Intelligent Robots and Systems, Maui, Hawaii.Google Scholar
Boesch, C. and Boesch, H. (2000). The Chimpanzees of the Tai Forest. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Breazeal, C. and Scassellati, B. (2002). Challenges in building robots that imitate people. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 363–90.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (1999). Imitation without intentionality. Using string parsing to copy the organization of behaviour. Animal Cognition, 2, 63–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. (2001). Body imitation in an enculturated orangutan. Cybernetics and Systems, 32, 97–119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J. and Carpenter, M. (2002). Three sources of information in social learning. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 211–28.Google Scholar
Call, J. and Carpenter, M. (2003). On imitation in apes and children. Infancia y Aprendizaje, 26, 325–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., Carpenter, M. and Tomasello, M. (2005). Copying results and copying actions in the process of social learning: chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8, 151–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., Hare, B., Carpenter, M. and Tomasello, M. (2004). Unwilling or unable: Chimpanzees' understanding of human intentional action. Developmental Science, 7, 488–98.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (1998). Distinguishing intentional from accidental actions in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 112, 192–206.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, F. M., Heyes, C. M. and Goldsmith, A. R. (1999). Stimulus learning and response learning by observation in the European starling in a two-object/two-action test. Animal Behaviour, 58, 151–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M. (2006). Instrumental, social, and shared goals and intentions in imitation. In Rogers, S. and Williams, J. (eds.), Imitation and the Development of the Social Mind: Lessons from Typical Development and Autism. New York: Guilford, 48–70.
Carpenter, M., Akhtar, N. and Tomasello, M. (1998). Fourteen through 18-month-old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 21, 315–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M. and Call, J. (2002). The chemistry of social learning. Developmental Science, 5, 22–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2002). Understanding ‘prior intentions’ enables 2-year-olds to imitatively learn a complex task. Child Development, 73, 1431–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Call, J. and Tomasello, M. (2005). Twelve- and 18-month-olds imitate actions in terms of goals. Developmental Science, 8, F13–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Custance, D., Whiten, A. and Bard, K. (1995). Can young chimpanzees imitate arbitrary actions?Behaviour, 132, 839–58.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dautenhahn, K. (1994). Trying to imitate – a step towards releasing robots from social isolation. In Gaussier, P. and Nicoud, J.-D. (eds.), Proceedings from Perception to Action Conference. Lausanne, Switzerland: IEEE Computer Society Press.Google Scholar
Dautenhahn, K. (1995). Getting to know each other – artificial social intelligence for autonomous robots. Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 16, 333–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (2002a). The agent-based perspective on imitation. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1–40.Google Scholar
Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.) (2002b). Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Demiris, J. and Hayes, G. (2002). Imitation as a dual-route process featuring predictive and learning components: a biologically plausible computational model. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 327–61.Google Scholar
Galef, B. and Zentall, T. (1988). Social learning: Psychological and biological perspectives. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.Google Scholar
Gergely, G. (2003). What should a robot learn from an infant? Mechanisms of action interpretation and observational learning in infancy. Connection Science, 15, 191–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G., Bekkering, H. and Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herman, L. M. (2002). Vocal, social, and self-imitation by bottlenosed dolphins. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and ArtifactsCambridge, MA: MIT Press, 63–108.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. and Galef, B. G. (1996). Social Learning in Animals. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. and Demiris, Y. (2004). Abstraction in recognition to solve the correspondence problem for robot imitation. In Proceedings of TAROS, Essex.Google Scholar
Klein, E. D. and Zentall, T. R. (2003). Imitation and affordance learning by pigeons (Columba livia). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 414–19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozima, H. and Yano, H. (2001). A robot that learns to communicate with human caregivers. In Proceedings of the 1st International Workshop on Epigenetic Robotics: Modeling Cognitive Development in Robotic Systems. Lund University Cognitive Studies, 47–52.Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meltzoff, A. N., Gopnik, A. and Repacholi, B. M. (1999). Toddlers' understanding of intentions, desires, and emotions: explorations of the dark ages. In Zelazo, P. D., Astington, J. W. and Olson, D. R. (eds.), Developing Theories of Intention: Social Understanding and Self-control. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 17–41.Google Scholar
Mottley, K. and Heyes, C. (2003). Budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) copy virtual demonstrators in a two-action test. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 117, 363–70.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M. and Matsuzawa, T. (1999). Factors influencing imitation of manipulatory actions in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 113, 128–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Myowa-Yamakoshi, M. and Matsuzawa, T. (2000) Imitation of intentional manipulatory actions in chimpanzees. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 114, 381–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nehaniv, C. L. and Dautenhahn, K. (1998). Mapping between dissimilar bodies: affordances and the algebraic foundations of imitation. In Demiris, J. and Birk, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the European Workshop on Learning Robots (EWLR-7), (Edinburgh, Scotland, 20 July 1998).Google Scholar
Nehaniv, C. L. and Dautenhahn, K. (2001). Like me? – Measures of correspondence and imitation. Cybernetics and Systems: An International Journal, 32, 11–51.Google Scholar
Nehaniv, C. L. and Dautenhahn, K. (2002). The correspondence problem. In Dautenhahn, K. and Nehaniv, C. L. (eds.), Imitation in Animals and Artifacts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 41–61.Google Scholar
Rakoczy, H., Tomasello, M. and Striano, T. (2004). Young children know that trying is not pretending – a test of the “behaving-as-if” construal of children's early concept of “pretense.” Developmental Psychology, 40, 388–99.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L. and Gallesse, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1996). Do apes ape? In Heyes, C. M. and Galef, B. G. Jr. (eds.). Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture. New York: Academic Press, 319–46.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. and Carpenter, M. (2005). The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 70(1, Serial No. 279).
Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T. and Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C. and Ratner, H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 495–552.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Užgiris, I. Č. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zentall, T. R., Sutton, J. E. and Sherburne, L. M. (1996). True imitative learning in pigeons. Psychological Science, 7, 343–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×