Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7bb8b95d7b-cx56b Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-09-08T02:20:27.635Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Biocultural Co-Construction of Lifespan Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 July 2009

Shu-chen Li
Affiliation:
Senior Research Scientist Max Planck Institute for Human Development, Berlin, Germany
Paul B. Baltes
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Bildungsforschung, Berlin
Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Frank Rösler
Affiliation:
Philipps-Universität Marburg, Germany
Get access

Summary

ABSTRACT

Neuroscientists have long recognized that the brain is an open, adaptive system and that the organism's experiences are environmentally contextualized. However, the proposition that sociocultural contexts may exert reciprocal influences on neurobiological mechanisms is rarely considered and could not be empirically explored until very recently. This chapter reviews an emerging trend of interdisciplinary research aimed at exploring the effects of sociocultural influences on human brain functioning. Viewed through the lens of a cross-level biocultural co-constructive framework, human development is co-constructed by biology and culture through a series of reciprocal interactions between developmental processes and plasticity at different levels.

“Mental exercise facilitates a greater development of … the nervous collaterals in the part of the brain in use. In this way, preexisting connections between groups of cells could be reinforced …”

(Ramón y Cajal, 1894, Croonian lecture to the Royal Society).

INTRODUCTION

For more than a century, neuroscientists have been interested in how neural mechanisms implement mental experiences and how experiences may exert reciprocal influences on the neurobiological substrates of the mind. Ramón y Cajal enunciated what today is known as the “activity-dependent synaptic plasticity and memory hypothesis.” Since the formulation and discoveries of synaptic processes of memory and learning, a great variety of neurochemical mechanisms involved in tuning synaptic efficacy have been identified (see Bliss, Collingridge, & Morris, 2003, for a recent review).

Type
Chapter
Information
Lifespan Development and the Brain
The Perspective of Biocultural Co-Constructivism
, pp. 40 - 58
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolphs, R. (2001). The neurobiology of social cognition. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 231–239CrossRef
Aoki, K. (1986). A stochastic model of gene-culture coevolution suggested by the “culture historical hypothesis” for the evolution of adult lactose absorption in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 83, 2929–2933CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltes, P. B. (1987). Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: On the dynamics between growth and decline. Developmental Psychology, 23, 611–626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baltes, P. B., & Kliegl, R. (1992). Further testing of the limits of cognitive plasticity: Negative age differences in a mnemonic skill are robust. Developmental Psychology, 28, 121–125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beja-Pereira, A., Luikart, G., England, P. R., Bradley, D. G., Jann, O. C., Bertorelle, G., et al. (2003). Gene–culture coevolution between cattle milk protein genes and human lactase genes. Nature Genetics, 35, 311–313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bjorklund, D. F., & Pellegrini, A. D. (2000). Child development and evolutionary psychology. Child Development, 71, 1687–1708CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bliss, T. V. P., Collingridge, G. L., & Morris, R. G. M. (Eds.). (2003). Long-term potentiation: Enhancing neuroscience for 30 years. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 358, 607–829
Bornstein, M. H., Tal, J., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (1991). Parenting in crosscultural perspective: The United States, France, and Japan. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Cultural approaches to parenting (pp. 69–90). Hillsdale, NJ:ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature–nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A bioecological model. Psychological Review, 101, 568–586CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model. Psychology and Aging, 17, 85–100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., Hawkley, L. C., & Berntson, G. G. (2003). The anatomy of loneliness. Current Directions in Psychological Sciences, 12, 71–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Changeux, J.-P. (1985). Neuronal man. New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Cheour, M., Ceponiene, R., Lehtokoski, A., Luuk, A., Allik, J., Alho, K., & Näätänen, R. (1998). Development of language-specific phoneme representations in the infant brain. Nature Neuroscience, 1, 351–353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2001). Where brain, body, and world collide. In G. M. Edelman & J.-P. Changeux (Eds.), The brain (pp. 257–280). London: TransactionGoogle Scholar
Clark, D. A., Mitra, P. P., & Wang, S. S. H. (2001). Scalable architecture in mammalian brains. Nature, 411, 189–193CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colcombe, S. J., Erickson, K.-I., Raz, N., Webb, A. G., Cohen, N. J., McAuley, E., & Kramer, A. F. (2003). Aerobic fitness reduces brain tissue loss in aging humans. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 58A, M176–M180Google Scholar
Cole, M. (1999). Culture in development. In M. H. Bornstein & M. E. Lamb (Eds.), Developmental psychology: An advanced textbook (pp. 73–123). Mahwah, NJ:ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Haan, M., Pascalis, O., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face recognition in human infants. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 199–209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunbar, R. (1993). Co-evolution of neocortical size, group size, and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 681–735CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elbert, T., Pantev, C., Wienbruch, C., Rockstroh, B., & Taub, E. (1995). Increased cortical representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science, 270, 305–307CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gauvain, M. (1995). Thinking in niches: Sociocultural influences on cognitive development. Human Development, 38, 24–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, G. (1998). Normally occurring environmental and behavioral influences of gene activity: From central dogma to probabilistic epigenesis. Psychological Review, 105, 792–802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gottlieb, G. (2002). Developmental-behavioral initiation of evolutionary change. Psychological Review, 109, 211–218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenough, W. T., & Black, J. E. (1992). Induction of brain structure by experience: Substrates for cognitive development. In M. R. Gunnar & C. A. Nelson (Eds.), Developmental behavioral neuroscience (Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology, Vol. 24, pp. 155–200). Hillsdale, NJ:ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Gross, C. G. (2000). Neurogenesis in the adult brain: Death of a dogma. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1, 67–73CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamilton, R. H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (1998). Cortical plasticity associated with Braille learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 168–174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York:WileyGoogle Scholar
Joffe, T. H. (1997). Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. Journal of Human Evolution, 32, 593–605CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. H. (2001). Functional brain development in humans. Nature Review Neuroscience, 2, 475–483CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karni, A., Meyer, G., Jezzard, P., Adams, M. M., Turner, R., & Ungerleider, L. G. (1995). Functional MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature, 377, 155–158Google ScholarPubMed
Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G., & Gage, F. H. (1997). More hippocampal neurons in adult mice living in an enriched environment. Nature, 386, 493–495CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kim, K. H. S., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388, 171–174CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kingsbury, M. A., Lettman, N. A., & Finlay, B. L. (2002). Reduction of early thalamic input alters adult corticocortical connectivity. Developmental Brain Research, 138, 35–43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., & Larsen, J. T. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: A cultural look at New Look. Psychological Science, 14, 201–206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Feldman, M. W. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 131–175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, S.-C. (2003). Biocultural orchestration of developmental plasticity across levels: The interplay of biology and culture in shaping the mind and behavior across the life span. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 171–194CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, S.-C., & Lindenberger, U. (2002). Co-constructed functionality instead of functional normality: Dynamic biocultural co-construction of brain–behaviour mappings. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 761–762CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnusson, D. (1988). Individual development from an interactional perspective: A longitudinal study. Hillsdale, NJ:ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences (USA), 97, 4398–4403CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyamoto, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2002). Cultural variation in correspondence bias: The critical role of attitude diagnosticity of socially constrained behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 1239–1248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohammed, A. H., Henriksson, B. G., Soderström, S., Ebendal, T., Olsson, T., & Seckl, J. R. (1993). Environmental influences on the central nervous system and their implications for the aging rat. Behavioural Brain Research, 23, 182–191Google Scholar
Nakamura, H., Kobajashi, S., Ohashi, Y., & Ando, S. (1999). Age-changes of brain synapses and synaptic plasticity in response to an enriched environment. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 56, 307–3153.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, C. A. (2000). Neural plasticity and human development: The role of early experience in sculpting memory systems. Developmental Science, 3, 115–136CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, C. A., & Carver, L. J. (1998). The effects of stress and trauma on brain and memory: A view from developmental cognitive neuroscience. Developmental Psychopathology, 10, 793–809CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neville, H. J., & Bavelier, D. (1998). Neural organization and plasticity of language. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 8, 254–258CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neville, H. J., & Mills, D. (1997). Epigenesis of language. Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 3, 282–2923.0.CO;2-Q>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108, 291–310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohnishi, T., Matsuda, H., Asada, T., Hirakata, M., Nishikawa, M., Katoh, A., & Imabayashi, E. (2001). Functional anatomy of musical perception in musicians. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 754–760CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulesu, E., McCrory, E., Fazio, F., Menoncello, L., Brunswick, N., Cappa, S. F., et al. (2000). A cultural effect on brain function. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 91–96CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quartz, S. R., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: A constructivist manifesto. Behavioral and Brain Science, 20, 537–596CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reis, H. T., Collins, W. A., & Berscheid, E. (2000). The relationship context of human behavior and development. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 844–872CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2002). New visions of the aging mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 394–400CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenzweig, M. R. (1996). Aspects of the search for neural mechanisms of memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 1–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saito, S., Kobajashi, S., Ohashi, Y., Igarashi, M., Komiya, Y., & Ando, S. (1994). Decreased synaptic density in aged brains and its prevention by rearing under enriched environment as revealed by synaptophysin contents. Journal of Neuroscience Research, 39, 57–62CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlaggar, B. L., Brown, T. T., Lugar, H. M., Visscher, K. M., Miezin, F. M., & Petersen, S. E. (2002). Functional neuroanatomical differences between adults and school-age children in the processing of single words. Science, 296, 1476–1479CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schooler, C., & Mulatu, M. S. (2004). Occupational self-direction, intellectual functioning, and self-directed orientation in older workers: Findings and implications for individuals and societies. American Journal of Sociology, 110, 161–197CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, T., Lindenberger, U., & Baltes, P. B. (2003). Plasticity of memory for new learning in very old age: A story of major loss?Psychology and Aging, 18, 306–318CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, M., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2002). Are developmental disorders like cases of adult brain damage? Implications from connectionist modelling. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 772–787CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Whorf, B. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Cambridge, MA: MIT PressGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×