Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-13T23:27:46.876Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Emotion, Learning, and the Brain: From Classical Conditioning to Cultural Bias

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 July 2009

Elizabeth A. Phelps
Affiliation:
Professor of Psychology and Neural Science New York University, USA
Paul B. Baltes
Affiliation:
Max-Planck-Institut für Bildungsforschung, Berlin
Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Frank Rösler
Affiliation:
Philipps-Universität Marburg, Germany
Get access

Summary

ABSTRACT

Classical conditioning, described by Pavlov, has emerged as an important tool in our efforts to understand the mechanisms of emotional learning. Using a classical fear conditioning paradigm, research with nonhuman animals has identified the amygdala as a critical structure for emotional learning. This chapter reviews how studies in humans have extended the role of the amygdala to social means of emotional learning and culturally acquired race bias. Although cultural knowledge and some forms of social communication may be uniquely human characteristics, how emotional value is expressed in these domains seems to rely on basic mechanisms that are shared across species.

OVERVIEW

The basic principles of classical conditioning were identified by Ivan Pavlov more than a century ago when he showed that dogs would salivate to the ringing of a bell that had previously been paired with the delivery of food. More recently, investigators have used classical conditioning paradigms to help understand the neural mechanisms of emotional learning. These studies have focused on classical fear conditioning. In a typical fear conditioning paradigm, a neutral stimulus, called the conditioned stimulus (CS), is paired with an aversive event, the unconditioned stimulus (UCS). After a few pairings, the animal learns that CS predicts the UCS, and this previously neutral stimulus begins to elicit a fear response, called the conditioned response (CR). Using this paradigm, scientists studying nonhuman animals have been able to map the neural pathways of emotional learning from stimulus input to response output (see LeDoux, 2002, for a review).

Type
Chapter
Information
Lifespan Development and the Brain
The Perspective of Biocultural Co-Constructivism
, pp. 200 - 216
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Hamann, S., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Phelps, E. A.. (1999). Recognition of facial emotion in nine individuals with bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1111–1117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal Social Psychology, 63, 575–582CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., Adolphs, R., Rockland, C., & Damasio, A. R. (1995). Double dissociation of conditioning and declarative knowledge relative to the amygdala and hippocampus in humans. Science, 269 (5227), 1115–1118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biernat, M., & Crandall, C. S. (1999). Racial attitudes. In J. P. Robinson, P. H. Shaver, & L. S. Wrightsman (Eds.), Measures of political attitudes (pp. 291–412). San Diego: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Buchel, C., Morris, J., Dolan, R. J., & Friston, K. J. (1998). Brain systems mediating aversive conditioning: An event-related fMRI study. Neuron, 20 (5), 947–957CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M. (1997). Neurobiology of fear responses: The role of the amygdala. Journal of Neuropsychiatry: Clinical Neuroscience, 9, 382–402Google ScholarPubMed
Falls, W. A., Miserendino, M. J., & Davis, M. (1992). Extinction of fear-potentiated startle: Blockade by infusion of an NMDA antagonist into the amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 12, 854–863CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013–1027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Funayama, E. S., Grillon, C., Davis, M., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). A double dissociation in the affective modulation of startle in humans: Effects of unilateral temporal lobectomy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 721–729CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of maternal modelling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behaviour in toddlers. Behaviour Research Therapy, 40, 279–287CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., & Banaji, M. R. (1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4–27CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, J. L., & Schwartz, J. L. (1998). Measuring individual differences in social cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamann, S. (2002). Cognitive and neural mechanisms of emotional memory. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 394–400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hart, A. J., Whalen, P. J., Shin, L. M., McInerney, S. C., Fischer, H., & Rauch, S. L. (2000). Differential response in the human amygdala to racial outgroup vs. ingroup face stimuli. Neuroreport, 11, 2351–2355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hugdahl, K., & Ohman, A. (1977). Effects of instruction on acquisition and extinction of electrodermal responses to fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 3, 608–618Google ScholarPubMed
Hygge, S., & Ohman, A. (1978). Modeling processes in the acquisition of fears: Vicarious electrodermal conditioning to fear-relevant stimuli. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 271–279CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapp, B. S., Frysinger, R. C., Gallagher, M., & Haselton, J. R. (1979). Amygdala central nucleus lesions: Effect on heart rate conditioning in the rabbit. Physiological Behavior, 23, 1109–1117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kensinger, E. A., & Corkin, S. (2003). Effect of negative emotional content on working memory and long-term memory. Emotion, 3, 378–393CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kluver, H., & Bucy, P. C. (1937). “Psychic blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. American Journal of Physiology, 119, 352–353Google Scholar
LaBar, K. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., LeDoux, J. E., & Phelps, E. A. (1998). Human amygdala activation during conditioned fear acquisition and extinction: A mixed-trial fMRI study. Neuron, 20, 937–945CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LaBar, K. S., LeDoux, J. E., Spencer, D. D., & Phelps, E. A. (1995). Impaired fear conditioning following unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 15, 6846–6855CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1990). Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97, 377–395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain. New York: Simon and SchusterGoogle Scholar
LeDoux, J. E. (2002). Synaptic self: How our brains become who we are. New York: Viking
Ling, S., Phelps, E. A., Holmes, B. D., & Carrasco, M. (2004). Emotion potentiates attentional effects in early vision [Abstract]. Journal of Vision, 4(8), 623a. Available: http://journalofvision.org/4/8/623/, doi:10.1167/4.8.623CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mather, M., Canli, T., English, T., Whitfield, S., Wais, P., Ochsner, K., et al. (2004). Amygdala responses to emotionally valenced stimuli in older and younger adults. Psychological Science, 15, 259–263CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConahay, J. P. (1986). Modern racism, ambivalence, and the modern racism scale. In J. F. Dovidio & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Prejudice, discrimination and racism (pp. 91–125). Orlando, FL: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Mineka, S., Davidson, M., Cook, M., & Keir, R. (1984). Observational conditioning of snake fear in rhesus monkeys. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 93, 355–372CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nosek, B. A., Cunningham, W. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2000, February). Measuring implicit attitudes on the internet. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Nashville, TNGoogle Scholar
Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 466–478CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ohman, A., & Mineka, S. (2001). Fears, phobias, and preparedness: Toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning. Psychological Review, 108, 483–522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olsson, A., Nearing, K., Zeng, J., & Phelps, E. A. (2004, October). Learning by observing: Neural correlates of fear learning through social observation. Paper presented at the 34th annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, CAGoogle Scholar
Olsson, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2004). Learned fear of “unseen” faces. Psychological Science, 15, 822–828CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papez, J. W. (1937). A proposed mechanism of emotion. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry, 79, 217–224Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A. (2002). The cognitive neuroscience of emotion. In M. S. Gazzaniga, R. B. Ivry, & G. R. Magnum (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of mind (2nd ed., pp. 537–576). New York:NortonGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E. A. (2004). Human emotion and memory: Interactions of the amygdala and hippocampal complex. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14, 198–202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2005). Animal models of human attitudes: Integrations across behavioral, cognitive, social neuroscience. In J. T. Cacioppo, P. S. Visser, & C. L. Pickett (Eds.), Social neuroscience: People thinking about thinking people (pp. 229–244). Cambridge, MA:MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Phelps, E. A., Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., & LeDoux, J. E. (2004). Extinction learning in humans: Role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron, 43, 897–905CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A., O'Connor, K. J., Cunningham, W. A., Funayama, E. S., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., & Banaji, M. R. (2000). Performance on indirect measures of race evaluation predicts amygdala activation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 729–738CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phelps, E. A., O'Connor, K. J., Gatenby, J. C., Gore, J. C., Grillon, C., & Davis, M. (2001). Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 437–441CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schuman, H., Steeh, C., & BoBo, L. (1997). Racial attitudes in America: Trends and interpretations.Cambridge, MA:Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Singer, Y., Seymour, B., O'Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L. (1956). Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdaloid complex in monkeys. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology, 49, 381–391CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalen, P. J. (1998). Fear, vigilance, and ambiguity: Initial neuroimaging studies of the human amygdala. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 177–188CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×