Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-s2hrs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-05T03:20:31.484Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

7 - Interacting time scales in personality (and cognitive) development: Intentions, emotions, and emergent forms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Marc D. Lewis
Affiliation:
Department of Human Development and Applied Psychology, Ontario Institute for Studies in Education, University of Toronto
Nira Granott
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Dallas
Jim Parziale
Affiliation:
University of Massachusetts, Boston
Get access

Summary

The relations between time scales of development, particularly microdevelopment and macrodevelopment, remain largely unexplored, both theoretically and empirically. How do processes in real time affect the laying down of shape and structure over developmental time? How do momentary events create novel forms or patterns that last for years or even a lifetime? The opposite causal direction is just as mysterious. How do longstanding patterns, only visible on a developmental scale, influence activity in real time, and very often in a way that serves to further entrench those patterns? Like the activity of a sculptor, small movements create lasting form that directs subsequent movements, as the sculptor comes to recognize the shape to which the medium gives rise. Yet there is no sculptor. Development may better resemble M. C. Escher's famous lithograph of two hands drawing each other: the form creates itself.

One of the most serious conundrums for cognitive developmentalists has been the integration of time scales of learning and development. When learning and development are simply viewed as different amounts of the same thing, theorists fail to look for the transformations through which they are related (Lerner, 1995). Conversely, when they are considered incommensurable, theorists tend to dismiss one and concentrate on the other rather than recognize their intrinsic complementarity (Granott, 1998). When learning and development are viewed as distinct yet complementary, their relationship is often assumed to be unidimensional and context-free, a perspective that obscures the multiple, nonlinear pathways that connect them (Fischer & Granott, 1995).

Type
Chapter
Information
Microdevelopment
Transition Processes in Development and Learning
, pp. 183 - 212
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2002

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abraham, F. D. (1995). Dynamics, bifurcation, self-organization, chaos, mind, conflict, insensitivity to initial conditions, time, unification, diversity, free will, and social responsibility. In R. Robertson & A. Combs (Eds.), Chaos theory in psychology and the life sciences (pp. 155–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Blaney, P. H. (1986). Affect and memory: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 99, 229–246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buck, R. (1985). Prime theory: An integrated view of motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92, 389–413CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cantor, N., & Harlow, R. (1994). Social intelligence and personality: flexible life-task pursuit. In R. J. Sternberg & P. Ruzgis (Eds.), Personality and intelligence (pp. 137–168). New York: Cambridge University Press
Cantor, N., Norem, J. K., Niedenthal, P. M., Langston, C. A., & Brower, A. M. (1987). Life tasks, self-concept ideals, and cognitive strategies in a life transition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1178–1191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Case, R. (1988). The whole child: Toward an integrated view of young children's cognitive, social, and emotional development. In A. D. Pellegrini (Ed.), Psychological bases for early education (pp. 155–184). New York: Wiley
Case, R., Hayward, S., Lewis, M. D., & Hurst, P. (1988). Toward a neo-Piagetian theory of cognitive and emotional development. Developmental Review, 8, 1–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York: Avon
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organization of temperament. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 633–652CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derryberry, D., & Tucker, D. M. (1994). Motivating the focus of attention. In P. Niedenthal & S. Kitayama (Eds.), The heart's eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention (pp. 170–196). San Diego: Academic PressCrossRef
Dore, J. (1989). Monologue as reenvoicement of dialogue. In K. Nelson (Ed.), Narratives from the crib (pp. 231–260). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Dunn, J. (1988). The beginnings of social understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Dweck, C. S., & Leggett, E. L. (1988). A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychological Review, 95, 256–273CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism. New York: Basic Books
Elman, J. L., Bates, E. A., Johnson, M. H., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Epstein, S. (1993). Implications of cognitive-experiential self-theory for personality and developmental psychology. In D. C. Funder, R. D. Parke, C. Tomlinson-Keasey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Studying lives through time: Personality and development (pp. 399–438). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: Norton
Estrada, P., Arsenio, W. F., Hess, R. D., & Holloway, S. D. (1987). Affective quality of the mother-child relationship: Longitudinal consequences for children's school-relevant functioning. Developmental Psychology, 23, 210–215CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., & Ayoub, C. (1996). Analyzing development of working models of close relationships: Illustration with a case of vulnerability and violence. In G. G. Noam & K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Development and vulnerability in close relationships (pp. 173–199). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Fischer, K. W., & Granott, N. (1995). Beyond one-dimensional change: Parallel, concurrent, socially distributed processes in learning and development. Human Development, 38, 302–314CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, K. W., Shaver, P. R., & Carnochan, P. (1990). How emotions develop and how they organise development. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 81–127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: Origins of communication, self, and culture. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Fogel, A., Messinger, D., Dickson, K. L., & Hsu, H. (1999). Posture and gaze in early mother-infant communication: Synchronization of developmental trajectories. Developmental Science, 2, 325–332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, W. J. (1995). Societies of brains. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Freeman, W. J. (2000). Emotion is essential to all intentional behaviors. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 209–235). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Freeman, W. J., & Baird, B. (1987). Relation of olfactory EEG to behavior: Spatial analysis. Behavioral Neuroscience, 101, 393–408CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frijda, N. H. (1986). The emotions. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press
Frijda, N. H. (1993a). Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (pp. 381–403). New York: Guilford
Frijda, N. H. (1993b). The place of appraisal in emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 357–387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, B. (1993). Development as a robust natural process. In W. Stein & F. J. Varela (Eds.), Thinking about biology (pp. 123–148). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley
Gottlieb, G. (1991). Experiential canalization of behavioral development: Theory. Developmental Psychology, 27, 4–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Granott, N. (1998). We learn, therefore we develop: Learning versus development – or developing learning? In M. C. Smith & T. Pourchot (Eds.), Adult learning and development: Perspectives from educational psychology (pp. 15–34). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Greenough, W. T., & Schwark, H. D. (1984). Age-related aspects of experience effects upon brain structure. In R. N. Emde & R. J. Harmon (Eds.), Continuities and discontinuities in development (pp. 69–91). New York: PlenumCrossRef
Harkness, K. L., & Tucker, D. M. (2000). Motivation of neural plasticity: Neural mechanisms in the self-organization of depression. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 186–208). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: A theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–340CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T. (1991). Development of self-regulatory and self-evaluative processes: Costs, benefits, and tradeoffs. In M. R. Gunnar & L. A. Sroufe (Eds.), The Minnesota symposia on child psychology, Vol. 23: Self processes and development (pp. 125–165). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Horowitz, M. J. (1998). Cognitive psychodynamics: From conflict to character. New York: Wiley
Isen, A. M. (1990). The influence of positive and negative affect on cognitive organization: Some implications for development. In N. Stein, B. Leventhal, & T. Trabasso (Eds.), Psychological and biological processes in the development of emotion (pp. 75–94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Izard, C. E. (1984). Emotion-cognition relationships and human development. In C. E. Izard, J. Kagan, & R. B. Zajonc (Eds.), Emotions, cognition and behavior (pp. 17–37). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Izard, C. E., Ackerman, B., Schoff, K., & Fine, S. (2000). Self-organization of discrete emotions, emotion patterns, and emotion–cognition relations. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 15–36). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Kauffman, S. (1995). At home in the universe: The search for the laws of self-organization and complexity. New York: Oxford University Press
Keating, D. P. (1990a). Charting pathways to the development of expertise. Educational Psychologist, 25, 243–267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keating, D. P. (1990b). Developmental processes in the socialization of cognitive structures. In Development and learning: Proceedings of a symposium in honour of Wolfgang Edelstein on his 60th birthday. Berlin: Max Planck Institute
Keating, D. P., & Miller, F. K. (2000). The dynamics of emotional development: Models, metaphors, and methods. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 373–392). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Kegan, R. (1982). The evolving self: Problem and process in human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press
Kelso, J. A. S. (1995). Dynamic patterns: The self-organization of brain and behavior. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view. Developmental Psychology, 25, 343–354CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kramer, P. D. (1994). Listening to prozac. New York: Penguin
Labouvie-Vief, G., Hakim-Larsen, J., DeVoe, M., & Schoeberlein, S. (1989). Emotions and self-regulation: A life-span view. Human Development, 32, 279–299CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamb, M. E. (1987). Predictive implications of individual differences in attachment. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 55, 817–824CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeDoux, J. E. (1992). Emotion and the amygdala. In J. P. Appleton (Ed.), The amygdala (pp. 339–351). New York: Wiley
Lerner, R. M. (1995). The place of learning within the human development system: A developmental contextual perspective. Human Development, 38, 361–366CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (1993a). Early socioemotional predictors of cognitive competency at four years. Developmental Psychology, 29, 1036–1045CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (1993b). Emotion-cognition interactions in early infant development. Cognition and Emotion, 7, 145–170CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (1995). Cognition-emotion feedback and the self-organization of developmental paths. Human Development, 38, 71–102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D. (1997). Personality self-organization: Cascading constraints on cognition-emotion interaction. In A. Fogel, M. C. Lyra, & J. Valsiner (Eds.), Dynamics and indeterminism in developmental and social processes (pp. 193–216). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Lewis, M. D. (2000a). Emotional self-organization at three time scales. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 37–55). New York: Cambridge University Press
Lewis, M. D. (2000b). The promise of dynamic systems approaches for an integrated account of human development. Child Development, 71, 36–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. D., & Douglas, L. (1998). A dynamic systems approach to cognition– emotion interactions in development. In M. F. Mascolo & S. Griffin (Eds.), What develops in emotional development? (pp. 159–188). New York: PlenumCrossRef
Lewis, M. D., Lamey, A. V., & Douglas, L. (1999). A new dynamic systems method for the analysis of early socioemotional development. Developmental Science, 2, 458–476CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magai, C., & McFadden, S. H. (1995). The role of emotions in social and personality development: History, theory, and research. New York: Plenum
Malatesta, C. Z., & Wilson, A. (1988). Emotion/cognition interaction in personality development: A discrete emotions, functionalist analysis. British Journal of Social Psychology, 27, 91–112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mareschal, D., & Thomas, M. S. C. (in press). Self-organization in normal and abnormal cognitive development. In A. F. Kalverboer, & A. Gramsbergen (Eds.), Brain and behavior in human development: A source book. Dordrecht: Kluwer
Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (1995). A cognitive-affective system theory of personality: Reconceptualizing situations, dispositions, dynamics, and invariance in personality structure. Psychological Review, 102, 246–268CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newell, K. M., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (1998). Applications of nonlinear dynamics to developmental process modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Morrow, J. (1991). A prospective study of depression and distress following a natural disaster: The 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 115–121CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1987). Towards a cognitive theory of emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 1, 29–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York: Oxford University Press
Pascual-Leone, J. (1987). Organismic processes for neo-Piagetian theories: A dialectical causal account of cognitive development. International Journal of Psychology, 22, 531–570CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pascual-Leone, J., & Goodman, D. (1979). Intelligence and experience: A neopiagetian approach. Instructional Science, 8, 301–367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1974/1976). The grasp of consciousness: Action and concept in the young child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Originally published 1974.)
Polivy, J. (1998). The effects of behavioral inhibition: Integrating internal cues, cognition, behavior, and affect. Psychological Inquiry, 9, 181–204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Prigogine, I., & Stengers, I. (1984). Order out of chaos. New York: Bantam
Renninger, K. A., Hidi, S., & Krapp, A. (1992). The role of interest in learning and development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Rolls, E. T. (1999). The brain and emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press
Rothbart, M. K., Ziaie, H., & O’Boyle, C. G. (1992). Self-regulation and emotion in infancy. In N. Eisenberg & R. A. Fabes (Eds.), New directions for child development, No. 55 (pp. 7–23). San Francisco: Jossey-BassCrossRef
Rumelhart, D. E., Hinton, G. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1986). A general framework for parallel distributed processing. In J. L. McClelland, D. E. Rumelhart, & the PDP Research Group (Eds.), Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (pp. 45–76). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Sameroff, A. J. (1983). Developmental systems: Contexts and evolution. In P. H. Mussen (Series Ed.), & W. Kessen (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, vol. 1 (pp. 237–294). New York: Wiley
Scheff, T. J. (1987). Creativity and repetition: A theory of the coarse emotions. In J. Rabow, G. Platt, & M. Goldman (Eds.), Advances in psychoanalytic sociology (pp. 70–100). Malabor, FL: Krieger
Scherer, K. R. (2000). Emotions as episodes of subsystem synchronization driven by nonlinear appraisal processes. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 70–99). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Schore, A. N. (1994). Affect regulation and the origin of the self: The neurobiology of emotional development. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Schore, A. N. (1997). Early organization of the nonlinear right brain and development of a predisposition to psychiatric disorders. Development and Psychopathology, 9, 595–631CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schore, A. N. (2000). The self-organization of the right brain and the neurobiology of emotional development. In M. D. Lewis & I. Granic (Eds.), Emotion, development, and self-organization: Dynamic systems approaches to emotional development (pp. 155–185). New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRef
Siegler, R. S. (1997). Concepts and methods for studying cognitive change. In E. Amsel & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), Change and development: Issues of theory, method, and application (pp. 77–97). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Siegler, R. S., & Jenkins, E. (1989). How children discover new strategies. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Skarda, C. A., & Freeman, W. J. (1987). How brains make chaos in order to make sense of the world. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 161–195CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 813–838CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teasdale, J. D., & Barnard, P. J. (1993). Affect, cognition, and change: Re-modelling depressive thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Thelen, E., & Corbetta, D. (this volume). Microdevelopment and dynamic systems: Applications to infant motor development
Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1994). A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: Bradford/MIT Press
Thelen, E., & Ulrich, B. D. (1991). Hidden skills: A dynamic systems analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 56(1) (Serial No. 223)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson, R. A. (1991). Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educational Psychology Review, 3, 269–307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of definition. In N. A. Fox (Ed.), The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(2–3) (Serial No. 240)
Thompson, R. A. (1998). Early sociopersonality development. In W. Damon (Series Ed.), & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (5th edn., pp. 25–104). New York: Wiley
Thorne, A., & Klohnen, E. (1993). Interpersonal memories as maps for personality consistency. In D. C. Funder, R. D. Parke, C. Tomlinson-Keasey, & K. Widaman (Eds.), Studying lives through time: Personality and development (pp. 223–253). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association
Tomkins, S. S. (1984). Affect theory. In K. R. Scherer & P. Ekman (Eds.), Approaches to emotion (pp. 163–195). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Tucker, D. M. (1992). Developing emotions and cortical networks. In M. R. Gunnar & C. Nelson (Eds.), Minnesota symposia on child psychology, Vol. 24: Developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp. 75–128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Vallacher, R. R., & Nowak, A. (1997). The emergence of dynamical social psychology. Psychological Inquiry, 8, 73–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Maas, H. L. J. (1998). The dynamical and statistical properties of cognitive strategies: Relations between strategies, attractors, and latent classes. In K. M. Newell & P. C. M. Molenaar (Eds.), Applications of nonlinear dynamics to developmental process modeling (pp. 161–176). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Maas, H. L. J., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (1992). Stagewise cognitive development: An application of catastrophe theory. Psychological Review, 99, 395–417CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geert, P. (1991). A dynamic systems model of cognitive and language growth. Psychological Review, 98, 3–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Geert, P. (1994). Dynamic systems of development: Change between complexity and chaos. New York: Prentice Hall/Harvester Wheatsheaf
Geert, P. (1995). Dimensions of change: A semantic and mathematical analysis of learning and development. Human Development, 38, 322–331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Geert, P. (this volume). Developmental dynamics, intentional action, and fuzzy sets
van Gelder, T., & Port, R. F. (1995). It's about time: An overview of the dynamical approach to cognition. In R. F. Port & T. van Gelder (Eds.), Mind as motion: Explorations in the dynamics of cognition (pp. 1–43). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×