Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-t6hkb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-13T02:26:29.047Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Biological and Experiential Influences on Psychological Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  03 May 2011

Michael Rutter
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, King's College, London
Daniel P. Keating
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Get access

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Over recent decades there have been major developments in our understanding of the various ways in which biological and experiential factors influence psychological development (Rutter & Rutter, 1993; Rutter 2000a, 2006a; Shonkoff & Phillips, 2000). In this chapter, I focus on the conceptual issues that are involved, on possible causal mechanisms, on effects on abnormal or suboptimal functioning, and on implications for intervention. Throughout, the arguments are based on empirical research findings (placing most emphasis on those that have been replicated by independent research groups) and, when dealing with causal mechanisms, reference is made to studies in biology and medicine, as well as in psychology, to draw conclusions on likely processes.

DEVELOPMENT

In much of the literature, there has been a tendency to seek to partition influences into those that are genetic (G) and those that are environmental (E), as if between them they accounted for all possibilities. This is a seriously misleading oversimplification because it focuses exclusively on individual differences without taking account of the universals of development (see Rutter, 2002). Also, it wrongly assumes that G and E are separate and involve no co-action (see Rutter, 2006a) and ignores the role of chance. Accordingly, I start with a consideration of some of the key features of developmental processes.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Human Memory: Theory and Practice. Hove & London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Baringa, M. (2003). Newborn neurons search for meaning. Science, 299, 32–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barker, D. J. P. (1999). Fetal programming and public health. In O'Brien, P. M. S., Wheeler, T., and Barker, D. J. P. (Eds.), Fetal Programming: Influences on Developmental and Disease in Later Life (pp. 3–11). London: RCOG Press.Google Scholar
Barker, D. J. P. (2007). The origins of the developmental origins theory. Journal of Internal Medicine, 261, 412–17.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., & Roe, K. (2001). Language development in children with unilateral brain injury. In Nelson, C. A. & Luciana, M. (Eds.), Handbook of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience (pp. 281–307). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bateson, P. P. (1966). The characteristics and context of imprinting. Biological Review, 41, 177–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bateson, P., Barker, D., Clutton-Brock, T., Deb, D., D'Udine, B., Foley, R. A. et al. (2004). Developmental plasticity and human health. Nature, 430, 419–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, P., & Martin, P. (1999). Design for a Life: How Behaviour Develops. London: Jonathan Cape.Google Scholar
Bebbington, P. (1996). The origins of sex differences in depressive disorder: Bridging the gap. International Review of Psychiatry, 8, 295–332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bebbington, P. (1998). Editorial: Sex and depression. Psychological Medicine, 28, 1–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beckett, C., Maughan, B., Rutter, M., Castle, J., Colvert, E., Groothues, C., et al. (2006). Do the effects of early severe deprivation on cognition persist into early adolescence? Findings from the English and Romanian Adoptees study. Child Development, 77, 696–711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, R. Q. (1968). A reinterpretation of the direction of effects in studies of socialization. Psychological Review, 75, 81–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, G. R., & Whelan, J. (1991). The Childhood Environment and Adult Disease. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Brent, D., & Weersing, V. (2008). Affective disorder. In Rutter, M., Bishop, D., Pine, D., Scott, S., Stevenson, J., Taylor, E., Thapar, A. (Eds.), Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. 5th edition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Bretherton, I. (1999). Updating the “internal working model” construct: Some reflections. Attachment and Human Development, 1, 343–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cadoret, R. J., Troughton, E., O'Gormon, T. W., & Heywood, E. (1986). An adoption study of genetic and environmental factors in drug abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 43, 1131–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cadoret, R. J., Yates, W. R., Troughton, E., Woodworth, G., & Stewart, M. A. (1995). Adoption study demonstrating two genetic pathways to drug abuse. Archives of General Psychiatry, 52, 42–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cameron, N. M., Parent, C., Champagne, F. A., Fish, E. W., Ozaki-Kuroda, K., & Meaney, M. J. (2005). The programming of individual differences in defensive responses and reproductive strategies in the rat through variations in maternal care. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29, 843–65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T. E., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297, 851–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (1993). When do individual differences matter? A paradoxical theory of personality coherence. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 247–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Gene-environment interactions in psychiatry: Joining forces with neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 583–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., Cannon, M., McClay, J., Murray, R., Harrington, H., et al. (2005). Moderation of the effect of adolescent-onset cannabis use on adult psychosis by a functional polymorphism in the C0MT gene: Longitudinal evidence of a gene-environment interaction. Biological Psychiatry, 57, 1117–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301, 386–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ceci, S. J., & Papierno, P. B. (2005). The rhetoric and reality of gap closing: When the “have-nots” gain but the “haves” gain even more. American Psychology, 60, 149–60.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champagne, F., Chretien, P., Stevenson, C. W., Zhang, T. Y., Gratton, A., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Variations in nucleus accumbens dopamine associated with individual differences in maternal behaviour in the rat. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 4113–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Champion, L. A., Goodall, G. M., & Rutter, M. (1995). Behavioural problems in childhood and stressors in early adult life: A 20-year follow-up of London schoolchildren. Psychological Medicine, 25, 231–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coe, S. L., & Lubach, G. R. (2000). Prenatal influences on neuroimmune set points in infancy. Annals of New York Academy of Sciences, 917, 468–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Thomsen, A. K., & Wadsworth, M. E. (2001) Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 87–127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cooke, R. W. I., & Abernethy, L. J. (1999). Cranial magnetic resonance imaging and school performance in very low birthweight infants in adolescence. Archives of Disease in Childhood: Fetal Neonatal Edition, 81, F116–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farrer, L. A., Cupples, L. A., Haines, J. L., Hyman, B., Kukull, W. A., Mayeux, R., Myers, R., Pericak-Vance, M. A., Risch, N., Duijn, C. M., for the APOE and Alzheimer Disease Meta Analysis Consortium (1997). Effects of age, sex, and ethnicity on the association between apolipoprotein E genotype and Alzheimer disease. Journal of the American Medical Association, 278, 1349–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 171–91.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2000). IQ gains, WISC subtests, and fluid g: g theory and the relevance of Spearman's hypothesis to race. In Bock, G. R., Goode, J. A., & Webb, K. (Eds.), The Nature of Intelligence. London: Wiley.Google Scholar
Giedd, J. N., Lalonde, F. M., Celano, M. J., White, S. L., Wallace, G. L., Lee, N. R., & Lenroot, R. K. (2009). Anatomical brain magnetic resonance imaging of typically developing children and adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 465–70.Google ScholarPubMed
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C. et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 101, 8174–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldapple, K., Segal, Z., Garson, C., Lau, M., Bieling, P., Kennedy, S., & Mayberg, H. (2004). Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: Treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 61, 34–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (1991). Growing together and growing apart: The nongenetic forces on children in the same family. In McGuffin, P. & Murray, R. (Eds.), The New Genetics of Mental Illness (pp. 212–24). Oxford: Heinemann Medical.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greenough, W. T., & Black, J. E. (1992). Induction of brain structure by experience: Substrates for cognitive development. In Gunnar, M. R. & Nelson, C. A. (Eds.), Developmental Behavior Neuroscience (pp. 155–200). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development. Child Development, 58, 539–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grigorenko, E. L., Wood, F. B., Meyer, M. S., & Pauls, D. L. (2000). Chromosome 6p influences on different dyslexia-related cognitive processes: Further confirmation. American Journal of Human Genetics, 66, 715–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, C. G. (2000). Neurogenesis in the adult brain: Death of a dogma. Nature Reviews – Neuroscience, 1, 67–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gunnar, M., & Vázquez, D. M. (2006). Stress, neurobiology, and developmental psychopathology. In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 2 (pp. 533–77). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Harris, J. R. (1998). The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do. London: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Hennessey, J. W., & Levine, S. (1979). Stress, arousal, and the pituitary-adrenal system: a psychoendocrine hypothesis. In Sprague, J. M. & Epstein, A. N. (Eds.), Progress in Psychobiology and Physiological Psychology (pp. 79–111). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Horn, G. (1990). Neural bases of recognition memory investigated through an analysis of imprinting. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 329, 133–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (2005). Brain and Visual Perception. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R. (2002). Neural Plasticity: The Effects of Environment on the Development of the Cerebral Cortex. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Jacobson, B., Eklund, G., Hamberger, L., Linnarsson, D., Sedvall, G., & Valverius, M. (1987). Perinatal origin of adult self-destructive behavior. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 76, 364–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensen, A. R. (1997). The puzzle of nongenetic variance. In Sternberg, R. J. & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.), Intelligence, Heredity, and Environment (pp. 42–88). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1980). Perspectives on continuity. In Brim, O. G. & Kagan, J. (Eds.), Constancy and Change in Human Development (pp. 26–74). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1981). The Second Year: The Emergence of Self-Awareness. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S. (1996). Major depression and generalised anxiety disorder. Same genes, (partly) different environments – revisited. British Journal of Psychiatry, 168 (suppl. 30), 68–75.Google Scholar
Kendler, K. S. (2005). “A gene for…” The nature of gene action in psychiatric disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 1243–52.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendler, K. S., & Baker, J. H. (2007). Genetic influences on measures of the environment: A systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 37, 615–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Kessler, R. C., Walters, E. E., MacLean, C., Neale, M. C., Heath, A. C., & Eaves, L. J. (1995). Stressful life events, genetic liability, and onset of an episode of major depression in women. American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 833–42.Google ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Gardner, C. O. (2000). Stressful life events and previous episodes in the etiology of major depression in women: An evaluation of the “Kindling” hypothesis. American Journal of Psychiatry 157, 1243–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendler, K. S., Thornton, L. M., & Gardner, C. O. (2001). Genetic risk, number of previous depressive episodes, and stressful life events in predicting onset of major depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 582–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, K. H. S., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K.-M., & Hirsch, J. (1997). Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388, 171–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
King, S., Mancini-Marïe, A., Brunet, A., Meaney, M., Laplante, D., & Walker, E. (2009). Prenatal maternal stress from a natural disaster predicts dermatoglyphic asymmetry in humans. Development and Psychopathology, 21(2), 343–53.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kreppner, J., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Colvert, E., Hawkins, A., Stevens, S., et al. (2007). Normality and impairment following profound early institutional deprivation: A longitudinal follow-up into early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 43(4), 931–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhl, P. K., Andruski, J. E., Chistovich, I. A., Chistovich, L. A., Kozhevnikova, E. V., Ryskina, V. L., Stolyarova, E. I., Sundberg, U., & Lacerda, F. (1997). Cross-language analysis of phonetic units in language addressed to infants. Science, 277, 684–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansford, J., Malone, P., Dodge, K., Pettit, G., Bates, J., & Crozier, J. (2006). A 12-year prospective study of patterns of social information processing problems and externalizing behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 34(5), 715–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lansford, J., Malone, P., Stevens, K., Bates, J., Pettit, G., & Dodge, K. (2006). Developmental trajectories of externalizing and internalizing behaviors: Factors underlying resilience in physically abused children. Development and Psychopathology, 18(1), 35–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levy, F., & Hay, D. (Eds.) (2001). Attention, Genes, and ADHD. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). The construct of resilience: A critical evaluation and guidelines for future work. Child Development, 71, 543–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayes, L. C. (1999). Developing brain and in-utero cocaine exposure: Effects on neural ontogeny. Development and Psychopathology, 11, 685–714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, C., Fearon, P., & Murray, R. M. (2000). Neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia 12 years on: Data and doubts. In Rapoport, J. L. (Ed.), Child Onset of ‘Adult’ Psychopathology: Clinical and Research Advances (pp. 193–220). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.Google Scholar
Meaney, M. J., & Szyf, M. (2005). Environmental programming of stress responses through DNA methylation: Life at the interface between a dynamic environment and a fixed genome. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 7, 103–23. Review.Google Scholar
Molenaar, P. C. M., Boomsma, D. I., & Dolan, C. V. (1993). A third source of developmental differences. Behavior Genetics, 23, 519–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naugler, C. T., & Ludman, M. D. (1996). Fluctuating asymmetry and disorders of developmental origin. American Journal of Medical Genetics, 66, 15–20.3.0.CO;2-V>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A., & Bloom, F. E. (1997). Child development and neuroscience. Child Development, 68, 970–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, C. A., Haan, M., & Thomas, K. M. (2006). Neuroscience and Cognitive Development: The Role of Experience and the Developing Brain. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
O'Brien, P. M. S., Wheeler, T., & Barker, D. J. P. (Eds.) (1999). Fetal Programming: Influences on Developmental and Disease in Later Life. London: RCOG Press.Google Scholar
Pasamanick, R., & Knobloch, H. (1966). Retrospective studies on the epidemiology of reproductive casualty: Old and new. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 12, 7–26.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Bergeman, C. S. (1991). The nature of nurture: Genetic influences on “environmental” measures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 373–427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., & Daniels, D. (1987). Why are children in the same family so different from one another? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 10, 1–15.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (1988). Nature and Nurture during Infancy and Early Childhood. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J., McClearn, G. E., & McGuffin, P. (2001). Behavioral Genetics, 4th Edition. New York: Worth.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Rutter, M. (1998). Child development, molecular genetics, and what to do with genes once they are found. Child Development, 69, 1223–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Post, R. (1992). Transduction of psychosocial stress into the neurobiology of recurrent affective disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 999–1010.Google ScholarPubMed
Quinton, D., & Rutter, M. (1988). Parenting Breakdown: The Making and Breaking of Intergenerational Links. Aldershot: Avebury.Google Scholar
Risch, N., Herrell, R., Lehner, T., Liang, K., Eaves, L., Hoh, J., Griem, A., Kovacs, M., Ott, J., & Merikangas, K. (2009). Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical Association, 301(23), 2462–71.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robins, L. (1966). Deviant Children Grown Up. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Roder, B., & Neville, H. (2003). Developmental functional plasticity. In Grafman, J. & Robertson, I. H. (Eds.), Handbook of Neuropsychology, 2nd Edition, Vol 9 (pp. 231–70). New York: Elsevier Science.Google Scholar
Rowe, D. C. (1994). The Limits of Family Influence: Genes, Experience, and Behaviour. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1989). Pathways from childhood to adult life. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 23–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (1993). An overview of paediatric neuropsychiatry. In Besag, F. & Williams, R. (Eds.), The Brain and Behaviour: Organic Influences on the Behaviour of Children. Special Supplement to Educational and Child Psychology, 10, 4–11.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1995). Causal concepts and their testing. In Rutter, M. & Smith, D. (Eds.), Psychosocial Disorders in Young People: Time Trends and Their Causes. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (1996). Transitions and turning points in developmental psychopathology: As applied to the age span between childhood and mid-adulthood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 19, 603–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (1997). Comorbidity: Concepts: claims and choices. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 7, 265–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (1998). Practitioner review: Routes from research to clinical practice paper in child psychiatry: Retrospect and prospect. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 39, 805–16.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (1999a). Resilience concepts and findings: Implications for family therapy. Journal of Family Therapy, 21, 119–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (1999b). The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture. Autism: Two-way interplay between research and clinical work. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 169–88.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2000a). Psychosocial influences: Critiques, findings, and research needs. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 375–405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2000b). Resilience reconsidered: Conceptual considerations, empirical findings, and policy implications. In Shonkoff, J. P. & Meisels, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of Early Childhood Intervention (pp. 651–82). New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (2001). Child psychiatry in the era following sequencing the genome. In Levy, F. & Hay, D. (Eds), Attention, Genes, and ADHD (pp. 225–48). Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2002). Nature, nurture, and development: From evangelism through science towards policy and practice. Child Development, 73, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (2006a). Genes and Behavior: Nature-Nurture Interplay Explained. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2006b). The psychological effects of early institutional rearing. In Marshall, P. J. & Fox, N. A. (Eds.), The Development of Social Engagement: Neurobiological Perspectives (pp. 355–91). Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M. (2006c). Implications of resilience concepts for scientific understanding. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1094, 1–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2007a). Gene-environment interdependence. Developmental Science, 10, 12–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2007b). Proceeding from observed correlation to causal inference: The use of natural experiments. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2(4), 377–95.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. (2009). Developmental perspectives on psychopathology. In Charney, D. J. & Nestler, E. S. (Eds.), The Neurobiology of Mental Illness, 3rd edition (pp. 1239–50) New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rutter, M. (2010). Commentary: Gene-environment interplay: Depression and anxiety. Cutting Edge Series.
Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Kreppner, J., Stevens, S., Sonuga-Barke, E., O'Connor, T. G., Groothues, C., & Castle, J. (2007a). Effects of profound early institutional deprivation: An overview of findings from a UK longitudinal study of Romanian adoptees. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 332–50.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., Champion, L., Quinton, D., Maughan, B., & Pickles, A. (1995). Understanding individual differences in environmental risk exposure. In Moen, P., Elder, G. H.., & Lüscher, K. (Eds.), Examining Lives in Context: Perspectives on the Ecology of Human Development (pp. 61–93). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Groothues, C., et al. (2007b). Early adolescent outcomes for institutionally deprived and nondeprived adoptees. I. Disinhibited attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48, 17–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., Dunn, J., Plomin, R., Simonoff, E., Pickles, A., Maughan, B., Ormel, J., Meyer, J., & Eaves, L. (1997). Integrating nature and nurture: implications of person-environment correlations and interactions for developmental psychopathology. Development & Psychopathology (Special Issue), 9, 335–66.Google ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Giller, H., & Hagell, A. (1998). Antisocial Behavior by Young People. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Kreppner, J., Croft, C., Colvert, E., Castle, J., Sonuga-Barke, E., Beckett, C., & Murin, M. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes of institutionally deprived and nondeprived adoptees. III. Quasi-autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(12), 1200–07.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutter, M., Kreppner, J., O'Connor, T., & the English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team (2001). Specificity and heterogeneity in children's responses to profound institutional privation. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 97–103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Maughan, B., Pickles, A., & Simonoff, E. (1998). Retrospective recall recalled. In Cairns, R. B., Bergman, L. R., & Kagan, J. (Eds.), Methods and Models for Studying the Individual. Essays in Honor of Marian Radke-Yarrow (pp. 219–42). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2006). Gene-environment interplay and psychopathology: Multiple varieties but real effects. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 226–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M. O'Connor, T., & the English and Romanian Adoptees Research Team. (2004). Are there biological programming effects for psychological development? Findings from a study of Romanian adoptees. Developmental Psychology, 40, 81–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Pickles, A., Murray, R., & Eaves, L. (2001). Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects on behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 291–324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., & Plomin, R. (1997). Opportunities for psychiatry from genetic findings. British Journal of Psychiatry, 171, 209–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Quinton, D., & Hill, J. (1990) Adult outcome of institution-reared children: Males and females compared. In Robins, L. & Rutter, M. (Eds.), Straight & Devious Pathways from Childhood to Adulthood (pp. 135–57). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., & Russell Jones, R. (Eds). (1983). Lead versus Health: Sources and Effects of Low-Level Lead Exposure. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., & Rutter, M. (1993). Developing Minds: Challenge and Continuity across the Lifespan. Harmondsworth, Middx. & New York: Penguin & Basic Books.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., & Silberg, J. (2002). Gene-environment interplay in relation to emotional and behavioral disturbance. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 463–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Silberg, J., O'Connor, T., & Simonoff, E. (1999a) Genetics and child psychiatry: I. Advances in quantitative and molecular genetics. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 3–18.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Silberg, J., O'Connor, T., & Simonoff, E. (1999b) Genetics and child psychiatry: II. Empirical research findings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 19–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., & Smith, D. (1995). Psychosocial Disorders in Young People: Time Trends and Their Causes. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Rutter, M., & Sroufe, L. A. (2000). Developmental psychopathology: Concepts and challenges. Development and Psychopathology, 12, 265–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Thapar, A., & Pickles, A. (2009). Gene-environment interactions: Biologically valid pathway or artifact? Archives of General Psychiatry, 66(12), 1287–9.
Salk, L., Lipsitt, L. P., Sturner, W. Q., Reilly, B. M., & Levat, R. H. (1985). Relationship of maternal and perinatal conditions to eventual adolescent suicide. Lancet, 1, 624–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandberg, S., & Rutter, M. (2008) The role of acute life stresses. In Rutter, M., Taylor, E., Scott, S., Bishop, D., Thapar, A., Stevenson, J., & Pine, D. (Eds.), Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 5th Edition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Scarr, S. (1992). Developmental theories for the 1990s: Development and individual differences. Child Development, 63, 1–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schneider, M. L., & Moore, C. F. (2000). Effect of prenatal stress on development: A nonhuman primate model. In Nelson, C. (Ed.), Minnesota Symposium on Child Psychology (pp. 201–43). New Jersey: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Schuetze, P., Eiden, R., & Edwards, E. (2009). A longitudinal examination of physiological regulation in cocaine-exposed infants across the first 7 months of life. Infancy, 14(1), 19–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Silberg, J., Rutter, M., Neale, M., & Eaves, L. (2001). Genetic moderation of environmental risk for depression and anxiety in girls. British Journal of Psychiatry, 179, 116–21.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sonuga-Barke, E., Beckett, C., Kreppner, J., Colvert, E., Hawkins, A., Rutter, M., Stevens, S., & Castle, J. (2008). Is subnutrition necessary for a poor outcome following early institutional deprivation? Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 50(9), 664–71.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, A. L., Rifkin, L., Amess, P. N., Kirkbride, V., Townsend, J. P., Miller, D. H., Lewis, S. W., Kingsley, D. P. E., Moseley, I. F., Foster, O., & Murray, R. M. (1999). Brain structure and neurocognitive and behavioural function in adolescents who were born very preterm. Lancet, 353, 1653–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stiles, J. (2000). Neural plasticity and cognitive development. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 237–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stratton, K., Howe, C., & Battaglia, F. (1996). Fetal Alcohol Syndrome: Diagnosis, Epidemiology, Prevention, and Treatment. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Streissguth, A. P., & Kanter, J. (1997). The Challenge of Fetal Alcohol Syndrome: Overcoming Secondary Disabilities. Seattle: University of Washington Press.Google Scholar
Tanner, J. M. (1962). Growth at Adolescence. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific.Google Scholar
Thapar, A., & Rutter, M. (2008). Genetics. In Rutter, M., Taylor, E., Scott, S., Bishop, D., Thapar, A., Stevenson, J., & Pine, D. (Eds.), Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 5th Edition. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Tizard, J. (1975). Race and IQ: The limits of probability. New Behaviour, 1, 6–9.Google Scholar
Toga, A. W., Thompson, P. M., & Sowell, E. R. (2006). Mapping brain maturation. Trends in Neuroscience, 29, 148–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tulving, E. (1983). Elements of Episodic Memory. London and New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Vargha-Khadem, F., Isaacs, E., Werf, S., Robb, S., & Wilson, J. (1992). Development of intelligence and memory in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: The deleterious consequences of early seizures. Brain, 115, 315–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vargha-Khadem, F., & Mishkin, M. (1997). Speech and language outcome after hemispherectomy in childhood. In Tuxhorn, I., Holthausen, H., & Boenigk, H. E. (Eds.), Paediatric Epilepsy Syndromes and Their Surgical Treatment (pp. 774–84). Sydney, Australia: Libbey.Google Scholar
Vogel, F., & Motulsky, A. G. (1997). Human Genetics: Problems and Approaches, Third Edition. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weatherall, D. J., & Clegg, J. B. (2001). The Thalassemia Syndromes, Fourth Edition. Oxford: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weaver, I. C., Diorio, J., Seckl, J. R., Szyf, M., & Meaney, M. J. (2004). Early environmental regulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene expression: Characterization of intracellular mediators and potential genomic target. Annals of the New York Academy of Science, 1024, 182–212.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Werker, J. F. (2003). Baby steps to learning language. Journal of Pediatrics, 143, S62–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×