Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T10:47:07.177Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - The Story of Risk

How Narratives Shape Risk Communication, Perception, and Policy

from Part I - The Conceptual Terrain of Crises and Risk Perceptions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 October 2017

Edward J. Balleisen
Affiliation:
Duke University, North Carolina
Lori S. Bennear
Affiliation:
Duke University, North Carolina
Kimberly D. Krawiec
Affiliation:
Duke University, North Carolina
Jonathan B. Wiener
Affiliation:
Duke University, North Carolina
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Policy Shock
Recalibrating Risk and Regulation after Oil Spills, Nuclear Accidents and Financial Crises
, pp. 107 - 128
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bartlett, F. C. and Burt, C.. 1933. Remembering: A Study in Experimental and Social Psychology. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 3(2): 187–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumgartner, F. R., and Jones, B. D.. 2010. Agendas and instability in American politics. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Benford, R. D., and Snow, D. A.. 2000. Framing Processes and Social Movements: An Overview and Assessment. Annual review of sociology: 611–39.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bennet, L. 1983. News, The Politics of Illusion. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
Best, J. and Horiuchi, G. T.. 1985. The Razor Blade in the Apple: The Social Construction of Urban Legends. Social Problems, 32(5): 488–49.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birkland, T. A., 1998. Focusing Events, Mobilization, and Agenda Setting. Journal of Public Policy 18(01): 5374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bruner, J. 1990. Acts of Meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Downs, A., 1972. Up and Down with Ecology: The Issue-Attention Cycle. The Public Interest (28): 3850.Google Scholar
Downs, A. 1957. An Economic Theory of Democracy. New York: Harper.Google Scholar
Edelman, M.J., 1964. The Symbolic Uses of Politics. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Entman, R. M., 1993. Framing: Toward Clarification of a Fractured Paradigm. Journal of Communication, 43(4): 5158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Entman, R. M., 2004. Projections of Power: Framing News, Public Opinion, and US Foreign Policy. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P., 2001. Feeling and Thinking: The Role of Affect in Social Cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gamson, W. A., 1992. Talking Politics. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge university press.Google Scholar
Goffman, E., 1974. Frame Analysis: An Essay on the Organization of Experience. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hamilton, J., 2004. All the News That’s Fit to Sell: How the Market Transforms Information into News. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hardy, B., 1977. Narrative as a Primary Act of Mind. In Meek, M., Warlow, A., and Barton, G. (eds.), The Cool Web: The Pattern of Children’s Reading. London: Bodley Head: 1223.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., 2011. Thinking, Fast and Slow. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kennedy, D. and Overholser, G.. 2010. Science and the Media. Washington, DC: American Academy of Arts and Sciences. www.amacad.org/pdfs/scienceMedia.pdf.Google Scholar
Kingdon, J. W., 1984. Agendas, Alternatives, and Public Policies. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Kotre, J., 1996. White Gloves: How We Create Ourselves Through Memory. New York: W.W. Norton and Company.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., and Welch, N.. 2001. Risk as Feelings. Psychological bulletin, 127(2): 267–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacIntyre, A. C. 1984. After Virtue: A Study in Moral Theory. Notre Dame, IN : University of Notre Dame Press.Google Scholar
Mayer, F. W. 2014. Narrative Politics: Stories and Collective Action. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAdams, D. P., 2001. The Psychology of Life Stories. Review of General Psychology, 5(2): 100–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, P. J., Potts, R., Fung, H., Hoogstra, L., and Mintz, J.. 1990. Narrative Practices and the Social Construction of Self in Childhood. American Ethnologist, 17(2): 292311.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, B. J., 1986. Making an Issue of Child Abuse: Political Agenda Setting for Social Problems. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, T. E., 2011. Out of Order: An Incisive and Boldly Original Critique of the News Media’s Domination of America’s Poliical Process. New York: Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. C., 1987. “The Mind as a Consuming Organ. In Elster, Jon, (ed.) The Multiple Self, Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press: 177–98.Google Scholar
Shiffman, D., 2013. Swimming with Sharks: Was Diana Nyad Really at Risk of Being Bitten During Her Historic Swim. Slate. September 4. www.slate.com/articles/health_and_science/science/2013/09/diana_nyad_shark_swim_how_dangerous_are_the_sharks_between_cuba_and_florida.html.Google Scholar
Slovic, P. E. 1987. Perception of Risk. Science 236: 280–85.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slovic, P. E., 2000. The Perception of Risk. New York: Earthscan publications.Google Scholar
Stone, D. A., 1988. Policy Paradox and Political Reason. New York: Addison-Wesley Longman.Google Scholar
Tilly, C., 2002. Stories, Identities, and Political Change. Oxford, UK: Rowman and Littlefield Publishers.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D., 1974. Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases. Science, 185(4157): 1124–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, A. and Kahneman, D., 1981. The Framing of Decisions and the Psychology of Choice. Science 211(4481): 453–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×