Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-lj6df Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-18T10:24:19.705Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Young Children's Knowledge about Overt and Covert Private Speech

from Part II - Language, Communication, Social Cognition, and Awareness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2009

Adam Winsler
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Charles Fernyhough
Affiliation:
University of Durham
Ignacio Montero
Affiliation:
Universidad Autónoma de Madrid
Get access

Summary

Private speech, known colloquially as “talking to oneself,” is used to describe any type of speech not obviously directed toward another person. Broadly classified, human speech falls into one of two categories: social speech, serving the function of social communication; and private speech, frequently serving the function of self-regulation. Research on private speech principally began with the work of Lev Vygotsky (1934/1986; see also Piaget, 1926). According to Vygotsky, the frequency of children's overt private speech rises during early childhood, then declines and virtually disappears. He suggested that its disappearance is the result of the child's progressive differentiation of social and private speech. As the child comes to utilize private speech's self-regulatory function, overt private speech is replaced by internal verbal thought. This internalization of private speech enables the child to participate in more sophisticated, adult forms of cognition.

Current research largely supports the Vygotskian view. Researchers have documented the rise in frequency of overt private speech during the preschool years and its gradual decline during the early elementary school years (Bivens & Berk, 1990; Díaz & Berk, 1992; Winsler, de León, Wallace, Carlton, & Willson-Quayle, 2003). Qualitative changes in private speech production have also been documented. At around age 3, private speech emerges in overt forms; at around age 5, the volume of utterances declines and partially covert forms predominate; eventually, even whispers and mouth movements tend to disappear as private speech becomes fully covert (Díaz & Berk, 1992; Winsler, Carlton, & Barry, 2000).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2009

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bivens, J. A., & Berk, L. E. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of school children's private speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 36, 443–463.Google Scholar
Díaz, R. M., & Berk, L. E. (1992). Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., & Green, F. L. (1995). Young children's knowledge about thinking. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 60(1, Serial No. 243).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (1993). Children's understanding of the stream of consciousness. Child Development, 64, 387–398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., & Flavell, E. R. (2000). Development of children's awareness of their own thoughts. Journal of Cognition and Development, 1, 97–112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., Flavell, E. R., & Grossman, J. B. (1997). The development of children's knowledge about inner speech. Child Development, 68, 39–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Henry Holt.Google Scholar
Johnston, R. S., & Conning, A. (1990). The effects of overt and covert rehearsal on the emergence of the phonological similarity effect in 5-year-old children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 8, 411–418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnston, R. S., Johnson, C., & Gray, C. (1987). The emergence of the word length effect in young children: The effects of overt and covert rehearsal. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 243–248.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manfra, L. (2003, April). Awareness of the use of private speech in preschool children. In Winsler, A. (Chair), Awareness, attitudes, and beliefs concerning children's private speech: Perspectives from children, parents, and teachers. Symposium conducted at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
Mayer, M. (1976). Hiccup. New York: Dial Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1926). Language and thought of the child. New York: Harcourt, Brace and World.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (Kozulin, A., Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. (Original work published 1934)Google Scholar
Winsler, A., Carlton, M. P., & Barry, M. J. (2000). Age-related changes in preschool children's systematic use of private speech in a natural setting. Journal of Child Language, 27, 665–687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., León, J. R., Wallace, B. A., Carlton, M. P., & Willson-Quayle, A. (2003). Private speech in children: Developmental stability and change, across-task consistency, and relations with classroom behavior. Journal of Child Language, 30, 583–608.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., Díaz, R. M., & Montero, I. (1997). The role of private speech in the transition from collaborative to independent task performance in young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 12, 55–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winsler, A., & Naglieri, J. A. (2003). Overt and covert verbal problem-solving strategies: Developmental trends in use, awareness, and relations with task performance in children age 5 to 17. Child Development, 74, 659–678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×