Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T07:19:16.809Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Perceptual uncertainty is a property of the cognitive system

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 August 2012

Manuel Perea
Affiliation:
Departamento de Metodología and ERI-Lectura, Universitat de València, 46010 Valencia, Spain. mperea@uv.eshttp://www.uv.es/mperea
Manuel Carreiras
Affiliation:
Basque Center on Cognition, Brain, and Language, 20009 San Sebastián-Donostia, Spain. m.carreiras@bcbl.euhttp://www.bcbl.eu/people/staff/manuel_carreiras

Abstract

We qualify Frost's proposals regarding letter-position coding in visual word recognition and the universal model of reading. First, we show that perceptual uncertainty regarding letter position is not tied to European languages – instead it is a general property of the cognitive system. Second, we argue that a universal model of reading should incorporate a developmental view of the reading process.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2012 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acha, J. & Perea, M. (2008) The effects of length and transposed-letter similarity in lexical decision: Evidence with beginning, intermediate, and adult readers. British Journal of Psychology 99:245–64.Google Scholar
Andrews, S. & Lo, S. (2012) Not all skilled readers have cracked the code: Individual differences in masked form priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 38:152–63.Google Scholar
Barlow, H. B. (1956) Retinal noise and absolute threshold. Journal of the Optical Society of America 46:634–39.Google Scholar
Carreiras, M., Duñabeitia, J. A. & Molinaro, N. (2009a) Consonants and vowels contribute differently to visual word recognition: ERPs of relative position priming. Cerebral Cortex 19(11):2659–70.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, M., Seghier, M., Baquero, S., Estévez, A., Lozano, A., Devlin, J. T. & Price, C. J. (2009b) An anatomical signature for literacy. Nature 461:983–86.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Castles, A., Davis, C., Cavalot, P. & Forster, K. I. (2007) Tracking the acquisition of orthographic skills in developing readers: Masked form priming and transposed-letter effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 97:165–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dehaene, S. & Cohen, L. (2011) The unique role of the visual word form area in reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 15(6):254–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duñabeitia, J. A., Perea, M. & Carreiras, M. (2009) There is no clam with coats in the calm coast: Delimiting the transposed-letter priming effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62:1930–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friedmann, N. & Rahamim, E. (2007) Developmental letter position dyslexia. Journal of Neuropsychology 1:201–36.Google Scholar
Lee, C. H. & Taft, M. (2009) Are onsets and codas important in processing letter position? A comparison of TL effects in English and Korean. Journal of Memory and Language 60(4):530–42. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2009.01.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, R. W., Klein, S. A. & Levi, D. M. (2006) The receptive field and internal noise for position acuity change with feature separation. Journal of Vision 6:311–21.Google Scholar
Logan, G. D. (1996) The CODE theory of visual attention: An integration of space-based and object-based attention. Psychological Review 103:603–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perea, M., Abu Mallouh, R. & Carreiras, M. (2010) The search of an input coding scheme: Transposed-letter priming in Arabic. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 17(3):375–80.Google Scholar
Perea, M., Abu Mallouh, R., García-Orza, J. & Carreiras, M. (2011a) Masked priming effects are modulated by expertise in the script. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 64:902–19.Google Scholar
Perea, M. & Estévez, A. (2008) Transposed-letter similarity effects in naming pseudowords: Evidence from children and adults. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 20:3346.Google Scholar
Perea, M., Nakatani, C. & van Leeuwen, C. (2011b) Transposition effects in reading Japanese Kana: Are they orthographic in nature? Memory and Cognition 39:700707.Google Scholar
Perea, M., Winskel, H. & Ratitamkul, T. (2012) On the flexibility of letter position coding during lexical processing: The case of Thai. Experimental Psychology 59: 6873.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rumelhart, D. E. & McClelland, J. L. (1982) An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review 89:6094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Velan, H. & Frost, R. (2011) Words with and without internal structure: What determines the nature of orthographic and morphological processing? Cognition 118:141–56.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed