Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7479d7b7d-qs9v7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-07-13T00:27:48.125Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Explaining Word-Final Stress Lapse

from Part I - Phonetic Correlates and Prominence Distinctions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 December 2018

Rob Goedemans
Affiliation:
Universiteit Leiden
Jeffrey Heinz
Affiliation:
Stony Brook University, State University of New York
Harry van der Hulst
Affiliation:
University of Connecticut
Get access

Summary

Focusing on rhythmic stress, this chapter examines evidence for a motivation behind the well-known asymmetrical tolerance for a stress lapse word-initially versus word-finally across languages. Evidence from two different sources is shown to support the connection between languages' tolerance of final stress lapse and their use of duration in stress. Drawing on a database of stress correlates that the author made for this research project, it is shown that languages which tolerate a final stress lapse are indeed extremely likely to have duration as a stress correlate, whereas no such correlation exists for final lapse and the stress correlates of pitch or intensity. Several perception experiments also support this connection: final lengthening was found to be confusable with stress, only if stressed syllables included increased duration. Finally, an account is sketched of how this could be captured in an Optimality Theoretic analysis.
Type
Chapter
Information
The Study of Word Stress and Accent
Theories, Methods and Data
, pp. 76 - 101
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alber, B. (2005). Clash, lapse and directionality. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 23: 485542.Google Scholar
Barnes, J. (2002). Positional neutralization: A phonologization approach to typographical patterns. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.Google Scholar
Besnier, N. (2000). Tuvaluan Descriptive Grammar. London and New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
Blevins, J. (2001). Nhanda: An Aboriginal Language of Western Australia. Honolulu: University of Hawai’i Press.Google Scholar
Boersma, P. and Weenink., D. (2014). Praat: A system for doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 5.3.78. www.praat.org.Google Scholar
Crowhurst, M. and Hewitt., M. (1995). Directional footing, degeneracy and alignment. Proceedings of NELS 25. Amherst: GLSA, 4761. http://roa.rutgers.edu.Google Scholar
Crystal, T. and House., A. (1990). Articulation rate and duration of syllables and stress groups in connected speech. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 88: 101112.Google Scholar
Derbyshire, D. (1985). Hixkaryana and Linguistic Typology. Dallas: Summer Institute of Linguistics.Google Scholar
Deschenes, N., Kalivoda, N., and Lunden, A.. (2012). Evidence for final lengthening as prominence. Poster given at WCCFL 30, UC Santa Cruz, April 15.Google Scholar
Drummond, A. (2014). Ibex: Internet based experiments. http://spellout.net/ibexfarm/.Google Scholar
Dubĕda, T. and Votrubec, J.. (2005). Acoustic analysis of Czech stress: Intonation, duration and intensity revisited. Proceedings of the 9th European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology, September 4–8, Lisbon, 14291432.Google Scholar
Dutoit, T., Pagel, V., Pierret, N., Bataille, F., and van der Vreken, O.. (1996). The MBROLA project: Towards a set of high-quality speech synthesizers free of use for noncommercial purposes. Proceedings International Congress of Spoken Language Processing, Volume 3. Philadelphia, 13931396.Google Scholar
Eades, D. (2005). A Grammar of Gayo: A Language of Aceh, Sumatra. Canberra: Pacific Linguistics.Google Scholar
Fleck, D. W. (2003). A grammar of Matses. Doctoral dissertation, Rice University.Google Scholar
Flemming, E. (2005). A phonetically-based model of phonological vowel reduction. Unpublished ms., MIT.Google Scholar
Goedemans, R. and van der Hulst., H. (2014). Stresstyp database. [Database accessed in 2014 before expansion to the current StressTyp2.] http://st2.ullet.net.Google Scholar
Gordon, M. (2002). A factorial typology of quantity-insensitive stress. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 20: 491552.Google Scholar
Gordon, M(2014). Disentangling stress and pitch-accent: A typology of prominence at different prosodic levels. In van den Hulst, Harry (ed.) Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Cambridge University Press, 83118.Google Scholar
Gordon, M., Jany, C., Nash, C., and Takara, N.. (2010). Syllable structure and extrametricality: A typological and phonetic study. Studies in Language 34: 131166.Google Scholar
Hale, K. and White Eagle, J.. (1980). A preliminary metrical account of Winnebago accent. International Journal of American Linguistics 46: 117132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, K. C. and Hansen, L. E.. (1969). Pintupi phonology. Oceanic Linguistics 8: 153170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayes, B. (1982). Extrametricality and English stress. Linguistic Inquiry 13: 227276.Google Scholar
Hayes, B.(1995). Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Hulst, H. van der. (2010). Word accent: Terms, typologies, and theories. In Goedemans, Rob, van der Hulst, Harry, and van Zanten, Ellen (eds.) A Survey of World Accentual Patterns in the Languages of the World. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter, 354.Google Scholar
Hulst, H. van der(2014). Representing rhythm. In van der Hulst, Harry (ed.) Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Cambridge University Press, 325365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kager, R. (1999). Optimality Theory. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kager, R(2001). Rhythmic directionality by positional licensing. Paper presented at HILP-5, University of Potsdam, 11 January, ROA-514. http://roa.rutgers.edu.Google Scholar
Kager, R(2005). Rhythmic licensing: An extended typology. Proceedings of the 3rd International Conference on Phonology. Seoul: The Phonology-Morphology Circle of Korea, 531.Google Scholar
Keating, P., Cho, T., Fourgerson, C., and Hus, C.. (2003). Domain-initial strengthening in four languages. Laboratory Phonology 6: 145163.Google Scholar
Künnap, A. (1999). Enets. Munich: Lincom Europa.Google Scholar
Lacy, P.de. (2014). Evaluating evidence for stress systems. In van der Hulst, Harry (ed.) Word Stress: Theoretical and Typological Issues. Cambridge University Press, 149193.Google Scholar
Liberman, M. and Prince, A.. (1977). On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguistic Inquiry 8: 249336.Google Scholar
Lindblom, B. (1968). Temporal organization of syllable production. Speech Transmission Laboratory Quarterly Progress, Volume 2–3. Stockholm: Royal Institute of Technology, 16.Google Scholar
Lunden, A. (2016). Effects of position and stress on English and Norwegian vowel quality. Unpublished ms., College of William and Mary.Google Scholar
Lunden, A. and Kalivoda, N.. (2016). Stress correlate database. Online database of stress correlates: http://wmpeople.wm.edu/sllund/stresscorrelatedatabase.Google Scholar
Lynch, J. (1978). A Grammar of Lenakel. Pacific Linguistics B55. Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Lynch, J. (2000). A Grammar of Anejom˜. Pacific Linguistics 507. Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Mihas, E. (2010). Essentials of Ashéninka Perené grammar. Doctoral dissertation, The University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee.Google Scholar
Miner, K. (1979). Dorsey’s law in Winnebago-Chiwere and Winnebago accent. International Journal of American Linguistics 45: 2533.Google Scholar
Oller, D. K. (1973). The effect of position in utterance on speech segment duration in English. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 54: 12351247.Google Scholar
Pickering, WN. (1971). Apurinã grammar. Anápolis, Brazil: SIL.Google Scholar
Prince, A and Smolensky, P. (1993). Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Unpublished ms., Rutgers University, New Brunswick and University of Colorado.Google Scholar
Quigley, E C. (2003). Awara phonology. Doctoral dissertation, University of North Dakota.Google Scholar
Sluijter, A and van Heuven., V. (1996). Spectral balance as an acoustic correlate of linguistic stress. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 100: 24712485.Google Scholar
Tegey, H. and Robson, B.. (1996). A Reference Grammar of Pashto. Washington DC: Center for Applied Linguistics.Google Scholar
Tryon, D. (1970). An Introduction to Maranungku. Canberra: Australian National University.Google Scholar
Walker, R. (1996). Prominence-driven stress. Unpublished ms., University of California, Santa Cruz. http://roa.rutgers.edu.Google Scholar
Wightman, C., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., Ostendorf, M., and Price, P.. (1992). Segmental durations in the vicinity of prosodic phrase boundaries. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 91: 17071717.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×